Context Investigators first described the night-eating syndrome (NES), which
consists of morning anorexia, evening hyperphagia, and insomnia, in 1955,
but, to our knowledge, this syndrome has never been subjected to careful clinical
study.
Objective To characterize NES on the basis of behavioral characteristics and neuroendocrine
data.
Design and Setting A behavioral observational study was conducted between January 1996
and June 1997 in a weight and eating disorders program at the University of
Pennsylvania. A neuroendocrine study was conducted from May through August
1997 at the Clinical Research Center of the University Hospital, Tromsö,
Norway.
Subjects The behavioral study included 10 obese subjects who met criteria for
NES and 10 matched control subjects. The neuroendocrine study included 12
night eaters and 21 control subjects. Behavioral study subjects were observed
for 1 week on an outpatient basis, and neuroendocrine study subjects were
observed during a 24-hour period in the hospital.
Main Outcome Measures The behavioral study measured timing of energy intake, mood level, and
sleep disturbances. The neuroendocrine study measured circadian levels of
plasma melatonin, leptin, and cortisol.
Results In the behavioral study, compared with control subjects, night eaters
had more eating episodes in the 24 hours (mean [SD], 9.3 [0.6] vs 4.2 [0.2]; P<.001) and consumed significantly more of their daily
energy intake at night than did control subjects (56% vs 15%; P<.001). They averaged 3.6 (0.9) awakenings per night compared with
0.3 (0.3) by controls (P<.001). In night eaters,
52% of these awakenings were associated with food intake, with a mean intake
per ingestion of 1134 (1197) kJ. None of the controls ate during their awakenings.
In the neuroendocrine study, compared with control subjects, night eaters
had attenuation of the nocturnal rise in plasma melatonin and leptin levels
(P<.001 for both) and higher circadian levels
of plasma cortisol (P = .001).
Conclusion A coherent pattern of behavioral and neuroendocrine characteristics
was found in subjects with NES.
Night-eating syndrome (NES), characterized by morning anorexia, evening
hyperphagia, and insomnia, was described in 1955 by Stunkard and colleagues.1 It occurred during periods of stress and was associated
with a poor outcome of efforts at weight reduction. Since 1955, NES has not
been subjected to careful clinical study, but its prevalence has been estimated
at 1.5% in the general population,2 8.9% in
an obesity clinic,3 12% of the obese patients
in a nutrition clinic,4 and 27% and 26%, respectively,
in 2 samples of severely obese persons.2,5
Although it does occur among nonobese persons, NES appears to be more common
among obese persons and to increase in prevalence with increasing adiposity.
This report describes 2 related studies designed to characterize NES.
The behavioral study attempted to define the behavioral characteristics of
the syndrome in terms of timing of energy consumption during eating episodes,
level of mood throughout the waking hours, and frequency of nighttime awakenings.
The neuroendocrine study attempted to characterize the syndrome in terms of
circadian profiles of plasma melatonin, leptin, and cortisol levels.
The behavioral study was conducted from January 1996 to June 1997 on
an outpatient basis in the Weight and Eating Disorders Program of the Department
of Psychiatry of the University of Pennsylvania School of Medicine, with the
approval of the institutional review board of the University of Pennsylvania.
Subjects were recruited by announcements in local newspapers that asked
for volunteers with morning anorexia, evening overeating, and insomnia. Candidates
for the study were interviewed by 1 of us (G.S.B.), who selected subjects
who reported the above characteristics, including consumption of at least
50% of their daily food intake after 6 PM. Exclusion criteria were serious
physical or emotional illness, including diabetes mellitus and other endocrine
disorders; use of psychotropic medications, steroids, diuretics, or hypnotics;
excessive consumption of alcohol; a concurrent eating disorder; participation
in a weight reduction program; or an occupation that involved night shifts
or other unusual time requirements that interfered with meals. The use of
tobacco, caffeine, and illegal drugs was not assessed.
Subjects consisted of 8 women and 2 men, all of whom were overweight.
A control group of 10 overweight subjects, matched for sex, age, and weight
with the night eaters, was recruited from persons who applied to take part
in the study but were found not to manifest signs of NES. To compare the size
of the ingestions, particularly at night, with the binges reported by persons
with bulimia nervosa, we enrolled 4 subjects with bulimia nervosa who followed
the standard protocol.
At the beginning of the study, subjects were given motion sensors and
instructed in their use.6,7 They
were also given a diary in which to record each nighttime awakening, all food
consumed at each ingestion, and mood on a 10-cm visual analog scale at the
time of each ingestion. After the completion of 1 week of data collection,
the motion sensors were collected and analyzed for nighttime awakenings. Records
of food consumption were analyzed for energy and macronutrient content.
The 24-hour food intake data were collected for 1 week. Since data for
the first and last days of the week sometimes did not include the full 24
hours, 5 days of complete data were analyzed graphically by examining the
cumulative energy intake from 6 AM to 5:59 AM the next morning. Data were
analyzed using generalized estimating equation8,9
regression models, which accommodated the dependence between the multiple
measurements for each subject and allowed separate line segments for the daytime
and nighttime periods for night eaters and controls. The effect of status
(night eaters or controls) on cumulative energy intake throughout the 24 hours
was tested using this model. A piecewise regression using generalized estimating
equations was also used to assess the significance of the difference between
night eaters and controls in level of mood throughout the 24 hours, as well
as the slope of the mood of the night eaters after 4 PM. A Poisson distribution,
also fit using generalized estimating equations, assessed the significance
of the difference between night eaters and controls in the number of nighttime
awakenings. A Wilcoxon test was used to compare age differences between night
eaters and controls.
The average number of eating episodes, amount of kilojoules consumed
per episode, and nighttime awakenings of the 4 women with bulimia nervosa
are reported, but, because of small sample size, data comparing them with
the night eaters and controls are not. In both the behavioral and neuroendocrine
studies, data are reported as mean (SD) if not otherwise stated, and all P values are 2-tailed.
The body mass index (BMI) of the night eaters was 28.5 (3.9) kg/m2, and that of the control subjects was 28.2 (4.9) kg/m2.
The ages of the night eaters (57.3 [12.2] years) were marginally greater than
those of the control subjects (47.1 [10.7] years; P
= .06).
The amount of food intake by night eaters and controls differed only
moderately. The night eaters consumed 12,259 (8326) kJ/d, compared with 9765
(3853) kJ/d for the control subjects (P=.05). By
contrast, the night eaters had 9.3 (0.6) eating episodes in the 24 hours,
compared with 4.2 (0.2) for the control subjects (P<.001).
Furthermore, the pattern of daytime and nighttime food intake of the 2 groups
differed dramatically (P<.001) (Figure 1). During the daytime hours, the cumulative energy intake
of the night eaters lagged behind that of the control subjects so that, in
accordance with the selection criteria, at 6 PM they had consumed only 37%
of their daily intake, compared with 74% consumed by the controls (P<.001). While the food intake of the controls slowed markedly by
8 PM, that of the night eaters continued at a rapid pace until after 12 AM.
During the period from 8 PM to 6 AM, the night eaters consumed 56% of their
energy intake, compared with 15% consumed by the control subjects (P<.001).
Figure 1 also shows that,
during the 24 hours, the average mood of the night eaters (5.1 [1.5] out of
a possible 10 units) was lower than that of the controls (7.8 [1.1]) (P<.001). Furthermore, after 4 PM, the mood of the night
eaters fell at a rate of 0.25 units per hour (95% confidence interval, 0.13-0.37)
while that of the controls remained unchanged (P<.001).
Nighttime awakenings were far more common among the night eaters (3.6
[0.9] per night) than among the controls (0.3 [0.3]) (P<.001). For the night eaters, 52% of the 178 awakenings were associated
with food intake, which averaged 1134 [1197] kJ. The carbohydrate content
of this food intake was 70.3% (12.5%) compared with 46.6% (13.6%) of kilojoules
for the rest of the day (P<.001). The ratio of
carbohydrate to protein in the snacks was 7:1. None of the 13 awakenings of
the controls was associated with food intake.
Subjects with bulimia nervosa averaged 6.2 (5.0) eating episodes per
day and consumed an average of 7987 (11,443) kJ per ingestion. None reported
nighttime awakenings.
The neuroendocrine study, conducted in the Clinical Research Department
and the Laboratory of Gastroenterology of the University Hospital, Tromsö,
Norway, from May through August 1997, investigated circadian neuroendocrine
patterns of subjects with NES. The study was conducted with the approval of
the Ethical Committee of Region V, Norway.
Night eaters were recruited, as in the behavioral study, by announcements
in local newspapers. Twelve night eaters and 21 control subjects, all women,
were selected by 1 of us (G.S.B.) using the same criteria as in the behavioral
study except that the criterion of consumption of more than 50% of the daily
food intake after 6 PM was shifted to 8 PM to allow for the later Norwegian
supper hour.
Subjects were admitted to the Clinical Research Center at 8 AM after
an overnight fast. They remained for 24 hours, during which time they were
free to move about until 11 PM, when they went to bed. Four meals of 1255
kJ each were served at 8 AM, 12 PM, 4 PM, and 8 PM. Shortly after admission,
a blood sample from fasting subjects was drawn from an indwelling catheter,
and blood was drawn every 2 hours thereafter. Blood was collected in precooled
glass tubes containing 20 mmol of EDTA and 1000 kallikrein inhibitor units
of aprotinin per milliliter of blood and kept on ice until centrifugation
at 4°C and storage at −27°C. Melatonin was measured with a commercial
kit, and lipid extraction of the samples was performed. Intra-assay and interassay
precisions were 10.8% and 15.1%, respectively. Leptin was measured using a
commercial kit, and intra-assay and interassay precisions were 2.7% and 3.9%,
respectively. Cortisol was measured using a commercial immunoassay kit, and
intra-assay and interassay precisions were 5.1% and 7.3%, respectively. Blood
glucose was measured by a glucose analyzer. Insulin was measured by radioimmunoassay.10
Every effort was made to control and equalize exposure to light. For
this purpose, subjects were admitted to the Clinical Research Center in groups
of 4. To further control exposure to light, the study was performed during
2-month periods, from May to June and July to August, when the length of daylight
hours was the same and the ambient light energy was 70 W/m2
and 1 W/m2 at 12 PM and 12 AM, respectively.
Subjects slept in a room designed to exclude any outdoor light. As an additional
precaution, nighttime blood drawings were carried out with the aid of a small
flashlight while the subjects' eyes were covered.
Statistical analysis of the differences between groups in plasma melatonin,
plasma cortisol, plasma insulin, blood glucose, and incremental plasma leptin
values (after subtracting the levels at baseline, 8 AM) during the 24-hour
observation period were evaluated by repeated measures multivariate analysis
of variance. A Wilcoxon rank sum test was used to compare age differences
and the time of the highest and lowest plasma concentrations of melatonin,
leptin, and cortisol.
The 12 night eaters were divided into 7 nonobese (BMI, 23.1 [3.6] kg/m2) and 5 obese (BMI, 36.1 [4.4] kg/m2) subjects. The BMI
of the 10 nonobese control subjects was 23.1 (1.5) kg/m2, whereas
that of the 11 obese control subjects was 30.4 (3.1) kg/m2. The
ages of the 12 night-eating subjects (53 [10] years) did not differ significantly
from those of the overweight control subjects (55 [10] years; P = .39) or those of the normal-weight control subjects (46 [11] years; P = .06). No subject reported having been diagnosed as
having sleep apnea and no episodes of sleep apnea were observed during the
studies in the clinical research department. As in the behavioral study, the
nighttime awakenings were far more common among the night eaters (3.0 [1.2])
than among the controls (0.1 [0.2]) (P<.001).
Dysregulation of the circadian levels of melatonin, leptin, and cortisol
was found among the night eaters and not among members of the 2 control groups.
Among the night eaters, both overweight and normal-weight, plasma melatonin
levels at night were lower than those of their respective control groups from
10 PM to 6 AM (P<.001) (Figure 2). The nocturnal plasma melatonin concentrations were higher
in the overweight control group than in the normal-weight control group from
2 AM to 4 AM (P<.001), but there was no difference
between the overweight and normal-weight night eaters (P=.63). Similarly, no difference was found between the normal-weight
groups (P=.11) and between the overweight groups
(P=.73) in the time of the highest plasma melatonin
concentration.
Plasma leptin levels were higher among the overweight subjects than
among the normal-weight subjects (both night eaters and controls) (P<.001) (Figure 3). When
compared with their respective control groups, the rise in the nocturnal (12
AM to 6 AM) plasma leptin levels was lower in both normal-weight and overweight
night eaters (P<.001). The time of the highest
plasma leptin concentration did not differ between the normal-weight groups
(P=.28) or between the overweight groups (P=.63).
There were no differences in plasma cortisol levels between overweight
and normal-weight subjects, either among night eaters or controls. The weight
groups were combined accordingly for comparison of night eaters with controls. Figure 4 shows that plasma cortisol levels
of the night eaters were higher than those of the control subjects from 8
AM to 2 AM (P<.002). The times of the highest
(P>.99) and lowest (P=.19)
plasma cortisol concentrations did not differ among the groups.
Blood Glucose and Plasma Insulin
The preprandial and postprandial blood glucose and plasma insulin levels
did not differ between night eaters and controls among either the overweight
or normal-weight groups.
This study revealed a surprising coherence of the behavioral and neuroendocrine
patterns of persons selected on the basis of morning anorexia, evening hyperphagia,
and insomnia. Persons selected on the basis of these minimal criteria were
found to manifest not only sleep-onset insomnia but, quite unexpectedly, nighttime
awakenings during half of which food was ingested . This distinctive circadian
pattern of behavior was associated with a similarly distinctive pattern of
mood disturbance. Contrary to the usual pattern found in depression, the mood
of the night eaters fell during the evening. The circadian neuroendocrine
findings included attenuation of the usual nighttime rise in melatonin and
leptin as well as elevated levels of plasma cortisol.
Night-eating
syndrome appears to represent a new eating disorder, different from the established
disorders of anorexia nervosa, bulimia nervosa, and binge eating disorder.
It differs from the latter 2 disorders in the frequency and size of ingestions,
particularly at night. In contrast to the very frequent night eating of persons
with NES, Greeno et al11 reported that only
6 of 40 patients with binge eating disorder ever ate at night and did so only
once a week. The size of ingestions by the night eaters (1134 kJ) is far smaller
than that of patients with bulimia nervosa (7987 kJ) whom we studied, than
that reported by Rosen et al12 (6104 kJ), by
Rossiter and Agras13 (4908 kJ), and by patients
with binge eating disorder reported by Grilo and Schiffman (5263 kJ).14 Clearly they do not represent "objective binges"15 or "night binging."
The carbohydrate-rich
(70.3%) nighttime snacks, especially the high carbohydrate to protein ratio
(7:1), suggests that night eating is designed to restore the disrupted sleep
of the night eaters. It has been reported that this pattern of eating increases
the availability of tryptophan for transport into the brain and conversion
into serotonin,16,17 resulting
in facilitation of sleep.
Night-eating syndrome appears to differ
also from the "nocturnal sleep-related eating disorders" reported by sleep
disorder clinics and characterized by eating upon awakening from sleep, often
in association with sleepwalking and related sleep disturbances. The relation
between these disorders and NES is unclear, in part because of uncertainty
regarding the nature of the former disorders. Thus, they have been reported
either mostly in infants18 or mostly in adults,19-21 primarily during
sleep18,19 or primarily during
wakefulness,20,22 and with18,19,21 or without20 polysomnographic evidence of parasomnias. These inconsistencies
may contribute to the uncertainty regarding their prevalence. For example,
Schenk and Mahowald19,22 reported
only 38 (0.5%) cases out of approximately 8000 polysomnographic examinations,
whereas Manni et al23 reported 18 (15%) cases
out of 120.
As noted above, the criterion for the time of onset
of night eating differed between the behavioral and neuroendocrine studies
to accommodate the different supper times in Philadelphia and Tromsö,
Norway. Supper occurs even later in the Mediterranean cultures but NES is
still defined there as overeating following the evening meal. These considerations
make it appropriate to establish the onset of night eating as that which occurs
after the end of the evening meal rather than by hour of the day (Table 1).
The minimal criteria used to
define NES identified not only persons with eating disorders but also those
with sleeping and probably mood disorders. The criteria also identified persons
with a distinctive neuroendocrine pattern: attenuation of the usual nocturnal
rise in melatonin and leptin levels and an elevated level of cortisol throughout
the day. There is an intriguing concurrence between the salient neuroendocrine
findings—attenuation of the nocturnal elevation in melatonin and leptin—and
the salient behavioral findings of nighttime awakenings, often linked with
eating.
Melatonin appears to induce and maintain sleep.24 Diminished melatonin levels have been associated
with impaired sleep25-28
and documented in those individuals with depression.29
It has been proposed that this hypomelatoninemia may be involved in the causal
chain leading to impaired sleep.27,28
The attenuation of the nocturnal rise in melatonin that we observed in night
eaters may well play a similar role in their nighttime awakenings.
Attenuation of the nocturnal rise in leptin may also contribute to the
nighttime awakenings and the associated food intake. Leptin levels rise at
night,30 and Sinha et al31
have proposed that this rise suppresses appetite and helps to maintain sleep,
but Matkovic et al32 suggest that attenuation
of this rise prevents adequate suppression of appetite. The latter sequence
may have occurred in night eaters in this study, with the breakthrough of
urges to eat and the resultant disruption of sleep.
The attenuation
of the nocturnal rise in leptin and melatonin may be related through the agency
of corticotropin releasing hormone (CRH), which suppresses the secretion of
melatonin.33 We did not measure CRH, but 3
findings suggest that CRH levels are increased. First, the lack of a nocturnal
increase in leptin removes the leptin-mediated inhibition of CRH, resulting
in an increase in levels of CRH.34 Second,
the elevated plasma levels of cortisol reflect increased activity of CRH.35 Third, stress influences CRH.35-37
Our relatively brief contact with the subjects of the 2 studies precludes
assessment of their level of stress, although the lower mood and its deterioration
during the course of the day is compatible with a stress response. In the
1955 study,1 however, intensive, long-term
psychotherapy of 20 night eaters revealed that NES occurred during periods
of life stress and, significantly, was alleviated with reduction of the stress.
The limitations of this report should be noted. The studies involved
a small number of subjects and were conducted in different settings with different
patients. The behavioral data were collected on an ambulatory basis during
1 week in which subjects had access to food at night. The neuroendocrine data
were collected in a clinical research department during a 24-hour period in
which the last meal was eaten at 8 PM. The distribution of ages and body weights
of subjects in the 2 studies did not differ significantly, however, and, with
4 exceptions, all subjects were women. In the neuroendocrine study, it is
possible that the restriction of nighttime food intake may have stressed the
night eaters and affected the neuroendocrine results. However, in our experience,
night eaters do not persist in their usual eating patterns in the hospital.
It was thought that our design of 4 small meals was most likely to reveal
circadian patterns. The concurrence between the behavioral and neuroendocrine
findings suggests that NES is a sufficiently robust phenomenon to be identified
despite the limitations noted in this report. Data from future behavioral
and neuroendocrine studies should, however, be collected from the same population.
Delineation of a new syndrome raises the question of treatment,
and features of NES suggest potentially fruitful approaches. Thus, the presence
of a sleep disorder linked to attenuation of the nighttime rise in melatonin
suggests administration of exogenous melatonin. The night eating related to
attenuation of the nighttime rise in leptin might be corrected by the administration
of exogenous leptin. Subjects' ingestion of high-carbohydrate snacks suggests
an attempt to improve sleep and mood by raising levels of serotonin; selective
serotonin reuptake inhibitors might more readily achieve this goal. Measures
to control stress may be useful in alleviating NES. More modern pharmacotherapy,
such as the use of CRH receptor antagonists, may be added to the old prescription
of psychotherapy.
1.Stunkard AJ, Grace WJ, Wolff HG. The night-eating syndrome: a pattern of food intake among certain obese
patients.
Am J Med.1955;19:78-86.Google Scholar 2.Rand CS, Macgregor AM, Stunkard AJ. The night-eating syndrome in the general population and among postoperative
obesity surgery patients.
Int J Eat Disord.1997;22:65-69.Google Scholar 3.Stunkard A, Berkowitz R, Wadden T, Tanrikut C, Reiss E, Young L. Binge eating disorder and the night-eating syndrome.
Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord.1996;20:1-6.Google Scholar 4.Stunkard AJ. Eating patterns and obesity.
Psychiatr Q.1959;33:284-295.Google Scholar 5.Rand CS, Kuldau JM. Morbid obesity: a comparison between a general population and obesity
surgery patients.
Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord.1993;17:657-661.Google Scholar 6.Sadeh A, Alster J, Aurbach D, Lavie P. Actigraphically based automatic bedtime sleep-wake scoring validity
and clinical applications.
J Ambulatory Monitoring.1989;25:305-319.Google Scholar 7.Wisberg J, Hauri P, Harris C. The scoring of wrist actigraphy in insomnia.
J Sleep Res.1990;19:384.Google Scholar 8.Zeger SL, Liang KY. Longitudinal data analysis for discrete and continuous outcomes.
Biometrics.1986;42:121-130.Google Scholar 9.Diggle PJ, Liang KY, Zeger SL. Analysis of Longitudinal Data. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 1994.
10.Vonen B, Florholmen J, Malm D, Torjessen P, Burhol PG. Glucagon mediates arginine-induced somatostatin secretion from isolated
rat pancreatic islets.
Scand J Clin Lab Invest.1992;52:107-112.Google Scholar 11.Greeno CG, Wing RR, Marcus MD. Nocturnal eating in binge eating disorder and matched-weight controls.
Int J Eat Disord.1995;18:343-349.Google Scholar 12.Rosen JC, Leitenberg H, Fisher C, Khazam C. Binge eating episodes in bulimia nervosa: the amount and type of food
consumed.
Int J Eat Disord.1986;5:255-267.Google Scholar 13.Rossiter EM, Agras NS. An empirical test of the DSM-III-R definition of binge.
Int J Eat Disord.1990;9:513-518.Google Scholar 14.Grilo CM, Schiffman S. Longitudinal investigation of the abstinence violation effect in binge
eaters.
J Consult Clin Psychol.1994;62:611-619.Google Scholar 15.Fairburn CG, Cooper Z. The eating disorder examination. In: Fairburn CG, Wilson GT, eds. Binge Eating:
Nature, Assessment and Treatment. 12th ed. New York, NY: Guilford Press;
1993.
16.Yokogoshi H, Wurtman RJ. Meal composition and plasma amino acid ratios: effects of various proteins
or carbohydrates, and of various protein concentrations.
Metabolism.1986;35:837-842.Google Scholar 17.Berry EM, Growdon JH, Wurtman JJ, Caballero B, Wurtman RJ. A balanced carbohydrate: protein diet in the management of Parkinson's
disease.
Neurology.1991;41:1295-1297.Google Scholar 18.American Sleep Disorders Association, Diagnostic Classification Steering
Committee. Thorpy MJ. International Classification of Sleep Disorders:
Diagnostic and Coding Manual. Lawrence, Kan: Allen Press; 1990.
19.Schenk CH, Mahowald MW. Review of nocturnal sleep-related eating disorders.
Int J Eating Disord.1994;15:343-356.Google Scholar 20.Winkelman JW, Dorsey C, Cunningham S, Lucas S. Nocturnal binge eating: sleep disorder or eating disorder?
J Sleep Res.1993;22:291.Google Scholar 21.Spaggiari MC, Granella F, Parrino L, Marchesi C, Melli I, Terzano MG. Nocturnal eating syndrome in adults.
Sleep.1994;17:339-344.Google Scholar 22.Schenk CH, Hurwitz TD, O'Connor KA, Mahowald MW. Additional categories of sleep-related eating disorders and the current
status of treatment.
Sleep.1993;16:457-466.Google Scholar 23.Manni R, Ratti MT, Tartara A. Nocturnal eating: prevalence and features in 120 insomniac referrals.
Sleep.1997;20:734-738.Google Scholar 24.Zhdanova IV, Wurtman RJ, Morabito C, Piotrovska VR, Lynch HJ. Effects of low oral doses of melatonin, given 2-4 hours before habitual
bedtime, on sleep in normal young humans.
Sleep.1996;19:423-431.Google Scholar 25.Hansen T, Bratlid T, Lingjarde O, Brenn T. Midwinter insomnia in the subarctic region: evening levels of serum
melatonin and cortisol before and after treatment with bright artificial light.
Acta Psychiatr Scand.1987;75:428-434.Google Scholar 26.Hajak G, Rodenbeck A, Staedt J, Bandelow B, Huether G, Ruther E. Nocturnal plasma melatonin levels in patients suffering from chronic
primary insomnia.
J Pineal Res.1995;19:116-122.Google Scholar 27.Haimov I, Laudon M, Zisapel N.
et al. Sleep disorders and melatonin rhythms in elderly people.
BMJ.1994;309:167.Google Scholar 28.Haimov I, Lavie P, Laudon M, Herer P, Vigder C, Zisapel N. Melatonin replacement therapy of elderly insomniacs.
Sleep.1995;18:598-603.Google Scholar 29.Kennedy SH, Garfinkel PE, Parienti V, Costa D, Brown GM. Changes in melatonin levels but not cortisol levels are associated
with depression in patients with eating disorders.
Arch Gen Psychiatry.1989;46:73-78.Google Scholar 30.Considine RV, Sinha MK, Heiman ML.
et al. Serum immunoreactive leptin concentrations in normal-weight and obese
humans.
N Engl J Med.1996;334:292-295.Google Scholar 31.Sinha MK, Ohannesian JP, Heiman ML.
et al. Nocturnal rise of leptin in lean, obese, and non–insulin-dependent
diabetes mellitus subjects.
J Clin Invest.1996;97:1344-1347.Google Scholar 32.Matkovic V, Ilich JZ, Badenhop NE.
et al. Gain in body fat is inversely related to the nocturnal rise in serum
leptin level in young females.
J Clin Endocrinol Metab.1997;82:1368-1372.Google Scholar 33.Kellner M, Yassouridis A, Manz B, Steiger A, Holsboer F, Wiedemann K. Corticotropin-releasing hormone inhibits melatonin secretion in healthy
volunteers—a potential link to low-melatonin syndrome in depression?
Neuroendocrinology.1997;65:284-290.Google Scholar 34.Heiman ML, Ahima RS, Craft LS, Schoner B, Stephens TW, Flier JS. Leptin inhibition of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis in response
to stress.
Endocrinology.1997;138:3859-3863.Google Scholar 35.Stratakos CA. Neuroendocrinology and pathophysiology of the stress system. In: Chrousos GP, ed. Stress: Basic Mechanisms and
Clinical Implications. New York, NY: New York Academy of Sciences;
1995:1-18.
36.Turnbull AV, Rivier C. Corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) and endocrine responses to stress:
CRF receptors, binding protein, and related peptides.
Proc Soc Exp Biol Med.1997;215:1-10.Google Scholar 37.Heinrichs SC, Manzaghi F, Pich EM, Britton KT, Koob GF. The role of CRF in behavioral aspects of stress. In: Chrousos GP, ed. Stress: Basic Mechanisms and
Clinical Implications. New York, NY: New York Academy of Sciences;
1995:93-104.