Context The World Health Organization and the United Nations Children's Fund
strongly discourage use of pacifiers because of their perceived interference
with breastfeeding. Observational studies have reported a strong association
between pacifier use and early weaning, but such studies are unable to determine
whether the association is causal.
Objectives To test whether regular pacifier use is causally related to weaning
by 3 months postpartum and to examine differences in results according to
randomized intervention allocation vs observational use or nonuse of pacifiers.
Design Double-blind, randomized controlled trial conducted from January 1998
to August 1999.
Setting Postpartum unit of a university teaching hospital in Montreal, Quebec.
Participants A total of 281 healthy, breastfeeding women and their healthy, term
singleton infants.
Interventions Participants were randomly allocated to 1 of 2 counseling interventions
provided by a research nurse trained in location counseling. The experimental
intervention (n = 140) differed from the control (n = 141) by recommending
avoidance of pacifier use and suggesting alternative ways to comfort a crying
or fussing infant.
Main Outcome Measures Early weaning, defined as weaning within the first 3 months, compared
between groups; 24-hour infant behavior logs detailing frequency and duration
of crying, fussing, and pacifier use at 4, 6, and 9 weeks.
Results A total of 258 mother-infant pairs (91.8%) completed follow-up. The
experimental intervention increased total avoidance of pacifier use (38.6%
vs 16.0% in the control group), reduced daily use (40.8% vs 55.7%), and decreased
the mean number of pacifier insertions per day (0.8 vs 2.4 at 4 weeks [P<.001]; 0.8 vs 3.0 at 6 weeks [P<.001]; and 1.3 vs 3.0 at 9 weeks [P =
.004]). In the analysis based on randomized intervention allocation, the experimental
intervention had no discernible effect on weaning at 3 months (18.9% vs 18.3%
in the experimental vs control group; relative risk [RR], 1.0; 95% confidence
interval [CI], 0.6-1.7), and no effect was observed on cry/fuss behavior (in
the experimental vs control groups, respectively, total daily duration, 143
vs 151 minutes at 4 weeks [P = .49]; 128 vs 131 minutes
at 6 weeks [P = .81]; and 110 vs 104 minutes at 9
weeks [P = .58]). When randomized allocation was
ignored, however, we observed a strong observational association between exposure
to daily pacifier use and weaning by 3 months (25.0% vs 12.9% of the exposed
vs unexposed groups; RR, 1.9; 95% CI, 1.1-3.3).
Conclusions We found a strong observational association between pacifier use and
early weaning. No such association was observed, however, when our data were
analyzed by randomized allocation, strongly suggesting that pacifier use is
a marker of breastfeeding difficulties or reduced motivation to breastfeed,
rather than a true cause of early weaning.
Pacifiers have been around for a very long time. Small clay pacifiers
have been found in Cypriot graves dating back to about 1000 BC, and breast-shaped
pottery nipples have been recovered from Roman graves dating from around AD
100.1,2 In the early 1900s, however,
pacifiers began to be condemned by the infant welfare movement. Various reformers
referred to the pacifier as a product of "perverted American ingenuity,"3 an "instrument of torture,"4
and a "curse of babyhood."5 More recently,
clinicians and public health practitioners have raised concerns that the pacifier
causes "nipple confusion" and thereby leads to early weaning.6
In fact, avoidance of pacifiers constitutes step 9 of the World Health Organization/United
Nations Children's Fund Baby-Friendly Hospital Initiative.7
What is the evidence of actual benefit or harm associated with pacifier
use? Several observational studies published since the Baby-Friendly Hospital
Initiative was developed, including studies from Brazil,8-10
Sweden,11,12 England,13,14 New Zealand,15
and the United States,16 have reported a significant
association between pacifier use and early weaning. The question is whether
such an association is causal, or whether pacifier use is a marker of breastfeeding
difficulties or a mother's reduced motivation to continue breastfeeding. No
physiological evidence has validated the concept of nipple confusion; an infant
can apparently distinguish nutritive from nonnutritive sucking.17-20
It is clear that pacifiers reduce crying in the short term21-25
but no studies have assessed whether the regular use of pacifiers reduces
the overall duration or frequency of crying and fussing. Such studies are
important, because proscription of pacifier use could conceivably increase
infant distress and thereby impair infant-parent relationships.
The major objectives of our study were to assess whether advice to avoid
pacifier use and to use other modes of calming a crying or fussing infant
reduces the risk of early weaning (before age 3 months) and increases the
frequency or duration of crying and fussing. We also wished to assess the
bias that occurs in using an observational vs an experimental design to study
the effect of pacifier use on breastfeeding duration.
We carried out a randomized controlled trial from January 1998 to August
1999 of women giving birth at the Royal Victoria Hospital, a McGill University–affiliated
maternity hospital in Montreal, Quebec. The Royal Victoria Hospital Research
Ethics Board approved the study. Women who intended to breastfeed for at least
3 months and who were delivered of (vaginally or by cesarean) healthy singleton
newborns of at least 37 completed weeks' gestational age and 2500 g birth
weight were eligible for inclusion. They were recruited during their postpartum
stay, with enrollment of at most 1 mother per room to avoid the treatment
contamination that would likely occur if mothers randomized to different interventions
occupied the same hospital room. Women were stratified by parity and, if multiparous,
according to whether they had breastfed previously. Randomization within each
stratum was accomplished using computer-generated random numbers in blocks
of 4. Women consented to randomization to 1 of 2 different breastfeeding promotion
"packages" (see below); the assigned allocation was contained in an opaque
envelope opened by a research nurse after consent was obtained. Based on the
literature available8,9,11,15
at the time we planned this trial, we estimated that a reduction in daily
pacifier use from 60% to 40% would reduce the risk of weaning before age 3
months from 40% to 35%. With an α level of .05 and a β of .10,
approximately 140 infants were required per group.
The basic breastfeeding promotion package included in both interventions
consisted of a 45-minute interview promoting breastfeeding plus an information
sheet, both provided by a nurse with specialized training in lactation counseling.
This interview and information focused on positioning, the importance of frequent
feeding and feeding on demand, the avoidance of formula and other liquids,
the management of sore nipples and breast engorgement, and provided the telephone
numbers of persons and agencies whom the mother could call for answers to
questions, help with difficulties, and general support. In addition, for the
experimental intervention, the mother was asked to avoid pacifiers when the
infant cried or fussed and to first offer the breast instead, and failing
that, to try carrying and rocking the infant. In the control intervention,
all options were discussed for calming the infant, including breastfeeding,
carrying, rocking, and using a pacifier. The experimental vs control intervention
was reinforced by the research nurse by telephone calls at 10 days and 3 weeks
postpartum.
Ascertainment of Outcomes
We asked mothers to complete a validated behavior diary26,27
on 3 consecutive days, including 2 weekdays and 1 weekend day, when their
infants were 4, 6, and 9 weeks of age. This diary provides exhaustive and
mutually exclusive indicators of infant behaviors, including the frequency
and duration of all crying and fussing episodes. Periods of unsoothable crying
are recorded separately. An indicator for each pacifier insertion was added
to a previous version of the instrument.28
Study mothers were interviewed at 3 months by a research assistant who was
blinded to the intervention status of the mother. The assistant asked whether
the mother was still breastfeeding. If so, the research assistant also asked
about the frequency of breastfeeding and whether the infant was receiving
other foods in addition to breast milk. If the infant was no longer breastfeeding,
the research assistant asked the age at which the infant was weaned (ie, when
breastfeeding was permanently discontinued) and the reasons for weaning. In
addition, she asked about the average frequency of pacifier use over the infant's
first 3 months of life.
The primary outcome, early weaning (weaning within the first 3 months),
was compared between the 2 randomized intervention groups using the relative
risk (RR) and the 95% confidence interval (CI). We also carried out a multiple
logistic regression analysis to ensure that the crude RR was not confounded
by observed differences in baseline characteristics. Secondary outcomes included
the frequency (number of episodes per day) and total duration (minutes per
day) of crying or fussing and the duration of unsoothable crying (minutes
per day); these outcomes were compared using 2-tailed t tests, with P<.05 indicating statistical
significance. All analyses were based on randomized intervention allocation
in the mothers who completed the study, because no outcomes could be ascertained
in those who were lost to follow-up.
To contrast the results obtained for the primary outcome based on randomized
intervention allocation with those obtained if randomized allocation was ignored,
we also analyzed the data as if we had done an observational study. This analysis
compared the RR and 95% CI in groups who had ever (vs never) been exposed
to a pacifier and in groups who were (vs were not) exposed on a daily basis.
All statistical analyses were carried out using SAS version 6.12 (SAS
Institute, Cary, NC).
Of the 281 mothers randomized, 258 (91.8%; 127 experimental and 131
control) completed the study (Figure 1). Table 1 compares the baseline characteristics
of these 258 mothers. Maternal age and education, infant birth weight, English-language
interview, maternal employment outside the home, parity, and previous breastfeeding
experience were very similar in the 2 groups, although a slightly lower proportion
of experimental mothers were married and a slightly higher proportion smoked
during pregnancy. The only differences in baseline characteristics observed
among the 23 mothers originally randomized who did not complete the study
were a lower proportion who were married (69.6% vs 81.4%), a higher proportion
who smoked (26.1% vs 13.0%), and, paradoxically, a lower proportion who worked
outside the home (43.5% vs 76.0%).
As shown in Table 2, the
intervention succeeded in substantially changing pacifier use; 38.6% of mothers
in the experimental group totally avoided pacifier use, compared with 16.0%
in the control group, for a statistically significant RR of 2.4. Daily use
of pacifiers was substantially reduced in the experimental group (40.8%) vs
the control group (55.7%), for a statistically significant 30% relative reduction.
The diary data indicated a large and highly significant reduction in the mean
number of pacifier insertions per day in the experimental group at 4, 6, and
9 weeks of age.
We first analyzed the results for early weaning based on randomized
allocation. Among the 258 infants whose mothers completed the study, 18.9%
of those in the experimental group were weaned prior to age 3 months vs 18.3%
of those in the control group (RR, 1.0; 95% CI, 0.6-1.7). The results for
exclusive breastfeeding were similar: 63.8% of infants in the experimental
group had discontinued exclusive breastfeeding by age 3 months vs 66.4% of
those in the control group (RR, 1.0; 95% CI, 0.8-1.1). To ensure that these
negative experimental results were not confounded by small baseline differences
in marital status or smoking, we carried out a logistic regression analysis
to control for these differences. The adjusted odds ratio and 95% CI for weaning
by age 3 months (1.0 [0.5-1.9]) was virtually unchanged from the crude odds
ratio (1.0 [0.6-1.9]).
In contrast, the observational analysis of early weaning yielded very
different results. When exposure was based on daily pacifier use, 25.0% of
exposed infants were weaned prior to age 3 months vs 12.9% of unexposed infants
(RR, 1.9; 95% CI, 1.1-3.3). When exposure was based on whether the infants
had ever used a pacifier, the corresponding proportions weaned before age
3 months were 21.3% vs 11.4% (RR, 1.9; 95% CI, 0.9-3.8). Observational associations
with exclusive breastfeeding were weaker but statistically significant. For
exposure based on daily pacifier use, 72.6% of exposed infants vs 58.3% of
unexposed infants had discontinued exclusive breastfeeding before age 3 months
(RR, 1.2; 95% CI, 1.04-1.5). For exposure based on ever using pacifiers, the
corresponding values were 69.1% vs 54.3% (RR, 1.3; 95% CI, 1.01-1.6).
Only 183 (70.9%), 156 (60.5%), and 148 (57.4%) of the mothers returned
the infant behavior diaries at 4, 6, and 9 weeks, respectively, with no differences
between experimental and control groups. Table 3 shows the results (analyzed by randomized intervention allocation)
for cry/fuss behavior, including daily frequency, ie, the number of episodes
of crying (including unsoothable crying) or fussing per day, total daily duration
of all crying and fussing, and total daily duration of unsoothable crying.
Cry/fuss frequency was slightly lower in the experimental group at 4 and 6
weeks, but almost identical in the 2 groups at 9 weeks. The total duration
of crying and fussing was similar in the 2 groups at all 3 ages, as was the
total duration of unsoothable crying.
Because of the substantial nonresponse rate to the diary completion,
we compared baseline characteristics of those who did not complete diaries
in the experimental and control groups at 4, 6, and 9 weeks postpartum. The
results (Table 4) indicate that
nonresponders at all 3 time periods were younger, less educated, and less
likely to be married. As shown in Table
1, however, these characteristics were similar between the experimental
and control groups (which was also true when stratified by diary response
at all 3 time periods), and thus the differences between responders and nonresponders
are highly unlikely to have biased the effects of intervention on cry/fuss
behavior.
Our experimental intervention succeeded in substantially reducing pacifier
use, yet it had no significant effect on cry/fuss behavior at ages 4, 6, or
9 weeks. Similarly, our intervention had no effect on the risk of weaning
before age 3 months. Despite these negative results, however, pacifier use
was strongly associated with the risk of early weaning in observational analyses,
similar to results reported in previous observational studies.8-16
This combination of findings leads us to conclude that pacifier use is a marker
of breastfeeding difficulties or reduced motivation to breastfeed, rather
than a true cause of early weaning. We reported an identical contrast between
experimental and observational results in an earlier trial of in-hospital
formula supplementation of breastfed infants.29,30
Although we found no evidence that pacifier use is harmful for breastfeeding,
we also detected no beneficial effects on infant crying and fussing. Thus
the nonpacifier soothing methods (breastfeeding, carrying, and rocking) advocated
in our experimental intervention appear adequate. Nonetheless, the absence
of a causal link between pacifier use and early weaning should lead breastfeeding
promotion programs and international agencies to reexamine their staunch opposition
to pacifiers.
Like any study, ours has limitations. A larger sample size would be
required to exclude a small increased risk of early weaning, as shown by the
width of our confidence interval for that outcome. We deliberately chose an
experimental intervention that could be feasibly implemented on a large scale
if it was successful in reducing the risk of early weaning, but our results
cannot be generalized to more potent interventions to avoid pacifier use.
Data on pacifier use were based on maternal self-report, but the recording
of use from the written behavior diaries at 4, 6, and 9 weeks and the telephone
interview at 3 months were consistent in showing large differences between
the 2 intervention groups. Maternal self-report has been the basis of data
on pacifier use in all previous studies; like ours, these studies have resulted
in strong and statistically significant observational associations between
pacifier use and early weaning. Finally, we had an approximately 40% nonresponse
rate to the diary completion, with a potential for selection bias in our analyses
of cry/fuss behavior. The characteristics of responders and nonresponders
were virtually identical in the experimental and control groups, however,
and thus such a bias seems unlikely.
Breastfeeding, pacifier use, and infant cry/fuss behavior are complex
behaviors heavily influenced by cultural, motivational, and psychological
factors that are extremely difficult to measure, and hence to control for,
in an observational study. As we have previously discussed,30
these potent factors are likely to lead to residual confounding and reverse
causality bias in observational studies of the pacifier-weaning association.
Unlike recent results suggesting that observational studies of pharmacological
and surgical treatments can yield valid results,31,32
valid assessment of the effects of behavioral interventions on behavioral
outcomes appears to require the bias reduction provided by randomized trials.
1.Gorelick L. On the use of pacifiers in preventing malocclusion.
N Y State Dent J.1955;21:3-10.Google Scholar 3.Pedley TF. The rubber teat and deformities of the jaws.
Dent Rec.1907;27:176-177.Google Scholar 5.King FT. Feeding and Care of Baby. London, England: McMillan; 1923.
6.Neifert M, Lawrence R, Seacat J. Nipple confusion: toward a formal definition.
J Pediatr.1995;126:S125-S129.Google Scholar 7.WHO/UNICEF. Protecting, Promoting and Supporting Breastfeeding:
The Special Role of Maternity Services. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization; 1989.
8.Victora CG, Tomasi E, Olinto MTA, Barros FC. Use of pacifiers and breastfeeding duration.
Lancet.1993;341:404-406.Google Scholar 9.Barros FC, Victora CG, Semer TC, Filho ST, Tomasi E, Weiderpass E. Use of pacifiers is associated with decreased breastfeeding duration.
Pediatrics.1995;95:497-499.Google Scholar 10.Victora CG, Behague DP, Barros FC, Olinto MTA, Weiderpass E. Pacifier use and short breastfeeding duration: cause, consequence,
or coincidence?
Pediatrics.1997;99:445-453.Google Scholar 11.Richard L, Alade MO. Sucking technique and its effect on success of breastfeeding.
Birth.1992;19:185-189.Google Scholar 12.Aarts C, Hörnell A, Kylberg E, Hofvander Y, Gebre-Medhin M. Breastfeeding patterns in relation to thumb sucking and pacifier use.
Pediatrics.1999;104:1-10.Google Scholar 13.Gale CR, Martyn CN. Dummies and the health of Hertfordshire infants, 1911-1930.
Soc Hist Med.1995;8:231-255.Google Scholar 14.Clements MS, Mitchell EA, Wright SP, Esmail A, Jones DR, Ford RP. Influences on breastfeeding in southeast England.
Acta Paediatr.1997;86:51-56.Google Scholar 15.Ford RP, Mitchell EA, Scragg R, Stewart AW, Taylor BJ, Allen EM. Factors adversely associated with breast feeding in New Zealand.
J Paediatr Child Health.1994;30:483-489.Google Scholar 16.Howard CR, Howard FM, Lanphear B, deBlieck EA, Eberly S, Lawrence RA. The effects of early pacifier use on breastfeeding duration.
Pediatrics.1999;103:E33.Google Scholar 17.Wolff PH. The serial organization of sucking in the young infant.
Pediatrics.1968;42:943-956.Google Scholar 18.Selley WG, Ellis RE, Flack FC, Brooks WA. Coordination of sucking, swallowing and breathing in the newborn: its
relationship to infant feeding and normal development.
Br J Disord Commun.1990;25:311-327.Google Scholar 19.Mathew OP, Bhatia J. Sucking and breathing patterns during breast- and bottle-feeding in
term neonates.
AJDC.1989;143:588-592.Google Scholar 20.Bowen-Jones A, Thompson C, Drewett RG. Milk flow and sucking rates during breast-feeding.
Dev Med Child Neurol.1982;24:626-633.Google Scholar 21.Smith BA, Fillion TJ, Blass EM. Orally mediated sources of calming in 1- to 3-day old human infants.
Dev Psych.1990;26:731-737.Google Scholar 22.Woodson R, Drinkwin J, Hamilton C. Effects of nonnutritive sucking on state and activity: term-preterm
comparisons.
Infant Behav Dev.1985;8:435-441.Google Scholar 23.Field T, Goldson E. Pacifying effects of nonnutritive sucking on term and preterm neonates
during heelstick procedures.
Pediatrics.1984;74:1012-1025.Google Scholar 24.Campos RG. Soothing pain-elicited distress in infants with swaddling and pacifiers.
Child Dev.1989;60:781-792.Google Scholar 25.Gunnar MR, Fisch RO, Malone S. The effects of a pacifying stimulus on behavioral and adrenocortical
responses to circumcision.
J Am Acad Child Psychiatry.1984;23:34-38.Google Scholar 26.Barr RG, Kramer MS, Leduc DG.
et al. Parental diary of infant cry and fuss behaviour.
Arch Dis Child.1988;63:380-387.Google Scholar 27.St James-Roberts I, Hurry J, Bower J. Objective confirmation of crying durations in infants referred for
excessive crying.
Arch Dis Child.1993;68:82-84.Google Scholar 28.Hunziker UA, Barr RG. Increased carrying reduces infant crying: a randomized controlled trial.
Pediatrics.1986;77:641-648.Google Scholar 29.Gray-Donald K, Kramer MS, Munday S, Leduc DG. Effect of formula supplementation in the hospital on the duration of
breast-feeding: a controlled clinical trial.
Pediatrics.1985;75:514-518.Google Scholar 30.Gray-Donald K, Kramer MS. Causality inference in observational vs. experimental studies: an empirical
comparison.
Am J Epidemiol.1988;127:885-892.Google Scholar 31.Benson K, Hartz AJ. A comparison of observational studies and randomized, controlled trials.
N Engl J Med.2000;342:1878-1886.Google Scholar 32.Concato J, Shah N, Horwitz RI. Randomized controlled trials, observational studies, and the hierarchy
of research designs.
N Engl J Med.2000;342:1887-1892.Google Scholar