[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Table 1. Patterns of Participation of Original Intervention and Comparison Groups in the Chicago Longitudinal Study
Image description not available.
Table 2. Equivalence of Intervention and Comparison Groups on Selected Attributes for the Age 20 Follow-up Study and Original Sample
Image description not available.
Table 3. Adjusted Means and Differences for Child-Parent Center (CPC) Preschool and School-Age Intervention Groups*
Image description not available.
Table 4. Adjusted Means and Differences for Extended Intervention and Nonextended Intervention Groups*
Image description not available.
1.
United States, General Accounting Office.  Education and Care: Early Childhood Programs and Services for Low-Income FamiliesWashington, DC: GAO; 1999. Report No. HEHS-00-11.
2.
Nelson CA, Bloom FE. Child development and neuroscience.  Child Dev.1997;68:970-987.Google Scholar
3.
Carnegie Task Force.  Starting Points: Meeting the Needs of Our Youngest ChildrenNew York, NY: Carnegie Corporation of New York; 1994.
4.
United States, National Science and Technology Council, United States, Office of Science and Technology Policy.  Investing in Our Future: A National Research Initiative for America's Children for the 21st CenturyWashington, DC: Executive Office of the President, National Science and Technology Council; 1997.
5.
Council of Economic Advisors.  The First Three Years: Investments That PayWashington, DC: Executive Office of the President; April 1997.
6.
Karoly LA, Greenwood PW, Everingham SS.  et al.  Investing in Our Children: What We Know and Don't Know About the Costs and Benefits of Early Childhood InterventionsSanta Monica, Calif: The RAND Corp; 1998.
7.
Barnett WS. Long-term effects of early childhood programs on cognitive and school outcomes.  Future Child.1995;5:25-50.Google Scholar
8.
Consortium for Longitudinal Studies.  As the Twig Is Bent...Lasting Effects of Preschool ProgramsHillsdale, NJ: L. Erlbaum Associates; 1983.
9.
Zigler E, Taussig C, Black K. Early childhood intervention: a promising preventative for juvenile delinquency.  Am Psychol.1992;47:997-1006.Google Scholar
10.
Schweinhart LJ, Barnes HV, Weikart DP. Significant Benefits the High-Scope Perry Preschool Study Through Age 27Ypsilanti, Mich: High/Scope Press; 1993.
11.
Ramey CT, Campbell FA, Burchinal M.  et al.  Persistent effects of early childhood education on high-risk children and their mothers.  Appl Dev Sci.2000;4:2-14.Google Scholar
12.
Reynolds AJ. Success in Early Intervention: The Chicago Child-Parent CentersLincoln: University of Nebraska Press; 2000.
13.
Wilson WJ. When Work Disappears: The World of the New Urban PoorNew York, NY: Knopf, distributed by Random House Inc; 1996.
14.
Zigler E, Styfco SJ. Head Start and Beyond: A National Plan for Extended Childhood InterventionNew Haven, Conn: Yale University Press; 1993.
15.
Reynolds AJ. Educational success in high-risk settings: contributions of the Chicago Longitudinal Study.  J Sch Psychol.1999;37:345-354.Google Scholar
16.
Reynolds AJ. Effects of a preschool plus follow-on intervention for children at risk.  Dev Psychol.1994;30:787-804.Google Scholar
17.
Reynolds AJ, Temple JA. Extended early childhood intervention and school achievement: age thirteen findings from the Chicago Longitudinal Study.  Child Dev.1998;69:231-246.Google Scholar
18.
Reynolds AJ, Temple JA. Quasi-experimental estimates of the effects of a preschool intervention: psychometric and econometric comparisons.  Eval Rev.1995;19:347-373.Google Scholar
19.
Rosenbaum PR. From association to causation in observational studies: the role of tests of strongly ignorable treatment assignment.  J Am Stat Assoc.1984;79:41-48.Google Scholar
20.
Bendersky M, Lewis M. Environmental risk, biological risk, and developmental outcome.  Dev Psychol.1994;30:484-494.Google Scholar
21.
Rutter M. Psychosocial resilience and protective mechanisms.  Am J Orthopsychiatry.1987;57:316-331.Google Scholar
22.
Chicago Board of Education.  Chicago EARLY: Instructional Activities for Ages 3 to 6Vernon Hills, Ill: Institute for Development of Educational Auditing (ETA); 1988.
23.
Chicago Public Schools.  Elementary School-Criteria for PromotionChicago, Ill: Dept of Curriculum and Instruction; 1985.
24.
Office of Special Education Programs.  Implementation of the Individuals With Disabilities Education Act: Sixteenth Report to CongressWashington, DC: US Dept of Education; 1994.
25.
Temple JA, Reynolds AJ, Miedel WT. Can early intervention prevent high school dropout? evidence from the Chicago Child-Parent Centers.  Urban Educ.2000;35:31-56.Google Scholar
26.
Hieronymus AN, Lindquist EF, Hoover HD. Iowa Tests of Basic Skills: Early Primary Battery, Form 7, Teacher's GuideChicago, Ill: Riverside Publishing Co; 1980.
27.
Greene WH. LIMDEP: User's Guide, Version 7Bellport, NY: Econometric Software, Inc; 1995.
28.
Cohen MA. The monetary value of saving a high-risk youth.  J Quant Criminol.1998;14:5-33.Google Scholar
29.
Bureau of Justice Statistics.  Sourcebook for Criminal Justice StatisticsWashington, DC: US Dept of Justice; 1997.
30.
Olds DL, Eckenrode J, Henderson CR.  et al.  Long-term effects of home visitation on maternal life course and child abuse and neglect: fifteen-year follow-up of a randomized trial.  JAMA.1997;278:637-643.Google Scholar
31.
Lally JR, Mangione PL, Honig AS, Wittner DS. More pride, less delinquency: findings from the ten-year follow-up study of the Syracuse University Family Development Research Program.  Zero Three.1988;8:13-18.Google Scholar
32.
Ramey CT, Bryant DM, Wasik BH, Sparling JJ, Fendt KH, LaVange LM. Infant health and development program for low birth weight, premature infants: program elements family participation and child intelligence.  Pediatrics.1992;89:454-465.Google Scholar
33.
Shea S, Stein AD, Basch CE.  et al.  Independent associations of educational attainment and ethnicity with behavioral risk factors for cardiovascular disease.  Am J Epidemiol.1991;134:567-582.Google Scholar
34.
Wolfe BL, Behrman JR. Women's schooling and children's health: are the effects robust with adult sibling control for the women's childhood background.  J Health Econ.1987;6:239-254.Google Scholar
35.
Reynolds AJ, Temple JA, Robertson DL, Mann EA. Long-term benefits of participation in the Title I Chicago Child-Parent Centers. Paper presented at: The Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research on Adolescence; March 30-April 2, 2000; Chicago, Ill.
36.
Reynolds AJ, Chang H, Temple JA. Early childhood intervention and juvenile delinquency: an exploratory analysis of the Chicago Child-Parent Centers.  Eval Rev.1998;22:341-372.Google Scholar
37.
Wood D, Halfon N, Scarlata D, Newacheck P, Nessim S. Impact of family relocation on children's growth, development, school function, and behavior.  JAMA.1993;270:1334-1338.Google Scholar
38.
Currie J, Thomas D. School quality and the longer-term effects of Head Start.  J Hum Res.2000;35:755-774.Google Scholar
Original Contribution
May 9, 2001

Long-term Effects of an Early Childhood Intervention on Educational Achievement and Juvenile Arrest: A 15-Year Follow-up of Low-Income Children in Public Schools

Author Affiliations

Author Affiliations: Waisman Center on Mental Retardation and Human Development, and School of Social Work, University of Wisconsin-Madison (Dr Reynolds, Mr Robertson, and Ms Mann); and Department of Economics, Northern Illinois University, DeKalb (Dr Temple).

JAMA. 2001;285(18):2339-2346. doi:10.1001/jama.285.18.2339
Abstract

Context Most studies of the long-term effects of early childhood educational interventions are of demonstration programs rather than large-scale public programs. Previous studies of one of the oldest federally funded preschool programs have reported positive effects on school performance, but effects on educational attainment and crime are unknown.

Objective To determine the long-term effectiveness of a federal center-based preschool and school–based intervention program for urban low-income children.

Design, Setting, and Participants Fifteen-year follow-up of a nonrandomized, matched-group cohort of 1539 low-income, mostly black children born in 1980 and enrolled in alternative early childhood programs in 25 sites in Chicago, Ill.

Interventions The Chicago Child-Parent Center (CPC) Program (n = 989 children) provides comprehensive education, family, and health services and includes half-day preschool at ages 3 to 4 years, half- or full-day kindergarten, and school-age services in linked elementary schools at ages 6 to 9 years. The comparison group (n = 550) consisted of children who participated in alternative early childhood programs (full-day kindergarten): 374 in the preschool comparison group from 5 randomly selected schools plus 2 others that provided full-day kindergarten and additional instructional resources and 176 who attended full-day kindergartens in 6 CPCs without preschool participation.

Main Outcome Measures Rates of high school completion and school dropout by age 20 years, juvenile arrests for violent and nonviolent offenses, and grade retention and special education placement by age 18 years.

Results Relative to the preschool comparison group and adjusted for several covariates, children who participated in the preschool intervention for 1 or 2 years had a higher rate of high school completion (49.7 % vs 38.5%; P = .01); more years of completed education (10.6 vs 10.2; P = .03); and lower rates of juvenile arrest (16.9% vs 25.1%; P = .003), violent arrests (9.0% vs 15.3%; P = .002), and school dropout (46.7% vs 55.0%; P = .047). Both preschool and school-age participation were significantly associated with lower rates of grade retention and special education services. The effects of preschool participation on educational attainment were greater for boys than girls, especially in reducing school dropout rates (P = .03). Relative to less extensive participation, children with extended program participation from preschool through second or third grade also experienced lower rates of grade retention (21.9% vs 32.3%; P = .001) and special education (13.5% vs 20.7%; P = .004).

Conclusions Participation in an established early childhood intervention for low-income children was associated with better educational and social outcomes up to age 20 years. These findings are among the strongest evidence that established programs administered through public schools can promote children's long-term success.

Early educational interventions during the preschool years are widely touted as an effective way to prevent learning difficulties and to promote healthy development. Preschool programs are central to many human service reforms. State and local expenditures for preschool exceed $15 billion annually, and they are expected to continue to increase.1 Advances in the neuroscience of brain development have further accelerated interest and investments in the early years of life.2,3

The main attraction of early childhood programs is their potential for prevention and cost-effectiveness.4,5 In the past 2 decades, many studies have demonstrated the positive effects of participation in early intervention for school readiness, health status, academic achievement, reduced need for grade retention, and special education services.6-8 Evidence is emerging for delinquency prevention and higher educational attainment.9-11 Yet several limitations remain that reduce confidence in the implications of findings for policy making.

First, most evidence for the link between preschool participation and its long-term effects on child health and development comes from model demonstration programs rather than established programs implemented by school districts and human service agencies.12 Although research on model programs provides crucial information concerning what effects are possible under the most controlled conditions, evidence from larger-scale, institutionalized programs can better assess the effectiveness of the existing state and federal investments.

A second limitation of the existing research is that few studies of program impact have been conducted in inner cities with high concentrations of neighborhood and family poverty. Beginning with Head Start, preschool programs were designed to benefit children at highest risk of school failure. Given increasing concentrations of social disadvantage in many urban settings,13 corresponding evidence about the compensatory effects of early childhood programs is warranted.

Finally, the impact of the length of participation has not been systematically investigated. Previous studies do not have sufficient sample sizes and variation in length of participation to investigate this issue. Knowledge about the added value of programs that continue into the primary grades may reveal, for example, the extent to which the fading effects of intervention on some outcomes can be moderated or reversed.7,14

In this report, we present evidence from the Chicago Longitudinal Study on the long-term effects of a preventive intervention called the Chicago Child-Parent Center (CPC) program.12,15 Located in 24 centers in high-poverty neighborhoods, this ESEA (Elementary and Secondary Education Act) Title I program began in 1967 and is the country's second oldest (after Head Start) federal preschool program and the oldest extended early intervention. We investigated the link between program participation and educational attainment by age 20 years, official juvenile arrests by age 18 years, and need for school remedial services. Earlier studies have found that program participation beginning in preschool is significantly associated with greater cognitive skills at school entry, higher school achievement in elementary school, and reduced rates of grade retention and special education by early adolescence.12,16,17 The duration of program participation also is positively associated with school performance.16 We expected this pattern of results would lead to higher rates of school completion and decreased likelihood of juvenile crime and remedial services.

Methods
Sample and Design

Data are from the Chicago Longitudinal Study, a prospective investigation that tracks the well-being of a same-age cohort of 1539 low-income minority children (93% black and 7% Hispanic) born in 1980 who attended early childhood programs in 25 sites in 1985-1986.15 Since 1985, data have been collected yearly on educational and family experiences from school records and participant surveys. The original sample included the entire cohort of 989 children who completed preschool and kindergarten in all 20 CPCs with combined programs. School-age services are provided in first to third grades in schools affiliated with the centers. The comparison group of 550 children in this nonrandomized cohort design participated in alternative early childhood programs (full-day kindergarten). The preschool comparison group included 374 children comprising the entire kindergarten class in 5 randomly selected schools plus 2 others that provided full-day kindergarten and additional instructional resources (23% enrolled in Head Start). An additional 176 children attended full-day kindergartens in 6 CPCs without preschool participation. They were eligible to receive some program services (ie, parent resources) but were located in separate classrooms. Because these 2 groups had similar demographic profiles, they were combined for analysis.

The intervention group was matched on age of kindergarten entry, eligibility for and participation in government-funded programs, and neighborhood and family poverty.12,15Neighborhood poverty is defined as residence in a Title I school attendance area. Family poverty is defined as eligibility for the subsidized lunch program (130% of the federal poverty level). All intervention and comparison group children were eligible and participated in the study under informed consent. The legal and ethical requirements to serve children most in need prevented random assignment in this established program. The study was approved by the institutional review board at the University of Wisconsin-Madison.

The design of the study assessed the impact of 3 measures of CPC participation. For preschool participation, children entering the program at ages 3 or 4 years (original cohort, n = 989) were compared with all other children in the study who did not participate in CPC preschool but had the alternative full-day kindergarten (preschool comparison group, n = 550; Table 1). The effects of school-age intervention, which was available to any child attending a program school, were estimated by comparing children participating for at least 1 year from first to third grade regardless of whether they participated in preschool (n = 850) with those with no participation in the school-age program (school-age comparison group, n = 689). The effects of each program component were estimated while controlling for the influence of the other. The effects of participation in extended intervention were estimated by comparing children who entered the CPCs in preschool and continued their participation through second or third grade (for 4-6 years, n = 553) with all other children with less participation in either preschool or first to third grades (nonextended intervention group, 1-4 years, n = 602). The pattern of participation and postprogram data collection are shown in Table 1.

The validity of the estimated intervention effects is strengthened by the following study features. First, most children in the preschool and school-age comparison groups did not enroll in the program because they did not live in the attendance area of the CPCs. Thus, home residency rather than parent interest determined their participation. Second, by comparing groups that received different intervention services, findings in this report estimate the value added by the CPC program above and beyond participation in more typical programs. Third, to assess the impact of extended vs nonextended participation, results of word analysis skill tests at the end of kindergarten were used as a control variable. Finally, previous studies of this project16,18 support the equivalence of the program groups and show no evidence of selection bias that would alter findings. Accordingly, confidence is high that the group differences reflect true program effects.19

Follow-up Study and Comparability of Intervention Groups

At age 20 years, 83.2% of the original sample (n = 1281) had data on educational attainment (84.6% and 80.7%, respectively, for the preschool and comparison groups) with no evidence of selective attrition in this study or previously.12,17 Rates of sample recovery were even higher for juvenile court records. As shown in Table 2, both the age 20 follow-up samples and original sample were similar on many child and family characteristics. The characteristics were measured from school records and family surveys up to age 12 years. The means of the risk index, a sum of 6 dichotomous factors associated with lower child health and with cumulative effects of child risk factors on later outcomes in many previous studies20,21 (ie, low parent education [parents did not complete high school], neighborhood poverty [≥60% of children in attendance area reside in low-income families], low family income [eligibility for the subsidized lunch program], single-parent family status, not employed full- or part-time [parent report of less than full-time employment], and large family size [ ≥4 children in the family as reported by parents]) were equal between groups. Extended and school-age intervention groups showed similar patterns. Consistent with developmental research, the risk index provides a summary measure of the cumulative effects of child risk factors on later outcomes.20,21 It was significantly associated with all but 1 child outcome. Rates of reported child abuse and neglect and births to teenage mothers also were similar between groups. Among the age 20 follow-up samples, the CPC preschool group had a higher proportion of girls and parents who had completed high school and fewer siblings. Alternatively, the preschool group was more likely than the comparison group to reside in higher-poverty neighborhoods and had higher unemployment rates. The latter differences are the result of the centers being located in the most disadvantaged neighborhoods, leading to conservative estimates of effects.

Intervention

The CPC is described fully in previous reports.12,16 It provides educational and family-support services to children ages 3 to 9 years (preschool to second or third grade). The centers serve 100 to 150 three- to five-year-olds in separate facilities or in wings of neighborhood schools. The centers are located in the poorest neighborhoods in Chicago. The mean rate of family poverty in 1989 for the community areas serving the CPCs was 41% compared with 17% for other areas of the city.12 Each center is directed by a head teacher and 2 coordinators. The parent-resource teacher coordinates the family-support component. The school-community representative provides outreach to families. The eligibility criteria for the program are (1) residence in a high-poverty (Title I) school area, (2) demonstration of educational need due to poverty and associated factors as assessed by a screening interview and community outreach by center staff, and (3) parent(s) agree to participate. Rates of participation exceeded 80%, as the program was located in areas not being served by other preschool programs.

The intervention emphasizes the acquisition of basic skills in language arts and math through relatively structured but diverse learning experiences (eg, whole class, small groups). After full-day or part-day kindergarten, continuing services are provided in the affiliated schools under the direction of the curriculum parent-resource teacher.12 Participation in the school-age intervention is open to any child in the school, either in first and second grade in 14 sites or first through third grade in 6 sites.

The following features are central to the program: (1) a structured set of learning activities as described in the instructional guide22; (2) low child to teacher ratios in preschool (17 to 2) and kindergarten (25 to 2); (3) a multifaceted parent program that includes participating in activities in the parent resource room with other parents (eg, educational workshops, reading groups, and craft projects), volunteering in the classroom, attending school events and field trips, and completing high school; (4) outreach activities including resource mobilization, home visitation, and enrollment of children; (5) ongoing staff development; (6) health and nutrition services, including health screening, speech therapy, and nursing and meal services; and (7) comprehensive school-age services to support the school transition through reduced class sizes (25 from ≥35 children), the addition of teacher aides, parent-program activities, extra instructional supplies, and coordinated instructional activities. The mean per-child expenditures in 1996 for 1 year of preschool and 1 year of school-age participation are $4350 and $15.00.12

Outcome Variables

Indicators of Educational Attainment. Three measures of educational attainment of youth by age 20 years (mean 19.7 years, January 2000) were included. These measures were extracted from administrative records in all schools youth attended and were supplemented by interviews with family members. High school completion measured whether youth completed their secondary education with an official diploma or were awarded a General Education Diploma (GED). All others, including those who remained in high school, were coded as "noncompleters." Highest grade completed was an ordinal indicator of educational attainment: the minimum value was 6 and the maximum value was 12 (graduation or GED). School dropout measured whether youth left formal education in an elementary school or in a diploma-granting high school prior to graduation for any reason other than school transfer. Youth who enrolled in a GED or equivalent program were coded as "dropouts." Graduates and active high school students were coded as "nondropouts."

Official Juvenile Arrests. Several indicators of juvenile arrests reported to the Cook County Juvenile Court and 2 other locations were analyzed. These arrests occurred between ages 10 and 18 years (from 1990 to 1998). They consist of formal petitions for youth who are arrested on criminal charges and go before a judge. Some petitions result in warnings or referrals to social service agencies. The indicators were the incidence of juvenile arrest (≥1 arrest), the incidence of multiple arrests (≥2 arrests), and the number of arrests. Arrests were further divided into those involving violent offenses (eg, assault, robbery) and nonviolent offenses (eg, property theft, drug possession). Data were collected through record searches at the juvenile court in spring and summer of 1999 without knowledge of youths' program participation. Searches were repeated twice for 5% random samples and verified against computer records. To be included in the analysis, youth had to reside in Chicago at age 10 years or older. The number of arrests ranged from 0 to 15 and included up to 38 individual charges. Property offenses were the most common, followed by violent and drug offenses.

School Remedial Services. Two cumulative measures of school-related competency indexed the receipt of remedial services. Data came from school administrative records. Incidence of grade retention was defined as whether children repeated a grade from kindergarten through the eighth grade (age 15 years) because of failure to meet minimum levels of performance.23 Once in high school, students are no longer formally retained in grade. Special education services were measured in 2 ways: number of years children received special education services from ages 6 to 18 years (grades 1-12) and incidence (any and ≥2) of special education services. Most children receiving special education services participated in the regular school program. The most frequent categories of placement (based in part on federal definitions) were specific learning disability, behavioral disorder, and speech and language impairments.24

Statistical Analysis

Following previous analyses in this project,17,25 intervention effects were estimated by probit and negative binomial regression within an alternative-program design. First, the impact of CPC preschool participation (1 or 2 years vs 0) and school-age participation (1-3 years vs 0) were assessed by including 2 dummy variables in the model. Second, the effects of participation in the extended intervention were assessed by estimating regressions with a dummy variable indicating children's participation for 4 to 6 years (preschool starting at age 3 or 4 years to second or third grade) vs nonextended participation for 1 to 4 years (all other children with any preschool or school-age participation). Analyses that included children with 0 years of participation or with only preschool participation yielded similar estimates of extended participation. Adjusted coefficients and group differences denote effects above and beyond the influence of the covariates. The covariates were sex of child, race/ethnicity, risk index, earlier/later program participation, and 20 dummy variables representing the sites of the program. All have demonstrated significant associations with child outcomes in previous studies. The program site indicators measure the local influences associated with attendance in a particular center. Results were unaffected by alternative covariate specifications, such as the individual risk indicators entered separately, and the addition of other indicators of family and neighborhood disadvantage (Table 1). To assess the effects of extended program participation, word analysis score results at the end of kindergarten on the Iowa Tests of Basic Skills were included.26

Probit regression analysis was used to estimate coefficients for the dichotomous outcomes of educational attainment (high school completion and school dropout) and the incidence of juvenile arrests, grade retention, and special education placement. Negative binomial regression analysis was used for the outcomes based on count data, including the number of years receiving special education services, the number of arrests (total, violent, and nonviolent), and the highest grade completed (with upper truncation). To enhance interpretability, the coefficients from these analyses were transformed to marginal effects using LIMDEP.27 Consistent with previous studies,12,17 corrections for nonrandom attrition and clustering (random-effects model) proved unnecessary and did not affect estimates. Similarly, no significant across-equation correlations were detected in models estimating the presence of selection bias into or out of the program. Following previous analyses, interaction terms were tested for program by sex of child, neighborhood poverty, and the risk index.

Results
Educational Attainment

Preschool Participation. Relative to the preschool comparison group and adjusting for the covariates, including school-age participation, preschool participants had a significantly higher rate of high school completion at age 20 years (49.7% vs 38.5%, P = .01) and a lower rate of school dropout (46.7% vs 55.0%, P = .047; Table 3). Preschool participants also completed more years of education than the comparison group (10.6 vs 10.2 years, P = .03).

Boys benefitted from preschool participation more than girls, but only for school dropout was the program by sex of child interaction significant (P = .03). Adjusted rates of school dropout between groups were substantially lower for boys (51.0% vs 67.7%, P = .004) but not for girls (42.4% vs 41.7%, P = .90). This finding is notable given that black males are at highest risk of school failure. Differences in rates of high school completion between groups also favored boys (42.6% vs 29.0%, P = .02) over girls (56.5% vs 48.0%, P = .17).

School-Age Participation. Relative to the school-age comparison group and controlling for other model variables, including preschool participation, school-age participation was not associated with any measure of educational attainment (Table 3).

Extended Program Participation. Although children with extended intervention for 4 to 6 years had the highest levels of educational attainment, these higher levels were, on average, not significantly different from children with nonextended program participation (P = .19; Table 4). School dropout rates for program participants were significantly lower than the nonextended group in the highest poverty neighborhoods (P = .048).

Official Juvenile Arrests

Preschool Participation. Preschool participation was associated with a significantly lower rate and number of juvenile arrests. The adjusted rate of arrest was 16.9% for the preschool group and 25.1% for the preschool comparison group (P = .003; Table 3). Preschool participants also had a lower rate of multiple arrests (9.5% vs 12.8%, P = .01) and violent arrests (9.0% vs 15.3%, P = .002). No differences in effects were detected by sex of child, risk index, and neighborhood poverty.

School-Age Participation. Unlike preschool participation, school-age participation was not associated with lower arrest rates or with fewer arrests for any measure (Table 3).

Extended Program Participation. Relative to nonextended participation, extended participation was marginally associated with only a lower rate of violent arrests (P = .09; Table 4). Rates of multiple violent arrests for participants were significantly lower than those of the nonextended group at higher levels of the risk index (P = .03).

School Remedial Services

Preschool Participation. Relative to the preschool comparison group, preschool participation was associated with significantly lower rates of grade retention (23.0% vs 38.4%; P <.001; Table 3) and special education placement (14.4% vs 24.6%; P <.001). Moreover, the program group spent, on average, 0.7 years in special education compared with 1.4 years for comparison counterparts.

School-Age Participation. As shown in Table 3, participation in the school-age program for at least 1 year was associated with significantly lower rates of special education (15.4% vs 21.3%, P = .02), multiple years of special education (13.9% vs 18.4%, P = .01), and grade retention (23.8% vs 34.3%, P = .001).

Extended Program Participation. As shown in Table 4, participation in the extended program was associated with lower rates of grade retention (21.9% vs 32.3%, P = .001) and 2 of the 3 measures of special education placement, including any placement (13.5% vs 20.7%, P = .004), above and beyond less extensive participation. Children with 5 or 6 years of participation had the lowest rates of remediation.

Comment

This study makes 3 contributions to the literature on child health and development. First, as one of the most comprehensive longitudinal studies of a large-scale early intervention on education and crime, the finding that preschool participation was associated with a significantly higher rate of school completion demonstrates that established public programs can have a positive impact through early adulthood. To date, almost all evidence for the effects of early intervention on educational attainment comes from model programs rather than large-scale programs.6,8,10 The largest increases in educational attainment (especially dropout rates) occurred for boys in the program. This may be explained by the finding that boys experienced a greater cognitive advantage at age 5 from preschool participation,12,18 culminating in larger educational benefits.

The second major contribution was the finding that participation in CPC preschool was associated with significantly lower rates of juvenile arrest. This is the only study of a contemporary preschool intervention reporting crime prevention effects. Preschool participants had lower rates of arrest and multiple arrest for all types of offenses. Given the high costs of treatment and incarceration,28,29 the results of this study reinforce those of model programs10,30,31 and demonstrate the value of public programs in reducing delinquency.

Third, participation in the extended childhood intervention program was associated with lower rates of special education and grade retention by late adolescence. Consistent with previous studies of the project,12,17 programs that extend into the primary grades can enhance school performance above and beyond less extensive intervention. That extended intervention was not significantly associated with educational attainment and official arrests suggests some limits to its long-term benefits. This may be due to the less intensive services of the school-age intervention as well to the conservative bias of the comparisons made. The nonextended group had some intervention exposure and was enrolled in school full-time. Nevertheless, participants in extended intervention consistently outperformed their comparison counterparts and had the highest levels of performance across outcomes.

As preventive interventions, the Chicago CPCs and others like it have advantages over other programs. They generally provide greater levels of intensity, longer durations, and comprehensive services. These attributes make it more likely that child outcomes will be improved.32 The demonstrated impact on education attainment is especially significant given its link to health status and lower disease risk.33,34 Given that the annual cost to society of school dropout and crime is estimated at $350 billion,4,28 study findings suggest that the benefits to society of program participation can exceed costs.35

While the results demonstrate the long-term benefits of early intervention, they also show the limits of intervention in meeting children's educational needs. Like earlier studies, rates of school dropout and delinquency for program participants are substantially higher than for children nationally. Although early intervention can provide a stronger foundation for learning than would otherwise be expected, it alone cannot ameliorate the effects of continuing disadvantages children may face.

Three limitations of this study are notable. The first is that while selection bias into the program appeared to be controlled, a randomized design would have further strengthened inferences as would have additional preschool baseline measures. Several study features and results, however, increase confidence in the validity of findings. Groups were reasonably well matched at the beginning of the study. Some of the differences that did exist (eg, neighborhood poverty) worked against the program group; others were included in the analysis. In addition, unlike many previous studies, comparison groups participated in an alternative early childhood program, generating more conservative estimates. Finally, extensive analyses of selection bias with alternative covariates and comparison groups have been conducted and findings continue to be robust.12,17,18

Another limitation concerns measurement of 2 outcomes. Official juvenile arrests is only 1 indicator of crime. Convictions and sentencing were not measured. Alternative measures, such as self-reports and school reports, have led to different estimates.36 Nevertheless, juvenile arrests are important predictors of adult crime. Second, educational attainment is likely to change as young adults reenter educational institutions. This process will continue to be monitored.

The third limitation is that while the findings of the study are more generalizable to contemporary federal and state programs than previous studies, they should be applied cautiously outside large urban cities with high proportions of black children. While the CPC program has a history of successful implementation in public schools, very few programs other than Head Start have this implementation experience.

One major question outstanding is the mechanisms that explain the link between program participation and later outcomes. Three seem likely given the program goals.12 One is that participation leads to cognitive advantages at school entry that increased educational and social success. A second is that program participation enhances family support behaviors on behalf of children that promote well-being. The third is that program graduates attain higher levels of success because of the school support they experience in the years after the program, either by attending higher-quality schools or having fewer school moves.36,37 Previous studies support the credibility of these hypotheses,10,38 and they deserve further investigation.

This study indicates that public investments in early educational programs in the first decade of life can contribute positively to children's later success. Replication and extension of findings to other locations and samples will further strengthen confidence in the benefits of large-scale preventive interventions for young children.

References
1.
United States, General Accounting Office.  Education and Care: Early Childhood Programs and Services for Low-Income FamiliesWashington, DC: GAO; 1999. Report No. HEHS-00-11.
2.
Nelson CA, Bloom FE. Child development and neuroscience.  Child Dev.1997;68:970-987.Google Scholar
3.
Carnegie Task Force.  Starting Points: Meeting the Needs of Our Youngest ChildrenNew York, NY: Carnegie Corporation of New York; 1994.
4.
United States, National Science and Technology Council, United States, Office of Science and Technology Policy.  Investing in Our Future: A National Research Initiative for America's Children for the 21st CenturyWashington, DC: Executive Office of the President, National Science and Technology Council; 1997.
5.
Council of Economic Advisors.  The First Three Years: Investments That PayWashington, DC: Executive Office of the President; April 1997.
6.
Karoly LA, Greenwood PW, Everingham SS.  et al.  Investing in Our Children: What We Know and Don't Know About the Costs and Benefits of Early Childhood InterventionsSanta Monica, Calif: The RAND Corp; 1998.
7.
Barnett WS. Long-term effects of early childhood programs on cognitive and school outcomes.  Future Child.1995;5:25-50.Google Scholar
8.
Consortium for Longitudinal Studies.  As the Twig Is Bent...Lasting Effects of Preschool ProgramsHillsdale, NJ: L. Erlbaum Associates; 1983.
9.
Zigler E, Taussig C, Black K. Early childhood intervention: a promising preventative for juvenile delinquency.  Am Psychol.1992;47:997-1006.Google Scholar
10.
Schweinhart LJ, Barnes HV, Weikart DP. Significant Benefits the High-Scope Perry Preschool Study Through Age 27Ypsilanti, Mich: High/Scope Press; 1993.
11.
Ramey CT, Campbell FA, Burchinal M.  et al.  Persistent effects of early childhood education on high-risk children and their mothers.  Appl Dev Sci.2000;4:2-14.Google Scholar
12.
Reynolds AJ. Success in Early Intervention: The Chicago Child-Parent CentersLincoln: University of Nebraska Press; 2000.
13.
Wilson WJ. When Work Disappears: The World of the New Urban PoorNew York, NY: Knopf, distributed by Random House Inc; 1996.
14.
Zigler E, Styfco SJ. Head Start and Beyond: A National Plan for Extended Childhood InterventionNew Haven, Conn: Yale University Press; 1993.
15.
Reynolds AJ. Educational success in high-risk settings: contributions of the Chicago Longitudinal Study.  J Sch Psychol.1999;37:345-354.Google Scholar
16.
Reynolds AJ. Effects of a preschool plus follow-on intervention for children at risk.  Dev Psychol.1994;30:787-804.Google Scholar
17.
Reynolds AJ, Temple JA. Extended early childhood intervention and school achievement: age thirteen findings from the Chicago Longitudinal Study.  Child Dev.1998;69:231-246.Google Scholar
18.
Reynolds AJ, Temple JA. Quasi-experimental estimates of the effects of a preschool intervention: psychometric and econometric comparisons.  Eval Rev.1995;19:347-373.Google Scholar
19.
Rosenbaum PR. From association to causation in observational studies: the role of tests of strongly ignorable treatment assignment.  J Am Stat Assoc.1984;79:41-48.Google Scholar
20.
Bendersky M, Lewis M. Environmental risk, biological risk, and developmental outcome.  Dev Psychol.1994;30:484-494.Google Scholar
21.
Rutter M. Psychosocial resilience and protective mechanisms.  Am J Orthopsychiatry.1987;57:316-331.Google Scholar
22.
Chicago Board of Education.  Chicago EARLY: Instructional Activities for Ages 3 to 6Vernon Hills, Ill: Institute for Development of Educational Auditing (ETA); 1988.
23.
Chicago Public Schools.  Elementary School-Criteria for PromotionChicago, Ill: Dept of Curriculum and Instruction; 1985.
24.
Office of Special Education Programs.  Implementation of the Individuals With Disabilities Education Act: Sixteenth Report to CongressWashington, DC: US Dept of Education; 1994.
25.
Temple JA, Reynolds AJ, Miedel WT. Can early intervention prevent high school dropout? evidence from the Chicago Child-Parent Centers.  Urban Educ.2000;35:31-56.Google Scholar
26.
Hieronymus AN, Lindquist EF, Hoover HD. Iowa Tests of Basic Skills: Early Primary Battery, Form 7, Teacher's GuideChicago, Ill: Riverside Publishing Co; 1980.
27.
Greene WH. LIMDEP: User's Guide, Version 7Bellport, NY: Econometric Software, Inc; 1995.
28.
Cohen MA. The monetary value of saving a high-risk youth.  J Quant Criminol.1998;14:5-33.Google Scholar
29.
Bureau of Justice Statistics.  Sourcebook for Criminal Justice StatisticsWashington, DC: US Dept of Justice; 1997.
30.
Olds DL, Eckenrode J, Henderson CR.  et al.  Long-term effects of home visitation on maternal life course and child abuse and neglect: fifteen-year follow-up of a randomized trial.  JAMA.1997;278:637-643.Google Scholar
31.
Lally JR, Mangione PL, Honig AS, Wittner DS. More pride, less delinquency: findings from the ten-year follow-up study of the Syracuse University Family Development Research Program.  Zero Three.1988;8:13-18.Google Scholar
32.
Ramey CT, Bryant DM, Wasik BH, Sparling JJ, Fendt KH, LaVange LM. Infant health and development program for low birth weight, premature infants: program elements family participation and child intelligence.  Pediatrics.1992;89:454-465.Google Scholar
33.
Shea S, Stein AD, Basch CE.  et al.  Independent associations of educational attainment and ethnicity with behavioral risk factors for cardiovascular disease.  Am J Epidemiol.1991;134:567-582.Google Scholar
34.
Wolfe BL, Behrman JR. Women's schooling and children's health: are the effects robust with adult sibling control for the women's childhood background.  J Health Econ.1987;6:239-254.Google Scholar
35.
Reynolds AJ, Temple JA, Robertson DL, Mann EA. Long-term benefits of participation in the Title I Chicago Child-Parent Centers. Paper presented at: The Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research on Adolescence; March 30-April 2, 2000; Chicago, Ill.
36.
Reynolds AJ, Chang H, Temple JA. Early childhood intervention and juvenile delinquency: an exploratory analysis of the Chicago Child-Parent Centers.  Eval Rev.1998;22:341-372.Google Scholar
37.
Wood D, Halfon N, Scarlata D, Newacheck P, Nessim S. Impact of family relocation on children's growth, development, school function, and behavior.  JAMA.1993;270:1334-1338.Google Scholar
38.
Currie J, Thomas D. School quality and the longer-term effects of Head Start.  J Hum Res.2000;35:755-774.Google Scholar
×