Context Previous research indicates physicians frequently
choose a patient problem to explore before determining the patient's
full spectrum of concerns.
Objective To examine the extent to which experienced family
physicians in various practice settings elicit the agenda of concerns
patients bring to the office.
Design A cross-sectional survey using linguistic analysis of a
convenience sample of 264 patient-physician interviews.
Setting and Participants Primary care offices of 29
board-certified family physicians practicing in rural Washington
(n=1; 3%), semirural Colorado (n=20;
69%), and urban settings in the United States and Canada
(n=8; 27%). Nine participants had fellowship training
in communication skills and family counseling.
Main Outcome Measures Patient-physician verbal interactions,
including physician solicitations of patient concerns, rate of
completion of patient responses, length of time for patient responses,
and frequency of late-arising patient concerns.
Results Physicians solicited patient concerns in 199 interviews
(75.4%). Patients' initial statements of concerns were completed in
74 interviews (28.0%). Physicians redirected the patient's opening
statement after a mean of 23.1 seconds. Patients allowed to complete
their statement of concerns used only 6 seconds more on average than
those who were redirected before completion of concerns. Late-arising
concerns were more common when physicians did not solicit patient
concerns during the interview (34.9% vs 14.9%). Fellowship-trained
physicians were more likely to solicit patient concerns and allow
patients to complete their initial statement of concerns (44% vs
22%).
Conclusions Physicians often redirect patients' initial
descriptions of their concerns. Once redirected, the descriptions are
rarely completed. Consequences of incomplete initial descriptions
include late-arising concerns and missed opportunities to gather
potentially important patient data. Soliciting the patient's agenda
takes little time and can improve interview efficiency and yield
increased data.
During the 1980s, renewed
interest in the patient-physician relationship and the availability of
more sophisticated audio and video technology allowed investigators to
explore medical discourse in unprecedented detail. One of the most
frequently quoted studies from this period (Beckman and
Frankel1) suggested that patients, asked to describe their
concerns by a physician, were most often redirected after the first
expressed concern and after a mean time of only 18
seconds.1 Additionally, in only 1 of 52 visits did
redirected patients return to their agenda and complete their offering
of concerns. As a result, the authors postulated that practitioners
often pursued a concern without knowing what other issues the patient
might wish to discuss or if the pursued concern was the most important
one. Although Beckman and Frankel used the term interruption
to describe this behavior, we prefer the term redirection to
indicate verbal interventions that directed the focus of the interview
before the patient had completed an initial statement of concerns.
Since others had found that patients, if given the opportunity, have an
average of 3 concerns per office visit,2,3 the chance of
ignoring important issues and creating less efficient visits seemed
realistic.
Most texts on the medical interview have advocated an
interviewing approach that solicits patients' reasons for seeking care
and encourages the practitioner to listen until all concerns have been
elicited.4-8 This component of the medical interview,
sometimes called the "survey of problems"8 or "agenda
setting,"6 precedes more focused open-ended and
closed-ended questions used to clarify further each concern. Although
the original research by Beckman and Frankel found the agenda setting
rarely completed, a number of concerns challenge whether the initial
findings are generalizable. First, 81% (n=60) of the
visits were with internal medicine residents. Second, the sample size
was small (n=74 visits and patients). Third, the
practitioners worked exclusively in an urban inner-city practice.
To address these concerns, the current study was designed to extend the
results of the original study by Beckman and
Frankel to answer the following questions. First, 12 years after
recognizing that physicians take control of the content of the
interview too early in the visit, did behavior change? Second, are the
patterns of behavior different in seasoned family physicians? Finally,
how are solicitations for additional concerns handled throughout the
remainder of an office visit?
Between June 1995 and July 1996, 300 visits to 29 board-certified
family physicians were audiotaped and transcribed. After securing
consent from both physician and patient, the recording was accomplished
by placing a microphone or minicasette recorder in the examination
room. To be included in the study, audiotapes had to include the first
physician utterance and continue until the participants left the room.
Regardless of the reason for the visit, all patients on the
physician's schedule were invited to participate by a trained research
assistant who explained that the study was being conducted to better
understand how physicians interview their patients. An attempt was made
to recruit 10 patients per physician. Physician encounters were
recorded in 1 or 2 days. Information collected from patients included
age, sex, and reason for the visit.
Physicians were selected from a convenience sample of practitioners
from 2 sources: the county membership list of family physicians
practicing in north central Colorado and a list of fellowship-trained
family physicians in the United States and Canada generated by the
Family Working Group of the Society of Teachers of Family Medicine.
Sixty-two potential physician participants were sent a letter of
invitation and contacted by telephone to explain the project. As part
of the informed consent, physicians were told that the purpose of the
study was to better understand medical interviewing. Demographic
information collected from the physicians included years of practice,
reported number of patients seen in a half day of practice, sex, and
postresidency fellowship training.
Characteristics of the study visits collected included the following:
length of visit (defined as opening utterances of either participant
until participants left the room or one of the participants clearly
terminated the discourse) and reason for visit (patient-initiated,
preventive, or physician-initiated.)
The project was approved by the IRB Committee of Poudre Valley
Hospital, Fort Collins, Colo.
Interview transcripts were analyzed using a method described by Beckman
and Frankel.1 The coding process involved several steps.
First, the physician solicitation(s) were defined as an open-ended
request for the patient's problems or reason for visit. Examples
include, "How can I help you?", "What brings you in today?", or
"Anything else?". Placement was coded as (1) the "opening of the
visit" or (2) "later." The opening of the visit consisted of the
initial greeting through pursuit of 1 specific concern. Visits in which
no physician solicitation was made were coded into a
"no-solicitation" category.
Next, for interviews in which a solicitation occurred, the patient's
response was coded as "completed" or "not completed." An
opening was coded as completed if any of the following occurred: (1) a
patient made a statement of completion (eg, "That's it"), (2) a
concern-related question was asked of the physician ("Is my chest
pain serious?"), or (3) a negative response to a physician query
about completion was made ("Anything else?"-"No").
Noncompleted sequences were coded when the physician disrupted the
patient's statement or initiated discussion of a specific topic
without determining if the patient's initial statements of concerns
were indeed completed. As in the study by Beckman and
Frankel,1 the reasons for noncompletion were coded as (1)
"closed question" ("When does the chest pain come?"), (2)
"elaborator" ("Tell me more about your pain"), (3)
"recompleter" (stroking beard, "chest pain"), or (4)
"statement" ("That sounds serious"). Elaborators often are
focused, open-ended inquiries. Although designed to facilitate patient
disclosure, they have the effect of directing the discussion toward a
particular concern. Statements and recompleters can be similarly
focused. Our coding system distinguished these focused questions and
statements from nondirective, open-ended inquiry ("Tell me more" or
"Anything else?") that was hypothesized to reduce the risk of
missing unstated concerns. After allowing the patient to describe the
full range of concerns, the physician would then be expected to explore
further using elaborators, recompleters, closed-ended questions, and
statements.
Also measured were the time in seconds for each postsolicitation
sequence, the number of concerns expressed by each patient in each
sequence and the interview, and the number of solicitation sequences
per interview.
To assess interrater reliability, 30 transcripts were coded
independently by both raters, and a κ statistic was calculated. Data
were analyzed by using both descriptive and inferential methods.
Descriptive statistics were used to present demographic data and
describe the frequency of occurrence of the communication variables
described. A χ2 test was used to assess the association
between nominal variables. The t test was used to assess the
difference in length of patient response time to solicitations in
completed and noncompleted visits and the relation between physicians'
training status and complete agenda setting. The Pearson coefficient
was used to assess the correlation between number of solicitations and
number of patient concerns expressed, and the association between
complete agenda setting and physician experience.
The initial consent rate of physicians to participate was 52% (32/62).
After initial physician agreement, the participation rate of patients
was 85% and physicians, 91% (3 consenting physicians did not
complete data collection.) The majority of the physicians were men
(79.3%), had a mean of 9.8 years of experience, and saw a mean of 11.3
patients per half day session. Nine had
completed fellowship training in family therapy and
communication skills. Twenty (69%) practiced in semirural Colorado, 1
(3%) in rural Washington, and 8 (27%) practiced in urban settings in
the United States and Canada. The majority of the patients were women
(56.4%) with an average age of 34.1 years. The mean visit length was
15 minutes, 0 seconds. The reason for the visit included
patient-initiated visits (eg, acute care, new patient with chronic
problems [51%]), physician-initiated visits (eg, follow-up to check
medication, obstetrics, chronic illness [24%]), and preventive (eg,
annual examination, well-child care [25%]).
Of the 300 audiotapes recorded, 36 were omitted from analysis because
of a delay in starting the tape recorder or poor audio quality. The
remaining 264 transcripts formed the corpus for further analysis. The
κ statistic was .66 (agreement on 26 of 30 transcripts) suggesting
moderately high agreement beyond chance between raters independently
coding for completion vs noncompletion of patient concerns.
In these 264 visits to experienced family physicians, the physician
solicited the patient's concerns in 75.4%. In the remaining 24.6%,
no solicitation was made. The distribution and placement of
solicitations is shown in Table 1. In
79% (n=157) of interviews with solicitation, the
physician asked the patient for his or her concerns either once or
multiple times only at the beginning of the visit. In 21%
(n=42) of these visits with solicitation, the physician
asked the patient for additional concerns later in the interview.
Patients completed their statement of concern(s) in only 74 (28.0%) of
the interviews. Causes for noncompletion of patients' statements are
found in Table 2. The most frequent
barriers to completion were closed-ended questioning (28.4%), absence
of solicitation (24.6%), and physician statement (14.0%).
As shown in Table 3, the number
of physician solicitations was positively associated with the number of
concerns expressed by the patient
(R262=.42, P<.001). The
mean number of concerns initiated by the patient was 1.23 (this figure
does not include concerns initiated by the physician, eg, "You're
here to check on your throat?"). The mean number of concerns per
nonsolicited visit was 0.83, compared to 1.37 concerns in solicited
visits (t262=3.09,
P=.002). For completed visits, the mean number
of concerns expressed was 1.30, while in noncompleted visits, the mean
number of concerns expressed was 1.17
(t262=0.80,
P=.43). Visit length was not associated with
completion status (15 minutes, 18 seconds and 14 minutes, 52 seconds
for completed and noncompleted visits, respectively)
(t226=0.24,
P=.81). However, the relationship between the
number of concerns expressed and visit length was statistically
significant (F7=10.36, P<.001).
The mean time available to patients to initially express their
concerns before the first physician redirection was 23.1 seconds. Most
redirections (76%) occurred after the first concern. The point of
first redirection and time to redirection in relation to concerns is
shown in Table 4.
Following the initial redirection, the patient went on to state 1 or
more additional concerns in 33% (45/137) of the interviews. The
physician made 1 or more additional solicitations in 21% (29/137) of
the interviews and, despite the redirection, the patient concerns were
eventually completed in 8% of the visits. When the additional time for
postredirection patient statements of concerns is included, the mean
total time available for patients to identify their concerns was 26.2
seconds per interview.
Completed and noncompleted statements took approximately the same time
(23.8 vs 27.7 seconds, P=.14).
Patients spontaneously initiated a new concern after the completion of
the history portion of the visit in 20.1% (N=53) of
the visits (both completed and noncompleted). The vast majority of
concerns were new medical questions directed to the physician. Late
concerns were more common when no solicitation occurred compared with
visits where a solicitation for concerns was made 1 or more times
(34.9% and 14.9%) (χ2=12.07,
P=.001). Late-arising concerns tended to be
less frequent in completed openings (15.8%) compared with noncompleted
openings (22.7%), although the difference was not statistically
significant (P=.21).
Individual physicians differed in their tendency to allow patients to
complete their statement of concerns, ranging from
0% to 75% of each physician's set of
interviews. The likelihood that a physician allowed patients to
complete their initial statement of concerns was not associated with
years of physician experience, number of patients seen per clinic day,
physician sex, patient sex, or the physician's familiarity with the
patient. Fellowship training, however, was associated with agenda
setting. Fellowship-trained physicians allowed patients to complete
their initial statements of concerns more often than the other
physicians (mean, 44% of interviews completed per physicians vs 22%
per physician, respectively)
(t27=2.71,
P=.012). Although the likelihood of physician
solicitation was not associated with the type of visit, complete agenda
setting occurred more frequently during preventive visits (47%) than
visits for acute or chronic problems initiated by the patient (20%) or
the physician (28%)
(χ22=15.16,
P=.001).
Physicians commonly redirect and focus clinical interviews before
giving patients the opportunity to complete their statement of
concerns. The relatively low frequency (28%) with which experienced
physicians solicited the patient's complete agenda is similar to the
finding (23%) of Beckman and Frankel1 12 years earlier
among resident physicians. Incomplete agenda setting was associated
with fewer patient concerns, late-arising concerns, and missed
opportunities to gather potentially important patient data. Once the
discussion became focused on a specific concern, the likelihood of
returning to complete the agenda was very low (8%).
The average length of time given patients to itemize their concerns
before the first redirection (23.1 seconds per interview) was 28%
longer than the 18 seconds reported by Beckman and
Frankel.1 When the entire visit is considered, patients had
over 26 seconds to present their agenda of concerns. Although 26
seconds may seem inadequate, it is noteworthy that patients who
initiated 1 or more concerns and were given the opportunity to complete
their concerns used an average of only 32 seconds. Given the relatively
small proportion of the interview needed to clarify the patient's
concerns, the related decreased likelihood of late-arising concerns and
the difficulty of exploring new concerns late in the visit, our data
support complete agenda setting as an efficient manner to open the
medical encounter.
Specific physician behaviors that prevented the complete
identification of patient concerns included failing to solicit the
patient's agenda (24.6%) and asking a closed-ended question following
a solicitation (28.4%). Conversely, the physician behavior associated
with soliciting the complete patient agenda was a continued query for
additional concerns (eg, "Anything else?"). Additional
solicitations often revealed additional patient concerns (Table 3).
While some physicians may avoid eliciting multiple concerns due to fear
of extending the encounter, unexpressed patient concerns may lead to a
prolonged investigation of a concern hypothesized to be the "chief
complaint," but in reality was the second most important problem.
Multiple solicitations early in the visit may enhance the efficiency of
the interview by decreasing late-arising concerns, allowing the
physician and patient to prioritize problems at the outset to make the
best use of their time and minimize implicit assumptions of what the
patient wants to discuss.
Fellowship-trained physicians solicited a complete listing of
concerns more frequently. This finding, while not surprising, confirms
that practitioners with advanced training in counseling and
communication skills conduct interviews differently than their cohorts.
The opening solicitation of patient concerns often was characterized by
an open-ended question followed by nondirective facilitating utterances
(eg, "Uh-huh" or "What else?"). Having heard the patient's
agenda, the multiple concerns were then explicitly prioritized with the
patient.
Identification of the spectrum of patient concerns has obvious
importance. However, physicians' time is limited9 and it
may not always be desirable or necessary to solicit an exhaustive list
of patient concerns rigidly at the opening of the interview. Patients
may defer emotionally laden topics until the trustworthiness of the
physician is better known or until the physician brings up the
topic.10 Sex differences and cultural values may interfere
with some patients' willingness to verbalize concerns at the opening.
Additional concerns may not occur to the patient until later in the
interview. Also, physicians vary in their style. One style that seemed
useful was to follow each open-ended solicitation with a focused
open-ended question (eg, "Tell me more about the leg pain"), then
revert back to another open-ended solicitation (eg, "Anything
else?") before moving into closed-ended questioning and the
examination. This style of interspersing agenda-setting solicitations
with focused questions occurred in 34 interviews, the majority (71%)
of which were conducted by fellowship-trained physicians. In such
cases, the coding system labeled the focused, open-ended questions as
redirections when, in fact, the interviewing style provided subsequent
opportunities for the patient to express an additional concern later
on. Despite this weakness in the coding system, the procedure was used
to make a direct comparison to the study by Beckman and Frankel. These
variations used by experienced physicians suggest that models of
medical interviewing should allow for flexibility in structure if
desired outcomes of a complete agenda and adequate problem definition
are achieved.11
Two aspects of this study warrant further comment. We use the term
redirection to indicate the physician began directing the
focus of the interview before determining whether the patient completed
an initial statement of concerns. Redirection has the same meaning as
interruption in the original article by Beckman and Frankel.
Second, patients often had further opportunities to describe their
concerns in response to focused physician questions. The length of time
reported in this study pertains to the agenda-setting portion of the
interview and should not be interpreted as the total time available
for the patient to describe his or her concerns in more detail.
The study is limited by exclusive reliance on transcripts of verbal
data. We did not code nonverbal cues, such as posture or facial
expression that may have informed the physician that the patient had
completed his or her agenda. Also, the coding system did not
distinguish cues such as inflection, tone of voice, or pauses in
communication. Reactivity to audiotaping may have altered physician and
patient behavior. For example, physicians may have been more attentive
to patients knowing that they were being audiotaped. There are data,
however, to suggest the effect of taping on patient-physician
interaction is minimal.12 We did not determine whether
physicians had information from charts or office staff that might have
influenced the need to obtain an agenda directly from the patient.
Although it is common practice for a medical assistant or nurse to
elicit patients' concerns before seeing the physician, this does not
eliminate the need to solicit additional patient concerns during the
visit. Finally, physicians who agreed to participate may have differed
from nonvolunteering physicians, possibly biasing the results.
The tendency of experienced family physicians not to solicit the
patient's complete agenda is similar to the finding of Beckman and
Frankel 15 years ago. Despite concern that a patient-centered approach
will take more time, our study further reinforces that soliciting all
of the patient's concerns does not decrease efficiency. Using a
simple opening solicitation, such as "What concerns do you have?,"
then asking "Anything else?" repeatedly until a complete agenda has
been identified appears to take 6 seconds longer than interviews in
which the patient's agenda is interrupted. Agenda setting is a
teachable and learnable skill13,14 that deserves emphasis
and reinforcement.
1.Beckman HB, Frankel RM. The effect of
physician behavior on the collection of data.
Ann Intern Med.1984;101:692-696.Google Scholar 2.Stewart M, Brown J, Levenstein J, McCracken E, McWhinney IR. The patient-centered clinical method, 3: changes in residents'
performance over two months of training.
Fam Pract.1986;3:164-167.Google Scholar 3.Kaplan SH, Gandek B, Greenfield S, Rogers W, Ware JE. Patient and visit characteristics related to physicians' participatory
decision-making style: results from the Medical Outcomes Study.
Med Care.1995;33:1176-1187.Google Scholar 4.Coulehan JL, Block MR. The Medical Interview:
Mastering Skills for Clinical Practice. Philadelphia, Pa: FA Davis
Co Publishers; 1997.
5.Levinson DL. A Guide to the Clinical Interview. Philadelphia, Pa: WB Saunders Co; 1987.
6.Smith RC. The Patient's Story: Integrated
Patient-Doctor Interviewing. Boston, Mass: Little Brown & Co Inc;
1996.
7.Frankel R, Beckman H. Evaluating the patient's primary
problem(s). In: Stewart M, Roter D, eds. Communicating With
Medical Patients. Newbury Park, Calif: Sage Publications;
1989:86-98.
8.Lipkin Jr M. The medical interview. In: Feldman MD,
Christensen JF, eds. Behavioral Medicine in Primary Care: A
Practical Guide. Stamford, Conn: Appleton & Lange; 1997:1-7.
9.Dugdale DC, Epstein RM, Pentilat S. Time, the
patient-physician relationship and managed care.
J Gen Intern
Med.In press.Google Scholar 10.Epstein RM, Morse DS, Frankel RM, Frarey L, Anderson K, Beckman HB. Awkward moments in patient-physician communication about
HIV risk.
Ann Intern Med.1998;128:435-442.Google Scholar 11.Beckman HB, Kaplan SH, Frankel R. Outcome-based
research on doctor-patient communication: a review. In: Stewart M,
Roter D, eds. Communicating With Medical Patients.Newbury
Park, Calif: Sage Publications; 1989:223-227.
12.Heath C. Body Movement and Speech in Medical
Interaction. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press; 1986.
13.Kaplan CB, Siegel B, Madill JM, Epstein RM. Communication and the medical interview: strategies for learning and
teaching.
J Gen Intern Med.1997;12(suppl 2):S49-S55.Google Scholar 14.Smith RC, Lyles JS, Mettler J.
et al. The effectiveness
of intensive training for residents in interviewing: a randomized,
controlled study.
Ann Intern Med.1998;128:118-126.Google Scholar