Context Fatigue and distress have been separately shown to be associated with medical errors. The contribution of each factor when assessed simultaneously is unknown.
Objective To determine the association of fatigue and distress with self-perceived major medical errors among resident physicians using validated metrics.
Design, Setting, and Participants Prospective longitudinal cohort study of categorical and preliminary internal medicine residents at Mayo Clinic, Rochester, Minnesota. Data were provided by 380 of 430 eligible residents (88.3%). Participants began training from 2003 to 2008 and completed surveys quarterly through February 2009. Surveys included self-assessment of medical errors, linear analog self-assessment of overall quality of life (QOL) and fatigue, the Maslach Burnout Inventory, the PRIME-MD depression screening instrument, and the Epworth Sleepiness Scale.
Main Outcome Measures Frequency of self-perceived, self-defined major medical errors was recorded. Associations of fatigue, QOL, burnout, and symptoms of depression with a subsequently reported major medical error were determined using generalized estimating equations for repeated measures.
Results The mean response rate to individual surveys was 67.5%. Of the 356 participants providing error data (93.7%), 139 (39%) reported making at least 1 major medical error during the study period. In univariate analyses, there was an association of subsequent self-reported error with the Epworth Sleepiness Scale score (odds ratio [OR], 1.10 per unit increase; 95% confidence interval [CI], 1.03-1.16; P = .002) and fatigue score (OR, 1.14 per unit increase; 95% CI, 1.08-1.21; P < .001). Subsequent error was also associated with burnout (ORs per 1-unit change: depersonalization OR, 1.09; 95% CI, 1.05-1.12; P < .001; emotional exhaustion OR, 1.06; 95% CI, 1.04-1.08; P < .001; lower personal accomplishment OR, 0.94; 95% CI, 0.92-0.97; P < .001), a positive depression screen (OR, 2.56; 95% CI, 1.76-3.72; P < .001), and overall QOL (OR, 0.84 per unit increase; 95% CI, 0.79-0.91; P < .001). Fatigue and distress variables remained statistically significant when modeled together with little change in the point estimates of effect. Sleepiness and distress, when modeled together, showed little change in point estimates of effect, but sleepiness no longer had a statistically significant association with errors when adjusted for burnout or depression.
Conclusion Among internal medicine residents, higher levels of fatigue and distress are independently associated with self-perceived medical errors.
Medical errors and patient safety continue to be an important concern for patients and physicians, especially since the Institute of Medicine reported in 1999 that between 48 000 and 98 000 Americans die each year due to preventable adverse events.1 As many as 50% of hospitalized patients may be affected by medical errors,2,3 and the human and monetary costs of these events are great.4,5 Numerous reports have implicated fatigue and sleepiness as contributors to medical errors.1,6-9 In separate studies, resident distress has also been shown to be an important factor in self-reported major medical errors10 and medication errors.11
This research on fatigue and distress has informed the 2008 Institute of Medicine report on resident duty hours calling for prevention or mitigation of fatigue and promotion of resident well-being. Specific recommendations have primarily focused on reducing fatigue by limiting shift lengths, reducing the frequency of overnight work, and protecting time off duty.12 The costs to implement these recommendations would be great and their effectiveness is unknown,13 but there is little doubt that the proposed changes would transform the modern training environment.
As such changes are considered, it is important to note that the independent contributions of fatigue and distress to medical errors are unknown because to our knowledge, fatigue and distress have not been assessed together in prior published research. Since fatigue and distress are related but distinct entities,12 assessment of their joint effects on patient safety outcomes is a critical part of efforts to improve patient care. To address this knowledge gap, the prospective longitudinal Mayo Internal Medicine Well-being Study was extended to assess the independent contributions of fatigue and distress to self-reported medical errors when considered simultaneously, as well as to independently confirm other reports on the association of fatigue and sleepiness with errors using validated metrics and to update the previous report on the associations of quality of life (QOL), burnout, and symptoms of depression with self-perceived errors.10
All categorical and preliminary internal medicine trainees at the Mayo Clinic Rochester Internal Medicine Residency program between July 2003 and February 2009 were eligible to participate. These residents attended 163 different US and international medical schools. Curricular structure and study enrollment procedures have been described previously.10 Current duty hour regulations were in effect for the entirety of this study. Participation in this study was voluntary and written informed consent was obtained from all participants. The Mayo Clinic Institutional Review Board approved this study.
Residents were electronically surveyed every 3 months throughout their training beginning in 2003. Surveys were administered by the Mayo Clinic Survey Research Center via e-mail link to an electronic form, and automated e-mail reminders were sent to enhance response rates. Participants were given approximately 10 days to complete each survey. Surveys were administered quarterly in July-August, October-November, January-February, and April-May, although exact survey timing differed slightly from quarter to quarter.
Surveys included questions about demographic characteristics, current rotation characteristics, coping strategies for dealing with stress, and report of self-perceived medical errors. Validated survey tools were used to measure fatigue, QOL, burnout, and symptoms of depression. Self-reported medical errors, QOL, and linear analog self-assessment of fatigue were assessed quarterly, while burnout and symptoms of depression were evaluated every 6 months. Quarterly recording of the Epworth Sleepiness Scale began in July 2007. Data were analyzed through February 2009. No study responses or identifying information for individual participants were accessible to the Mayo Clinic Department of Medicine.
Self-reported Medical Errors. Perceived medical errors were evaluated by self-report every 3 months by asking residents, “Are you concerned you have made any major medical errors in the last 3 months?” As discussed previously,10 self-reported errors in this study represent major medical errors as perceived by each individual resident.
Fatigue and Sleepiness. Fatigue and sleepiness are overlapping but different concepts.14,15 Fatigue may reflect a broader sense of weariness and depleted energy, while sleepiness refers to drowsiness and decreased alertness. In this study, fatigue was measured beginning in 2003 using a standardized linear analog self-assessment question. Respondents indicated their level of fatigue during the past week according to their own definition of the term on a 0 (“As bad as it can be”) to 10 (“As good as it can be”) scale. Therefore, worsening fatigue is indicated by a decrease in fatigue score. Beginning in July 2007, the Epworth Sleepiness Scale was added to the quarterly surveys. This instrument assesses an individual's recent level of daytime sleepiness using 8 scenarios scored on a Likert scale from 0 (“No chance of dozing”) to 3 (“High chance of dozing”).16,17 A score of at least 10 is considered indicative of excessive daytime sleepiness.
QOL, Burnout, and Depression. QOL was measured by a single-item linear analog self-assessment. This instrument measured overall QOL on a 0 to 10 scale with the same anchors as the fatigue question. This scale has been validated across a wide range of medical conditions and populations.18-20
Burnout is a syndrome encompassing 3 domains (depersonalization, emotional exhaustion, and a sense of low personal accomplishment) associated with decreased work performance.21 Burnout was measured using the Maslach Burnout Inventory21 in which respondents rate the frequency of experiencing various feelings or emotions on a 7-point Likert scale with response options ranging from never to daily. Higher values of depersonalization and emotional exhaustion and lower values of personal accomplishment indicate burnout. This instrument has been used in previous studies of physicians.22-25
Depression screening used the 2-question approach described by Spitzer et al26 and validated by Whooley et al.27 This instrument has been used in a variety of patient populations26,27 including studies of physicians.10,22 This tool includes questions about depressed mood and anhedonia: (1) “During the past month, have you often been bothered by feeling down, depressed, or hopeless?” and (2) “During the past month, have you often been bothered by little interest or pleasure in doing things?” A positive screen for depression is indicated by a yes response to either question. As discussed previously,10 this screening instrument compares favorably with other depression screening instruments reported in the literature.27,28
Standard univariate statistics were used to characterize the sample. Comparisons between residents reporting errors and residents reporting no errors were initially made using summary statistics, collapsing responses within each individual into a single mean outcome.29 These were analyzed using the Wilcoxon-Mann-Whitney test for continuous variables and the Fisher exact test for proportions.
To accommodate the repeated measures study design, the association of self-perceived errors with QOL, burnout, depression, and fatigue was evaluated using generalized estimating equations—an extension of generalized linear models that accounts for correlated repeated measurements within individuals.29,30 Analyses were performed examining the association of distress and fatigue with the likelihood of a self-perceived error during the following 3 months as reported at the subsequent survey time point. Thus, the assessment of all distress variables preceded the self-reported errors.
Multicollinearity among distress variables required that each model include self-reported errors and no more than 2 distress variables. Because the multivariable models did not yield significantly different estimates of effect for any variable, we reported the simpler models with each containing only a single distress variable. Each model accounting for the effect of fatigue or sleepiness also included only 1 distress variable and either fatigue or sleepiness for the same reason. To properly calculate variance terms for repeated measures analyses, the generalized estimating equations method requires that a correlation structure be specified. Selecting the correct correlation structure for generalized estimating equations analyses does not in general affect parameter estimation, but does allow more precise estimates. An exchangeable correlation structure was specified for these analyses, and correlations between variables across time were evaluated.
With a conservative assumption of a 75% response rate, this repeated-measures study had 80% power to detect a small Cohen effect size of 0.15 for variables collected beginning in 2003 and an effect size of 0.22 for variables collected beginning in 2007. Statistical analyses were conducted using SAS version 9.1 (SAS Institute Inc, Cary, North Carolina). Statistical significance was set at the .05 level, and all tests were 2-tailed.
Of 430 eligible residents, there were 380 participants (88%) with no statistically significant differences in age, sex, or program type between participants and nonparticipants. The demographic characteristics of study participants are shown in Table 1. Of the participants, 356 (93.7%) completed at least 1 survey and 122 (32.1%) completed all surveys during the study period with a mean response rate to individual surveys of 67.5% (range, 52.2%-88.2%). In total, 2951 surveys were administered. A mean of 134 residents (range, 68-214) were surveyed in each quarter and 120 729 of 189 489 possible item responses (63.7%) were provided. Baseline participant characteristics for QOL, burnout, depression screening, and fatigue are shown in Table 2.
Errors were reported in 279 of 1950 resident-quarters (14.3%). Overall, 139 study participants (39%) reported at least 1 major medical error during the study period, and 127 of 301 residents (42%) completing at least 1 year of training reported errors. Self-perceived error rates did not vary significantly by age, sex, program type, amount of student loan debt, relationship status, or parental status.
Summary measures to identify general associations between self-perceived errors and resident fatigue, QOL, burnout, and symptoms of depression are shown in Table 2. Consistent with our previous report,10 residents reporting at least 1 error during the study period had significantly lower overall QOL (difference, −0.41; P = .02) and higher levels of burnout as evidenced by increased depersonalization (difference, 3.49; P < .001), increased emotional exhaustion (difference, 5.33; P < .001), and a lower sense of personal accomplishment (difference, −2.25; P = .001). In aggregate, 92 of 134 residents (68.7%) reporting an error screened positive for depression at least once during the study period, compared with 82 of 188 residents (43.6%) reporting no errors (odds ratio [OR], 2.83; P < .001). Residents who reported errors experienced greater fatigue as indicated by lower scores on the fatigue scale (difference, −0.54; P = .006).
Univariate associations between fatigue and distress at each time point and a self-perceived error in the subsequent 3 months are shown in Table 3. Increased fatigue and sleepiness were associated with increased odds of reporting an error in the subsequent 3 months. Each 1-point increase in fatigue or Epworth Sleepiness Scale score was associated with a 14% and 10% increase in this odds, respectively. Diminished QOL, higher levels of burnout in all domains, and positive screening for depression were also each associated with increased odds of reporting an error in the subsequent 3 months. Each 1-point increase in overall QOL and personal accomplishment was associated with a 16% and 6% decrease in this odds, respectively. Each 1-point increase in depersonalization or emotional exhaustion was associated with a 9% and 6% increase in this odds, respectively. A positive depression screen was associated with a 2.56-fold increased odds of a self-reported error in the following 3 months.
Analyses modeling distress variables together with the fatigue score showed persistent statistical significance of all variables and little change in point estimates of effect, with only 1 exception (Table 4). In the model incorporating both emotional exhaustion and fatigue, fatigue no longer had a statistically significant association with subsequent errors. Analyses modeling distress variables together with the Epworth Sleepiness Scale showed similarly minimal changes in point estimates of effect (Table 4). However, sleepiness was not significantly associated with errors when adjusted for burnout or depression. The correlation between fatigue and sleepiness was modest at 0.32, and the correlations of emotional exhaustion with fatigue and sleepiness were moderate at 0.47 and 0.42, respectively.
The results were not significantly altered by the addition to these models of other potential confounding or interacting factors (categorical or preliminary resident status, postgraduate year of training, type of clinical rotation, occurrence of a major negative life event [eg, divorce or a death in the family], occurrence of a major positive life event [eg, marriage or a birth in the family], and preferred coping strategies). Results also did not differ for residents completing all surveys vs residents with missing survey data.
The results of this prospective longitudinal study suggest that fatigue and distress are distinct, and each meaningfully contribute to risk of perceived errors when considered in concert in adjusted models. The study also confirms previously reported associations of greater fatigue and sleepiness with self-perceived major medical errors.
The ORs for self-reported errors reported in this study are of a magnitude relevant to patient safety. For example, in the adjusted model containing fatigue and depression, the OR associated with a 5-point decrease (worsening) in fatigue score is 1.59, the OR associated with a positive depression screen is 2.22, and the OR associated with both changes together is 3.52. Considering a conservative estimate of the likelihood of a perceived error in a given 3 months of 10% (less than that observed in this study and prior reports31), these ORs suggest the risk of an internal medicine resident reporting a major medical error could increase to 15%, 20%, and 28%, respectively, as fatigue, depression, or both increase. Given that changes in depression and fatigue of this magnitude were common in our study, this degree of excess risk of a reported error appears to represent a realistic concern.
Within graduate medical education, training environments that result in excessive resident fatigue have been targeted by duty hour reforms and the most recent Institute of Medicine recommendations.7,12,32,33 These results reaffirm the importance of efforts to control fatigue. However, far less attention has been directed to reducing specific elements of distress among resident physicians. Although distress may be more likely to develop after an extended burden of fatigue, these results suggest that distress can and does occur independent of fatigue. Thus, the current findings emphasize the potential importance of reducing burnout and depression and improving resident QOL as part of residency reform efforts. Additional research is needed to determine the most effective strategies for accomplishing these goals, as such strategies will likely be distinct from efforts primarily focused on fatigue reduction.
It is noteworthy that associations with self-perceived errors were generally similar for fatigue and sleepiness. Indeed, the modest observed overall correlation between fatigue and sleepiness in our study supports the notion that these variables are related but distinct. Limited power related to the smaller sample size for the Epworth Sleepiness Scale, due to its later addition to the study, may account for the less significant associations with reported errors in adjusted models incorporating sleepiness. Further work is needed to clarify the unique roles fatigue and sleepiness may play in patient safety.
This study has several limitations. First, the extent to which the self-perceived errors reported in this study accurately reflect the frequency of medical errors and whether these perceived errors actually affected patient outcomes cannot be determined. No single method of measuring errors is ideal in all settings, but previous work has suggested that self-reported adverse events may be more likely to represent preventable medical errors.34 Self-reported errors have also been shown to have good overall correlation with events documented in the medical record.35
Second, the generalizability of these results from a single academic medical center to other training programs is unknown. However, the participation and survey response rates were high relative to other physician surveys,36 and the error rates,31,37,38 burnout scores,22-25 rates of a positive depression screen,22 and fatigue levels39,40 observed in this study were similar to those found in other samples of medical residents and junior physicians.
Third, it is possible that feelings of distress or fatigue or experience with prior perceived errors might affect retrospective error reporting, although it is unclear whether such feelings would make reporting of errors more likely or less likely. This is an area worthy of additional study.
Fourth, the Epworth Sleepiness Scale measures daytime sleepiness, and the effect of daytime sleepiness on errors occurring at night is unclear. Additionally, it is unclear how acute fatigue may differ from chronic fatigue in its effect on error occurrence, and this study does not allow direct assessment of specific factors such as extended work shifts.
Fifth, the depression screening instrument cannot be used alone to diagnose depression. Although the positive likelihood ratio for this instrument is similar to that of other accepted depression screening tools,27,28 additional clinical evaluation would be necessary to diagnose depression in participants with positive screening scores.
Sixth, due to multicollinearity there was limited ability to separate the effects of all individual well-being variables from one another, or to separate the effects of fatigue and sleepiness. Larger sample sizes may be necessary to better resolve the associations among these variables. Because of these limitations, these results should be interpreted as associations rather than as definitive evidence of causation.
In summary, this study suggests that fatigue, sleepiness, burnout, depression, and reduced QOL are independently associated with an increased risk of future self-perceived major medical errors. In addition to the national efforts to reduce fatigue and sleepiness, well-designed interventions to prevent, identify, and treat distress among physicians are needed. Additional research is necessary to determine the most effective strategies for accomplishing these goals. Changes to the process of physician training should address both resident fatigue and distress in an effort to improve resident and patient safety.
Corresponding Author: Colin P. West, MD, PhD, Divisions of General Internal Medicine and Biomedical Statistics and Informatics, Departments of Medicine and Health Sciences Research, Mayo Clinic, 200 First St, SW, Rochester, MN 55905 (west.colin@mayo.edu).
Author Contributions: Dr West had full access to all of the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.
Study concept and design: West, Habermann, Sloan, Shanafelt.
Acquisition of data: West, Tan, Shanafelt.
Analysis and interpretation of data: West, Sloan, Shanafelt.
Drafting of the manuscript: West, Shanafelt.
Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: West, Tan, Habermann, Sloan, Shanafelt.
Statistical analysis: West.
Obtained funding: West, Habermann, Shanafelt.
Administrative, technical, or material support: Tan, Habermann, Sloan, Shanafelt.
Financial Disclosures: None reported.
Funding/Support: This work was supported by the Mayo Clinic Department of Medicine Program on Physician Well-being.
Role of the Sponsor: The Mayo Clinic Department of Medicine Program on Physician Well-being played no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; and preparation of the manuscript.
1.Kohn LT, ed, Corrigan JM, ed, Donaldson MS, ed. To Err Is Human: Building a Safer Health System. Washington, DC: National Academy Press; 1999
2.Brennan TA, Leape LL, Laird NM,
et al. Incidence of adverse events and negligence in hospitalized patients: results of the Harvard Medical Practice Study.
N Engl J Med. 1991;324(6):370-3761987460
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 3.Baker GR, Norton PG, Flintoff V,
et al. The Canadian Adverse Events Study: the incidence of adverse events among hospital patients in Canada.
CMAJ. 2004;170(11):1678-168615159366
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 4.Zhan C, Miller MR. Excess length of stay, charges, and mortality attributable to medical injuries during hospitalization.
JAMA. 2003;290(14):1868-187414532315
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 5.Rothschild JM, Landrigan CP, Cronin JW,
et al. The Critical Care Safety Study: the incidence and nature of adverse events and serious medical errors in intensive care.
Crit Care Med. 2005;33(8):1694-170016096443
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 6.Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality. Making health care safer: a critical analysis of patient safety practices.
AHRQ Web site. http://www.ahrq.gov/clinic/ptsafety/. Accessed May 28, 2009 7.Landrigan CP, Rothschild JM, Cronin JW,
et al. Effect of reducing interns' work hours on serious medical errors in intensive care units.
N Engl J Med. 2004;351(18):1838-184815509817
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 8.Barger LK, Ayas NT, Cade BE,
et al. Impact of extended-duration shifts on medical errors, adverse events, and attentional failures.
PLoS Med. 2006;3(12):e4871705824
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 9.Lockley SW, Barger LK, Ayas NT, Rothschild JM, Czeisler CA, Landrigan CP.Harvard Work Hours, Health and Safety Group. Effects of health care provider work hours and sleep deprivation on safety and performance.
Jt Comm J Qual Patient Saf. 2007;33(11):(suppl)
7-1818173162
PubMedGoogle Scholar 10.West CP, Huschka MM, Novotny PJ,
et al. Association of perceived medical errors with resident distress and empathy: a prospective longitudinal study.
JAMA. 2006;296(9):1071-107816954486
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 11.Fahrenkopf AM, Sectish TC, Barger LK,
et al. Rates of medication errors among depressed and burnt out residents: prospective cohort study.
BMJ. 2008;336(7642):488-49118258931
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 13.Nuckols TK, Bhattacharya J, Wolman DM, Ulmer C, Escarce JJ. Cost implications of reduced work hours and workloads for resident physicians.
N Engl J Med. 2009;360(21):2202-221519458365
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 14.Pigeon WR, Sateia MJ, Ferguson RJ. Distinguishing between excessive daytime sleepiness and fatigue: toward improved detection and treatment.
J Psychosom Res. 2003;54(1):61-6912505556
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 15.Shen J, Barbera J, Shapiro CM. Distinguishing sleepiness and fatigue: focus on definition and measurement.
Sleep Med Rev. 2006;10(1):63-7616376590
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 16.Johns MW. A new method for measuring daytime sleepiness: the Epworth Sleepiness Scale.
Sleep. 1991;14(6):540-5451798888
PubMedGoogle Scholar 17.Johns MW. Reliability and factor analysis of the Epworth Sleepiness Scale.
Sleep. 1992;15(4):376-3811519015
PubMedGoogle Scholar 18.Gudex C, Dolan P, Kind P, Williams A. Health state valuations from the general public using the visual analogue scale.
Qual Life Res. 1996;5(6):521-5318993098
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 19.Shanafelt TD, Novotny P, Johnson ME,
et al. The well-being and personal wellness promotion strategies of medical oncologists in the North Central Cancer Treatment Group.
Oncology. 2005;68(1):23-3215775690
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 20.Rummans TA, Clark MM, Sloan JA,
et al. Impacting quality of life for patients with advanced cancer with a structured multidisciplinary intervention: a randomized controlled trial.
J Clin Oncol. 2006;24(4):635-64216446335
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 21.Maslach C, Jackson SE, Leiter MP. Maslach Burnout Inventory Manual. 3rd ed. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press; 1996
22.Shanafelt TD, Bradley KA, Wipf JE, Back AL. Burnout and self-reported patient care in an internal medicine residency program.
Ann Intern Med. 2002;136(5):358-36711874308
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 24.Gopal R, Glasheen JJ, Miyoshi TJ, Prochazka AV. Burnout and internal medicine resident work-hour restrictions.
Arch Intern Med. 2005;165(22):2595-260016344416
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 25.Rosen IM, Gimotty PA, Shea JA, Bellini LM. Evolution of sleep quantity, sleep deprivation, mood disturbances, empathy, and burnout among interns.
Acad Med. 2006;81(1):82-8516377826
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 26.Spitzer RL, Williams JB, Kroenke K,
et al. Utility of a new procedure for diagnosing mental disorders in primary care: the PRIME-MD 1000 study.
JAMA. 1994;272(22):1749-17567966923
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 27.Whooley MA, Avins AL, Miranda J, Browner WS. Case-finding instruments for depression: two questions are as good as many.
J Gen Intern Med. 1997;12(7):439-4459229283
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 28.Williams JW Jr, Noel PH, Cordes JA, Ramirez G, Pignone M. Is this patient clinically depressed?
JAMA. 2002;287(9):1160-117011879114
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 29.Diggle PJ, Liang KY, Zeger SL. Analysis of Longitudinal Data. Oxford, England: Clarendon Press; 1994
30.Liang KY, Zeger SL. Longitudinal data analysis using generalized linear models.
Biometrika. 1986;73(1):13-22
Google ScholarCrossref 31.Jagsi R, Kitch BT, Weinstein DF, Campbell EG, Hutter M, Weissman JS. Residents report on adverse events and their causes.
Arch Intern Med. 2005;165(22):2607-261316344418
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 32.Gaba DM, Howard SK. Patient safety: fatigue among clinicians and the safety of patients.
N Engl J Med. 2002;347(16):1249-125512393823
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 33.Philibert I, Friedmann P, Williams WT.ACGME Work Group on Resident Duty Hours; Accreditation Council for Graduate Medical Education. New requirements for resident duty hours.
JAMA. 2002;288(9):1112-111412204081
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 34.O’Neil AC, Petersen LA, Cook EF, Bates DW, Lee TH, Brennan TA. Physician reporting compared with medical-record review to identify adverse medical events.
Ann Intern Med. 1993;119(5):370-3768338290
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 35.Weingart SN, Callanan LD, Ship AN, Aronson MD. A physician-based voluntary reporting system for adverse events and medical errors.
J Gen Intern Med. 2001;16(12):809-81411903759
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 37.Mizrahi T. Managing medical mistakes: ideology, insularity, and accountability among internists-in-training.
Soc Sci Med. 1984;19(2):135-1466474229
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 39.Handel DA, Raja A, Lindsell CJ. The use of sleep aids among emergency medicine residents: a web based survey.
BMC Health Serv Res. 2006;6:13617052349
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 40.Gander P, Purnell H, Garden A, Woodward A. Work patterns and fatigue-related risk among junior doctors.
Occup Environ Med. 2007;64(11):733-73817387138
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref