Context Cognitive impairments are associated with long-term cannabis use, but
the parameters of use that contribute to impairments and the nature and endurance
of cognitive dysfunction remain uncertain.
Objective To examine the effects of duration of cannabis use on specific areas
of cognitive functioning among users seeking treatment for cannabis dependence.
Design, Setting, and Participants Multisite retrospective cross-sectional neuropsychological study conducted
in the United States (Seattle, Wash; Farmington, Conn; and Miami, Fla) between
1997 and 2000 among 102 near-daily cannabis users (51 long-term users: mean,
23.9 years of use; 51 shorter-term users: mean, 10.2 years of use) compared
with 33 nonuser controls.
Main Outcome Measures Measures from 9 standard neuropsychological tests that assessed attention,
memory, and executive functioning, and were administered prior to entry to
a treatment program and following a median 17-hour abstinence.
Results Long-term cannabis users performed significantly less well than shorter-term
users and controls on tests of memory and attention. On the Rey Auditory Verbal
Learning Test, long-term users recalled significantly fewer words than either
shorter-term users (P = .001) or controls (P = .005); there was no difference between shorter-term
users and controls. Long-term users showed impaired learning (P = .007), retention (P = .003), and retrieval
(P = .002) compared with controls. Both user groups
performed poorly on a time estimation task (P<.001
vs controls). Performance measures often correlated significantly with the
duration of cannabis use, being worse with increasing years of use, but were
unrelated to withdrawal symptoms and persisted after controlling for recent
cannabis use and other drug use.
Conclusions These results confirm that long-term heavy cannabis users show impairments
in memory and attention that endure beyond the period of intoxication and
worsen with increasing years of regular cannabis use.
In the current climate of debate about marijuana laws and interest in
marijuana as medicine,1 one issue remains unresolved:
Does heavy, frequent, or prolonged use of cannabis lead to a deterioration
in cognitive function that persists well beyond any period of acute intoxication?
Is the functioning of the brain altered in the long term? With over 7 million
people using cannabis weekly or more often in the United States alone2 and the potential for increased physician recommendations
for select patients to use cannabis therapeutically,1
answers to these questions are of significant public health concern.3,4 Scientific evidence from past research
clearly showed that gross impairment related to chronic cannabis use did not
occur but was inconclusive with regard to the presence of more specific deficits.5,6 Recent studies with improved methods
have demonstrated changes in cognition and brain function associated with
long-term or frequent use of cannabis. Specific impairments of attention,
memory, and executive function have been found in cannabis users in the unintoxicated
state (and in children exposed to cannabis in utero7)
in controlled studies using brain event-related potential techniques6,8-10 and
neuropsychological assessments11-15
including complex tasks.
Brain imaging studies of cannabis users have demonstrated altered function,
blood flow, and metabolism in prefrontal and cerebellar regions.16-19
Studies failing to detect cognitive decline associated with cannabis use20 may reflect insufficient heavy or chronic use of
cannabis in the sample or the use of insensitive assessment instruments. Impairments
appear to increase with duration and frequency of cannabis use; however, the
parameters of use that are associated with short- or long-lasting cognitive
and brain dysfunction have not been fully elucidated. The attribution of deficits
to lingering acute effects, drug residues, abstinence effects, or lasting
changes caused by chronic use continues to be debated.5,6
Animal research suggests an important role for the cannabinoid receptor in
regulating the neural activity critical for memory processing.21-24
Long-term use of cannabis may result in altered functioning of the cannabinoid
receptor and its associated neuromodulator systems.
This study investigated the nature of cognitive impairments associated
with long-term cannabis use employing data collected from a large clinical
trial of chronic users seeking treatment for cannabis dependence. The study
compared 102 cannabis users assessed prior to treatment on carefully selected
neuropsychological tests with 33 nonuser controls. The parameters of cannabis
use that contribute to impairment were examined. It was hypothesized that
performance would deteriorate as the number of years of regular use increased.
A multisite, retrospective, cross-sectional comparison-group design
was used to compare (1) long-term users with a mean of 23.9 years of regular
cannabis use; (2) shorter-term users with a mean of 10.2 years of regular
use; and (3) nonusers of cannabis. Key confounding variables (age, IQ, other
drug use) were controlled through matching or statistical methods. The sample
size required for this study was determined by estimating a 94% chance of
detecting a moderate effect size of 0.5 SD units at a 2-tailed α of
.05.
Recruitment Procedure and Assessment of Drug Use
Sixty-five of the 102 cannabis users were delayed-treatment participants
from the Marijuana Treatment Project, a multisite US study (Seattle, Wash;
Farmington, Conn; and Miami, Fla) conducted between 1997 and 2000 of the effectiveness
of brief treatments for cannabis dependence.25
The remainder were recruited through the Marijuana Treatment Project specifically
for this study. Participants provided written informed consent as approved
by the ethics committees of the participating institutions and were paid $75
for completing the cognitive assessments. Controls (n = 33) were recruited
from the general population through media advertisements at only 1 site. The
controls were told that the researchers were studying the effects of exposure
to drugs and alcohol on cognitive functioning, and that at present only individuals
at the lighter end of the spectrum of drug experience were required. The aim
was to minimize cannabis use among controls while approximating the other
characteristics of the cannabis-using sample. Assessors were not blinded with
regard to group assignment. Self-reported drug and alcohol use were assessed
by the Addiction Severity Index,26 a separate
structured interview, and the Time Line Follow Back procedure.27,28
The Structured Clinical Interview for Diagnostic and Statistical
Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Edition (DSM-IV) Axis I Disorders (SCID)29 assessed cannabis dependence. Duration of regular
(at least twice per month) cannabis use was an averaged composite measure
derived from the Addiction Severity Index, SCID, and the structured interview.
Current frequency of cannabis use was calculated from the Time Line Follow
Back procedure.
Inclusion/Exclusion Criteria
Cannabis users were included if they had used cannabis regularly for
at least 3 years, were currently using at least once a week, were seeking
treatment to assist them to cease or reduce their use of cannabis, and were
willing to participate in the treatment program offered. Participants were
excluded if they had ever had a serious illness or injury that may have affected
the brain, any psychotic disorder, met a current DSM-IV diagnosis of dependence on any other drug or alcohol, or had a poor
command of the English language.
Table 1 provides demographic
information and cannabis use parameters. The user group was split at the median
for duration of cannabis use to enable comparisons of long-term users, shorter-term
users, and controls. No meaningful division of groups could be achieved on
the basis of frequency of cannabis use, which was almost daily for the majority
of the sample. Sex distribution and years of education did not differ between
groups. The majority of users (68.6%) and controls (63.6%) were white. Overall,
users and controls did not differ in age, but long-term users were significantly
older and shorter-term users were significantly younger than controls (P<.001). Premorbid intelligence was estimated by several
methods and averaged: the Wide Range Achievement Test—Revised reading
subtest (WRAT-R READ)30,31; the
North American Adult Reading Test (NAART)32;
and the Barona Index.33 The mean estimated
full-scale IQ (FSIQ) did not differ between the 3 groups based on duration
of cannabis use. The majority of the sample (82.4% long-term, 88.2% shorter-term
users) reported experiencing problems with memory, attention, or concentration,
which they attributed to their use of cannabis.
Cannabis Use, Required Abstinence, and Urinalysis
Users first tried cannabis at a mean age of 15.3 (SD, 2.6) years with
regular use (at least twice a month) commencing at age 17.5 (SD, 3.2) years.
Cannabis had been used on a median 29 of the past 30 days (range, 1-30). Almost
the entire sample (98%) met the DSM-IV criteria for
cannabis dependence. The median amount of cannabis smoked per week was 1 quarter
of an ounce (range, 0.01-2.00 oz) with 2 average-sized joints typically smoked
per day (range, 0.12-20.00). None of these cannabis-use parameters differed
between the long- and shorter-term user groups. Twenty-two controls had either
never tried cannabis or used it 10 or fewer times in their lives and 11 had
used cannabis weekly to monthly while at school or college between 4 and 30
years ago. Controls with a history of cannabis use were excluded from "pure
sample" analyses.
Participants were required to abstain from cannabis for at least 12
hours prior to testing and to provide 2 urine samples (1 the night before
testing, another during the test session). The median self-reported time since
last use of cannabis was 17 hours (range, 7-240 hours); this did not differ
between long- and shorter-term users. At the time of testing, 70% of the sample
reported that they were not experiencing any discomfort after abstaining from
cannabis. Twice as many shorter-term users than long-term users (P = .03) reported mild withdrawal symptoms such as cravings, irritability,
depression, anxiety, sleep, or appetite disturbances. In 78.3% of cases, creatinine-normalized
urinary cannabinoid metabolite (THC-COOH) levels on the day of testing were
less than or equivalent to those from the night before.34-37
Abstinence from cannabis was supported by significant correlations between
the level of normalized urinary cannabinoid metabolite on the day of testing
and the self-reported time since last use (bivariate correlation coefficient
[r], − 0.46; P<.001),
and the quantity used on the last occasion divided by the time since last
use (r, 0.39; P<.001).
The effects of these measures of recent use were examined in relation to test
performance. "Pure sample" analyses excluded users with higher metabolites
in the second urine sample. No cannabinoid metabolites were detected in the
urine of the control participants.
No other drug metabolites were detected in any urine sample. Tobacco
and alcohol use was minimal. Alcohol was consumed on a median of 3.4 and 1.7
days per month among users and controls, respectively. Almost one third of
users and 46.8% of controls drank less than once a month or not at all. Forty-eight
percent of the cannabis users had only tried drugs other than cannabis a few
times or never; 52% had used other drugs socially/recreationally primarily
during high school and college. Past histories of regular drug use included
cocaine (n = 24), amphetamines (n = 11), hallucinogens (n = 17), and sedatives/hypnotics
or minor tranquilizers (n = 7). Current use of other drugs was less than once
a month or not at all for 93.1% of the sample. More than half of the controls
(51.5%) had never tried any other drug and the remainder had only tried other
drugs experimentally. "Pure sample" analyses excluded all participants with
histories of regular or heavy use of alcohol or other drugs.
Neuropsychological Tests and Procedures
Nine neuropsychological tests were administered in the order listed
in Table 2,38-46
along with the 2 tests used to assess premorbid IQ.30-32
A 10-minute rest break was given after the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test
(RAVLT) Recognition test. Tests were administered by trained assistants and
took approximately 2 hours to complete. Quality assurance procedures were
adopted to ensure that procedures were standardized at each site with ongoing
supervision and review of audiotaped assessments by centralized staff throughout
the course of the study.
Each cognitive test was analysed using SPSS version 10.0 (SPSS Institute,
Chicago, Ill) with analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) for normally distributed
variables or nonparametric tests of group differences for skewed data. The
FSIQ and age were included as covariates in analyses where they correlated
with test performance. All participants were initially included in analysis,
with the overall cannabis user sample first compared with the control group
(evaluated at P<.05), followed by comparisons
on the basis of duration of cannabis use (long vs shorter-term users vs controls,
evaluated at P<.01). For 2-way interactions, the
Greenhouse-Geisser method was used to adjust the df
where appropriate and for multiple comparisons, a Bonferroni adjustment controlled
for type I error. Analysis of covariance was repeated on a purer sample that
strictly excluded those participants with either a history of other drug use
or possible recent use of cannabis prior to testing. Semipartial correlations
examined the unique contributions of FSIQ, age, duration of cannabis use,
and recency of cannabis use to the variance in cognitive test performance.
Results from the 9 neuropsychological tests are shown in Table 3a for cannabis users overall, for groups based on duration
of cannabis use, and for controls. Effect sizes are calculated between long-term
users and controls using the SD of the controls.
Cannabis user groups did not differ from controls in the number of items
completed (range, 23-100) but users overall made more errors (P = .03) (range, 0-5). These results suggest that cannabis users are
more likely to sacrifice accuracy for speed.
Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test
Mean words recalled on each trial are depicted in Figure 1. The learning curves of shorter-term users and controls
were similar but long-term users showed a learning curve with a less steep
gradient and long-term users recalled fewer words on every trial. The sum
of words recalled across all trials I through VII inclusive of trial B (referred
to here as RAVLT sum; range, 37-114) correlated significantly and inversely
with the duration of cannabis use after controlling for age and FSIQ (partial r, − 0.23; P = .01). When
analysed by ANCOVA, there was a significant effect of group (F2,127
= 8.36; P<.001) whereby long-term users recalled
significantly fewer words than either shorter-term users (95% confidence interval
[CI] for difference, 3.84-19.18; P = .001) or controls
(95% CI for difference, 2.83-19.93; P = .005) with
no difference between shorter-term users and controls. When all trials were
included in a repeated measures ANCOVA, a significant interaction between
group and trial (F14,889 = 2.84; P = .007)
suggested that long-term users recalled fewer words than shorter-term users
or controls on every trial (P<.05 for each comparison)
except the first, with a trend on trial B (the interference list presented
only once; P = .08).
The proportion of subjects with a very poor learning ability (acquisition
<3 words over 5 trials) was greater among long-term users (13.7%) than
controls (0%) (P = .007) but not shorter-term users
(5.9%). The proportion of long-term users recalling fewer than 10 words on
trial V (27.5%) was more than among shorter-term users (8.5%) or controls
(3.0%) (P = .002). Significantly more long-term users
(23.5%) lost 3 or more words over the 20-minute delay between trials VI and
VII than shorter-term users (4.3%) or controls (3.0%) (P = .003). Long-term users showed a smaller primacy effect in the serial
position curve than either other group (P = .02).
Groups did not differ in the recency effect or in words recalled from the
middle of the list.
Users overall and long-term users recognized fewer words than controls
from list A (overall, P = .03; long-term, P = .01) and list B (overall, P = .01; long-term, P = .04) but long-term users did not differ from shorter-term
users. More than half of the long-term users (55%) had a recognition score
for list A of 12 or less compared with 28% of shorter-term users and 21% of
controls (P = .002). Long-term users misassigned
more words (median, 2) than shorter-term users and controls (each median,
0) (P<.001). A greater proportion of long-term
users (13.7%) compared with shorter-term users (6.4%) and controls (0%) actually
identified fewer words on recognition than they had just prior during recall
on trial VII (P = .02). Long-term users' performance
was significantly poorer than published norms47
for the general population on most measures from the RAVLT.
Cannabis users did not differ significantly from controls after inclusion
of covariates in any condition or on interference scores. While there were
no performance differences between Color-Word (CW) and Color-Read (CR) in
the control group, performance on CR was, however, poorer than on CW in both
long (P<.001) and shorter-term users (P = .03). Color-Read was the additional interference condition designed
to increase demands on executive function.43
There was an inverse relationship between duration of cannabis use and number
of items completed on CR (partial r, − 0.27; P = .003) and CW (partial r, −
0.27; P = .004) after controlling for age and FSIQ.
These results suggest that cannabis users are vulnerable to task complexity
with increasing demands creating more sources of interference that adversely
affect performance.
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test
There were no significant group differences on any Wisconsin Card Sorting
Test (WCST) measure but a trend on one: long-term users failed to maintain
the set more often than shorter-term users (P = .05)
or controls (P = .07). Research suggests that this
measure best represents attentional dysfunction.39
There was no evidence of impaired performance with increasing years of cannabis
use after controlling for covariates.
Alphabet Task and Omitted Numbers
Groups did not differ in the time taken to complete any trial of the
Alphabet Task or in the number of items correct in the Omitted Numbers task.
The log time to complete the alternating trial of the Alphabet Task increased
as a function of duration of cannabis use (partial r,
0.26; P = .006), as did the square root difference
between times taken to complete the alternating and loud trials, an index
of interference and lack of flexibility (partial r,
0.26; P = .006).
Cannabis users differed from controls (P<.001)
in Time Estimation Task A where they estimated the time taken to complete
the preceding (Omitted Numbers) task. Both long- and shorter-term users underestimated
the time by about one third of the actual time taken (64.4 seconds) and differed
significantly from controls (P = .01 and P<.001,
respectively). Groups did not differ in the simple and brief warned passive
Time Estimation Task B or Time Production, where they could use strategies
such as counting. Time estimation measures did not correlate with duration
of cannabis use.
Auditory Consonant Trigrams
Long-term users recalled significantly fewer items than shorter-term
users (P = .007), controls (P
= .002), and published norms48 on only the
9-second delay condition. The number of items recalled did not correlate with
duration of cannabis use. In the general population, the greater the delay
interval the worse the performance. In cannabis users, this general pattern
was apparent, though there was greater interference at the shorter-delay interval
than would be expected.
Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test
Long-term users had slower processing rates than shorter-term users
on trial 1 (P = .007), with trends on trial 2 (P = .03) and the total processing rate across all trials
(P = .02). Group differences on all other measures
failed to reach significance but the performance of the long-term users was
poorer in comparison with one set of norms49
but not another.50
Pure Effects Attributable to Cannabis Use and Effects of Recent vs
Chronic Use
Excluding all participants with histories of regular other drug or alcohol
use, dependence or treatment, and controls with any history of regular cannabis
use within the past 20 years reduced the sample to 27 long-term users, 33
shorter-term users, and 26 controls. Despite the reduction in power to detect
differences between groups, there remained a significant difference with α
= .05 between long-term users and controls on RAVLTsum (P = .03), recognition of lists A (P = .004)
and B (P = .01), and between users overall and controls
on the unwarned Time Estimation task (P = .02). These
results support the hypothesis that impaired memory function and time estimation
are specific to chronic use of cannabis.
In a separate analysis, exclusion of users whose urinary cannabinoid
metabolite levels exceeded those from the night before testing by 50 ng/mg
or more (n = 18) still resulted in significant differences between long- and
shorter-term users, and long-term users and controls on RAVLT sum (P = .002 and P = .002, respectively), on recognition
of lists A (P = .005 and P
= .006) and B (P = .01 and P<.001),
on the 9-second delay of the Auditory Consonant Trigrams test (P = .02 and P = .03), and users still differed
from controls on time estimation (P = .005). When
the sample was split at the median for time since last use or level of urinary
cannabinoid metabolite on the day of testing and analyzed by ANCOVA, there
were no differences on any measure between those who had used cannabis within
the past 17 hours and those who had used cannabis 17 or more hours ago, or
those with high vs low levels of urinary metabolites and no interactions with
duration of cannabis use. Including measures of recent use as covariates in
ANCOVA did not change the significance of differences between long- and shorter-term
users. These results support the hypothesis that impaired performance is not
a consequence of recent use prior to testing or the extent of cannabinoid
residues present.
To explore further the influences of duration of cannabis use and recency
of use, semipartial correlations were calculated using the following predictors:
FSIQ, age, duration of cannabis use, and hours since last use of cannabis.
As shown in Table 4, the unique
contribution of duration of cannabis use to the variance of each test variable
was superior or at least equivalent to that of recency of use in all 6 test
variables that had significant contributions from at least 1 cannabis use
parameter. Recent use contributed only to performance on the memory tests.
The fact that a minority of the sample, primarily shorter-term users, reported
experiencing mild withdrawal symptoms, yet shorter-term users' performance
was not impaired, supports the interpretation of the cognitive impairments
observed as a long-term consequence of cannabis use and not a manifestation
of overtly experienced withdrawal.
The results of this study have confirmed and extended previous findings
of cognitive impairments among chronic heavy cannabis users. Long-term users
with a mean 24 years of regular cannabis use performed significantly less
well on tests of memory and attention than nonuser controls and shorter-term
users with a mean of 10 years' use. The greatest impairment on almost every
measure was from the RAVLT, indicating a generalized memory deficit with impaired
learning, retention, and retrieval. Long-term users recalled 2.5 fewer words
than controls on the delayed recall trial where 49% of the long-term users'
scores were more than 1 SD, and 21.6% were more than 2 SDs, below the control
mean and normative data.47 A large proportion
of long-term users' recognition scores were more than 1 SD (51%) or 2 SDs
(31.4%) below the control mean and norms.47
Effect sizes for measures that differed significantly between long-term users
and controls ranged from 0.56 to 1.29 across all tests, indicating moderate
to large effects.
These results do not indicate a severe memory problem but could nevertheless
translate into clinically significant cognitive impairment and could impact
functioning in daily life. There were significant differences between long-term
users and controls on 6 of the 9 tests administered and performance on 4 tests
worsened as a function of increasing years of cannabis use. Despite this and
a range of up to 17 years of cannabis use in the shorter-term user group,
they differed significantly from controls only on time estimation.
Altered brain metabolism in shorter-term users may be detected with
sensitive techniques, such as functional magnetic resonance imaging and positron
emission tomography, but the clinical significance of such changes remains
obscure. The strength of this study is in its assessment of overtly relevant
cognitive processes; our results suggest that shorter-term cannabis users
are not impaired to an extent that would interfere with cognitive functioning
in their daily lives. The fact that the frequency of use was near daily among
long- and shorter-term users suggests that the duration of cannabis use is
a more salient contributor to the development of cognitive impairment than
quantity or frequency of use.
While most cannabis users cease using in their mid-20s to late 20s,
approximately 20% continue to use through their 30s and beyond.2
This is the first study to our knowledge of a relatively large sample of long-term
entrenched cannabis users seeking treatment. Concern about perceived cognitive
impairment was one of many problems associated with cannabis use that led
the users in this study to seek treatment. This concern is unlikely to have
biased the results of this study since a slightly higher proportion of shorter-term
vs long-term users reported experiencing cognitive problems, yet shorter-term
users mostly did not differ from controls on the cognitive tests. Nevertheless,
it is possible that long-term cannabis users in the community who are not
seeking treatment may not experience impairments to the same degree as those
assessed in this study.
While acknowledging the limitations of retrospective designs, if carefully
controlled and analyzed, this approach is the most efficient way to evaluate
the long-term cognitive effects of cannabis, given the costs and logistical
difficulties in using prospective research designs. The matching of groups
on measures of premorbid intellectual functioning that are resilient to brain
damage, together with the observed relationships between duration of cannabis
use and test performance, support the assumption that the cognitive impairments
observed in the long-term users were not preexisting but developed as a result
of their prolonged use of cannabis. Impairment appeared unrelated to withdrawal
phenomena. The cognitive functions assessed in this study are dependent on
the intact functioning of the hippocampus, prefrontal cortex, and cerebellum,39,51-55
which are dense with cannabinoid receptors.56
The effects that exogenous cannabinoids exert on the cannabinoid receptor
system and the role of endogenous cannabinoids as suggested by animal research6,21-24
provide a credible neurophysiological explanation for the development of cognitive
impairments as the result of hypothesized long-term changes occurring over
many years of exposure to the drug.
In conclusion, our results confirm that cognitive impairments develop
as a result of prolonged cannabis use, they endure beyond the period of acute
intoxication, and they worsen with increasing years of use. Impairments develop
gradually but may only become clinically significant and detectable by standard
neuropsychological tests after 1 to 2 decades of cannabis use. Nevertheless,
altered brain function with subtle impairment has been shown to manifest earlier.6,8,9,11,17,18
It is also likely that impairments would be greater among comorbid substance-dependent
persons. The risk to most medical cannabis users is likely to be small, as
long as they are not maintained at high doses for many years. For habitual
users, the kinds of impairments observed in this study have the potential
to impact academic achievements, occupational proficiency, interpersonal relationships,
and daily functioning. The extent to which these cognitive impairments may
recover following cessation or reduction of cannabis use will be addressed
in a follow-up of this sample subsequent to treatment for cannabis dependence.
1.Watson SJ, Benson JA, Joy JE. Marijuana and medicine: assessing the science base: a summary of the
1999 Institute of Medicine report.
Arch Gen Psychiatry.2000;57:547-552.Google Scholar 2.Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration (SAMHSA). National Household Survey on Drug Abuse Series: H-6:
Preliminary Results from the 1997 National Household Survey on Drug Abuse. Rockville, Md: Office on Applied Studies; 1998. DHHS publication
SMA 98-3251.
3.Hall W, Babor TF. Cannabis use and public health: assessing the burden.
Addiction.2000;95:485-490.Google Scholar 4.Voelker R. "Decent research and closure" needed on medical marijuana, says head
of NIH panel.
JAMA.1997;278:802.Google Scholar 5.Pope HG, Gruber AJ, Yurgelun-Todd D. The residual neuropsychological effects of cannabis: the current status
of research.
Drug Alcohol Depend.1995;38:25-34.Google Scholar 6.Solowij N. Cannabis and Cognitive Functioning. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press; 1998.
7.Fried PA, Watkinson B, Gray R. Differential effects on cognitive functioning in 9- to 12-year olds
prenatally exposed to cigarettes and marihuana.
Neurotoxicol Teratol.1998;20:293-306.Google Scholar 8.Solowij N, Michie PT, Fox AM. Effects of long term cannabis use on selective attention: an event-related
potential study.
Pharmacol Biochem Behav.1991;40:683-688.Google Scholar 9.Solowij N, Michie PT, Fox AM. Differential impairments of selective attention due to frequency and
duration of cannabis use.
Biol Psychiatry.1995;37:731-739.Google Scholar 10.Solowij N. Do cognitive impairments recover following cessation of cannabis use?
Life Sci.1995;56:2119-2126.Google Scholar 11.Pope HG, Yurgelun-Todd D. The residual cognitive effects of heavy marijuana use in college students.
JAMA.1996;275:521-527.Google Scholar 12.Pope HG, Jacobs A, Mialet JP, Yurgelun-Todd D, Gruber S. Evidence for a sex-specific residual effect of cannabis on visuospatial
memory.
Psychother Psychosom.1997;66:179-184.Google Scholar 13.Fletcher JM, Page JB, Francis DJ.
et al. Cognitive correlates of long-term cannabis use in Costa Rican men.
Arch Gen Psychiatry.1996;53:1051-1057.Google Scholar 14.Elwan O, Hassan AAH, Naseer MA.
et al. Brain aging in a sample of normal Egyptians: cognition, education,
addiction and smoking.
J Neurol Sci.1997;148:79-86.Google Scholar 15.Solowij N, Grenyer BFS, Peters R, Chesher G. Long term cannabis use impairs memory processes and frontal lobe function. In: 1997 Symposium on the Cannabinoids.
Burlington, Vt: International Cannabinoid Research Society; 1997:84.
16.Loeber RT, Yurgelun-Todd DA. Human neuroimaging of acute and chronic marijuana use: implications
for frontocerebellar dysfunction.
Hum Psychopharmacol Clin Exp.1999;14:291-301.Google Scholar 17.Block RI, O'Leary DS, Hichwa RD.
et al. Cerebellar hypoactivity in frequent marijuana users.
Neuroreport.2000;11:749-753.Google Scholar 18.Block RI, O'Leary DS, Hichwa RD.
et al. Effects of frequent marijuana use on memory-related regional cerebral
blood flow.
Pharmacol Biochem Behav.In press.Google Scholar 19.Block RI, O'Leary DS, Augustinack JC.
et al. Effects of frequent marijuana use on attention-related regional cerebral
blood flow.
Abstr Soc Neurosci.2000;26:2080.Google Scholar 20.Lyketsos CG, Garrett E, Liang K-Y, Anthony JC. Cannabis use and cognitive decline in persons under 65 years of age.
Am J Epidemiol.1999;149:794-800.Google Scholar 21.Hampson RE, Deadwyler SA. Cannabinoids, hippocampal function and memory.
Life Sci.1999;65:715-723.Google Scholar 22.Hampson RE, Deadwyler SA. Cannabinoids reveal the necessity of hippocampal neural encoding for
short-term memory in rats.
J Neurosci.2000;20:8932-8942.Google Scholar 23.Reibaud M, Obinu MC, Ledent C.
et al. Enhancement of memory in cannabinoid CB1 receptor knock-out mice.
Eur J Pharmacol.1999;379:R1-R2.Google Scholar 24.Bohme GA, Laville M, Ledent C, Parmentier M, Imperato A. Enhanced long-term potentiation in mice lacking cannabinoid CB1 receptors.
Neuroscience.2000;95:5-7.Google Scholar 25.Marijuana Treatment Project Research Group. Treating marijuana dependence: findings from a multi-site study.
Arch Gen Psychiatry.In press.Google Scholar 26.McLellan AT, Kushner H, Metzger D.
et al. The Fifth Edition of the Addiction Severity Index.
J Subst Abuse Treat.1992;9:199-213.Google Scholar 27.Sobell LC, Sobell MB. Timeline follow-back: a technique for assessing self reported alcohol
consumption. In: Litten RZ, Allen JP, eds. Measuring Alcohol
Consumption: Psychological and Biochemical Methods. Totowa, NJ: Humana
Press; 1992.
28.Miller WR. Form 90: A Structured Assessment Interview for Drinking
and Related Behaviors: Test Manual: Project MATCH Monograph Series, Vol. 5. Bethesda, Md: National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism;
1996. NIH Publication 96-4004.
29.First MB, Gibbon M, Spitzer RL, Williams JB. User's Guide for the Structured Clinical Interview
for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders—Research Version. New York, NY: Biometrics Research Department, New York State Psychiatric
Institute; 1996.
30.Jastak S, Wilkinson G. The Wide Range Achievement Test: Manual of Instructions. Wilmington, Del: Jastak Associates; 1984.
31.Kareken DA, Gur RC, Saykin AJ. Reading on the Wide Range Achievement Test-Revised and parental education
as predictors of IQ: comparison with the Barona formula.
Arch Clin Neuropsychol.1995;10:147-157.Google Scholar 32.Blair JR, Spreen O. Predicting premorbid IQ: a revision of the National Adult Reading Test.
Clin Neuropsychol.1989;3:129-136.Google Scholar 33.Barona A, Reynolds CR, Chastain R. A demographically based index of pre-morbid intelligence for the WAIS-R.
J Consult Clin Psychol.1984;52:885-887.Google Scholar 34.Bell R, Taylor EH, Ackerman B, Pappas AA. Interpretation of urine quantitative 11-nor-delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol-9-carboxylic
acid to determine abstinence from marijuana smoking.
J Toxicol Clin Toxicol.1989;27:109-115.Google Scholar 35.Dackis CA, Pottash ALC, Annitto W, Gold MS. Persistence of urinary marijuana levels after supervised abstinence.
Am J Psychiatry.1982;139:1196-1198.Google Scholar 36.Ellis GM, Mann MA, Judson BA, Schramm NT, Tashchian A. Excretion patterns of cannabinoid metabolites after last use in a group
of chronic users.
Clin Pharmacol Ther.1985;38:572-578.Google Scholar 37.Fraser AD, Worth D. Urinary excretion profiles of 11-nor-9-carboxy-delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol:
a delta-9-THCCOOH to creatinine ratio study.
J Anal Toxicol.1999;23:531-534.Google Scholar 38.Baddeley A, Emslie H, Nimmo Smith I. The Speed and Capacity of Language-Processing Test
Manual. Bury St Edmonds, England: Thames Valley Test Co; 1992.
39.Lezak MD. Neuropsychological Assessment. 3rd ed. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 1995.
40.Rey A. L'examen clinique en psychologie. Paris, France: Presse Universitaire de France; 1964.
41.Spreen O, Strauss E. A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests: Administration,
Norms and Commentary. 2nd ed. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 1998.
42.Golden CJ. Stroop Color and Word Test: A Manual for Clinical
and Experimental Uses. Wood Dale, Ill: Stoelting; 1978.
43.Bohnen N, Jolles J, Twijnstra A. Modification of the Stroop Color Word Test improves differentiation
between patients with mild head injury and matched controls.
Clin Neuropsychol.1992;6:178-184.Google Scholar 44. The Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: Computer Version. Alpharetta, Ga: Psychological Assessment Resources Inc, CyberMetrics
Testing Software Services; 1989.
45.Stuss DT, Stethem LL, Poirier CA. Comparison of three tests of attention and rapid information processing
across six age groups.
Clin Neuropsychol.1987;1:139-152.Google Scholar 46.Levin HS, Mattis S, Ruff RM.
et al. Neurobehavioral outcome following minor head injury: a three-center
study.
J Neurosurg.1987;66:234-243.Google Scholar 47.Geffen G, Moar KJ, O'Hanlon AP, Clark CR, Geffen LB. Performance measures of 16- to 86-year old males and females on the
Auditory Verbal Learning Test.
Clin Neuropsychol.1990;4:45-63.Google Scholar 48.Stuss DT, Stetham L, Pelchat G. Three tests of attention and rapid information processing: an extension.
Clin Neuropsychol.1988;2:246-250.Google Scholar 49.Roman DD, Edwall GE, Buchanan RJ, Patton JH. Extended norms for the Paced Auditory Serial Addition Task.
Clin Neuropsychol.1991;5:33-40.Google Scholar 50.Brittain JL, La Marche JA, Reeder KP, Roth DL, Boll TJ. Effects of age and IQ on Paced Auditory Serial Addition Task (PASAT)
performance.
Clin Neuropsychol.1991;5:163-175.Google Scholar 51.Levy R, Goldman-Rakic PS. Segregation of working memory functions within the dorsolateral prefrontal
cortex.
Exp Brain Res.2000;133:23-32.Google Scholar 52.Herman BP, Seidenberg M, Wyler A.
et al. The effects of human hippocampal resection on the serial position curve.
Cortex.1996;32:323-334.Google Scholar 53.Middleton FA, Strick PL. Anatomical evidence for cerebellar and basal ganglia involvement in
higher cognitive function.
Science.1994;266:458-461.Google Scholar 55.Schmahmann JD. The Cerebellum and Cognition. San Diego, Calif: Academic Press; 1997.
56.Herkenham M, Lynn AB, Little MD.
et al. Cannabinoid receptor localization in brain.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A.1990;87:1932-1936.Google Scholar