Context Maternal use of cocaine during pregnancy remains a significant public
health problem, particularly in urban areas of the United States and among
women of low socioeconomic status. Few longitudinal studies have examined
cocaine-exposed infants, however, and findings are contradictory because of
methodologic limitations.
Objective To assess the effects of prenatal cocaine exposure on child developmental
outcomes.
Design Longitudinal, prospective, masked, comparison birth cohort study with
recruitment in 1994-1996.
Setting Obstetric unit of a large US urban teaching hospital.
Participants Four hundred fifteen consecutively enrolled infants (218 cocaine-exposed
and 197 unexposed) identified from a high-risk, low–socioeconomic status,
primarily black (80%) population screened through clinical interview and urine
and meconium samples for drug use. The retention rate was 94% at 2 years of
age.
Main Outcome Measures The Bayley Mental and Motor Scales of Infant Development, assessed at
6.5, 12, and 24 months of corrected age.
Results Controlled for confounding variables, cocaine exposure had significant
effects on cognitive development, accounting for a 6-point deficit in Bayley
Mental and Motor Scales of Infant Development scores at 2 years, with cocaine-exposed
children twice as likely to have significant delay (mental development index
<80) (odds ratio, 1.98; 95% confidence interval, 1.21-3.24; P = .006). For motor outcomes, there were no significant cocaine effects.
Conclusions Cocaine-exposed children had significant cognitive deficits and a doubling
of the rate of developmental delay during the first 2 years of life. Because
2-year outcomes are predictive of later cognitive outcomes, it is possible
that these children will continue to have learning difficulties at school
age.
Maternal use of cocaine during pregnancy remains a significant and enduring
public health problem, particularly in urban areas of the United States and
among women of low socioeconomic status.1 An
estimated 1 million children have been born after fetal cocaine exposure since
the mid-1980s, when the so-called crack epidemic emerged with the availability
of a cheap, potent, smokable form of cocaine.2
Cocaine has effects on monoaminergic neurotransmitter systems important for
the development of neuronal circuitry and human learning.3-5
A growing body of research documents relationships between prenatal cocaine
exposure and prematurity,6,7 low
birth weight,8 microcephaly,9,10
and newborn behavioral abnormalities,11,12
which has raised concerns regarding long-term cognitive and developmental
outcomes.
There are few longitudinal studies of cocaine-exposed infants, however,
and their findings are contradictory.13 While
some studies have found generalized developmental delays in cocaine-exposed
infants,14-16
others have not demonstrated differences.17
Still other studies, including a recent meta-analysis18,
show only subtle cognitive effects or find deficits only when more specific
areas of functioning are measured.18-20
These studies are inconclusive for a number of reasons. Most had high rates
of attrition ranging from 30% to 55%,14-17
often combined with small sample sizes.15,18,20
High attrition and small sample sizes are especially problematic in assessing
teratogenic effects of fetal cocaine exposure on cognitive outcomes. Cocaine-exposed
infants experience a large number of negative environmental factors known
to be related to poorer child developmental outcomes that are also likely
to differentially affect subject recruitment and retention.21
These confounding variables include minority race, low socioeconomic status,
poor prenatal care, low maternal education and IQ, greater maternal psychological
distress, a less stimulating home environment, larger family, higher risk
for out-of-home placement, and maternal use of drugs in addition to cocaine,
especially alcohol, marijuana, and tobacco.
The effects of such factors must be considered before poorer child outcomes
are attributed to fetal cocaine exposure22;
thus, adequate sample sizes must be used. Some studies enrolled only full-term
infants or only infants who received prenatal care, perhaps restricting the
samples to the least-affected infants. Moreover, in most studies, biological
measures of severity of exposure, important in validating a dose-response
relationship, have been lacking. Such validation is important when effects
of illegal drugs such as cocaine are investigated because maternal self-report
about illegal drug use can be less than candid, and such measures reflect
unreliable so-called street dosages of drugs. Biological measures to establish
severity of exposure more reliably8,23
are also important because some negative effects of cocaine have been demonstrated
only with heavy exposure.12,15,24,25
Finally, with the exception of one study15
that found effects of heavy cocaine exposure on cognitive development at 18
months, all published studies have used the old (1969) version of the Bayley
Scales of Infant Development to assess developmental outcomes.26
These scales had outdated normatives that might be insensitive to drug effects.
This study followed a large cohort of cocaine-exposed infants recruited
from a sample prospectively screened at birth at a large urban county hospital.
They were compared with unexposed infants from the same population on standardized
normative measures of cognitive and motor development until 2 years of age.
Both maternal self-report measures and biological assays of cocaine and other
drug metabolites in maternal urine and infant meconium were used to classify
infants and measure severity of fetal exposure.
Mothers and infants were recruited between 1994 and 1996 at a large
urban county teaching hospital and had been identified from a high-risk population
screened for drug use. Urine samples were obtained immediately before or after
labor and delivery from all women who received no prenatal care, appeared
to be intoxicated or taking drugs, had a history of involvement with the Department
of Human Services in previous pregnancies, or either admitted that they were
using drugs or appeared to be at high risk for drug use after interview by
hospital staff. Urine was analyzed for the presence of cocaine metabolites,
cannabinoids, opiates, phencyclidine (PCP), and amphetamines by the Syva Emit
method (Syva Company, Palo Alto, Calif). The specificity for benzoylecgonine
was 99% at 0.3 mg/mL. Follow-up gas chromatography analyses were performed.
Infants of screened mothers also had the following meconium drug analyses
for cocaine and its metabolites: benzoylecgonine, meta-hydroxybenzoylecgonine,
cocaethylene, cannabinoids, opiates, PCP, amphetamines, and benzodiazepines.27,28 Meconium was collected in the hospital
from infants' diapers and was scraped from the diaper with a wooden spatula
and placed in a plastic container. Specimens from multiple diapers of the
same newborn were accumulated and kept refrigerated. The entire sample was
then stirred for 5 minutes to ensure homogeneity. Meconium analysis was conducted
with gas chromatography–mass spectrometry. Screening assays were conducted
with fluorescence polarization immunoassay (United States Drug Testing Laboratories
Inc, Des Plaines, Ill). Cutoff levels for drugs of interest were as follows:
cocaine and metabolites, 25 ng/g; opiates, 25 ng/g; amphetamines, 100 ng/g;
PCP, 25 ng/g; and tetrahydrocannabinol, 25 ng/g. Confirmatory assays were
conducted with gas chromatography–mass spectrometry operated in electron-impact
selected-ion monitoring mode.
Internal validity for use of concentrations of cocaine metabolites to
assess severity of prenatal exposure has been demonstrated in prior studies,23 which found reasonable correlations between maternal
self-report and meconium concentrations of benzoylecgonine (0.57), meta-hydroxybenzoylecgonine
(0.51), cocaine (0.46), and cocaethylene (0.29).23
External validity can also be derived from these drugs' relationships to fetal
and infant outcomes in this sample.10,12,25
Although maternal urine and infant meconium were collected systematically,
infant urine specimens were not. However, when available, they were analyzed
like maternal urine specimens.
A nurse recruiter approached all screened women shortly before or after
infant birth. Six hundred forty-seven mothers and their infants were identified,
of whom 54 were excluded (20 cocaine positive and 34 cocaine negative). Reasons
for exclusion included no meconium (15), Down syndrome (2), maternal psychiatric
history (16), primary heroin use (2), being HIV positive (5), maternal low
IQ (1), fetal alcohol syndrome (1), maternal age younger than 19 years (2),
infant illness (3), maternal chronic illness (4), and other (3). One hundred
fifty-five mothers refused to participate (49 cocaine positive and 106 cocaine
negative), and 23 (9 cocaine positive and 14 cocaine negative) did not come
to the enrollment visit. Mothers who refused to participate were more likely
not to be cocaine users (P<.001) and to be younger
(P = .04). There were no differences in race, sex,
or infant birth characteristics. Therefore, 415 women and their infants enrolled
in the study (218 positive and 197 negative). Cocaine-exposed infants were
identified by a positive response on any of the following measures: infant
meconium, urine, or maternal urine positive for cocaine; maternal report to
hospital staff; or maternal self-report during clinical interview (see below).
For the majority of the control subjects, all of these indicators were negative;
for 11 subjects (6%), meconium was unavailable, but all other screening and
follow-up indicated no evidence of infant exposure.
Caregivers were approached as soon as possible after the infants were
born, at which time the caregiver was interviewed regarding drug use. For
infants in nonmaternal care, biological mothers were interviewed separately.
An adaptation of the Maternal Post-Partum Interview29
quantified maternal drug use. For the month before pregnancy and during each
trimester, mothers recalled frequency and amount of drug use. For tobacco,
they noted the number of cigarettes smoked; for marijuana, the number of marijuana
cigarettes smoked; for alcohol, the number of drinks of beer, wine, or hard
liquor each day, with each drink equivalent to 0.5 mL of absolute alcohol;
and for cocaine, the number of so-called rocks consumed and amount of money
spent daily. For each drug, frequency of use was recorded on a Likert-type
scale ranging from 0 (not at all) to 7 (daily use), which was converted to
reflect the average number of days a drug was used each week. The frequency
was multiplied by the amount used daily to compute a severity score for the
month before pregnancy and for each trimester. This score was then averaged
for a total score for the prenatal exposure for each drug.
At the neonatal visit, maternal education level and socioeconomic status
were determined. Mothers were given the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test—Revised,30 the block design and picture completion subscales
of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scales—Revised,31
and the Brief Symptom Inventory32 to obtain
measures of maternal vocabulary, nonverbal intelligence, and psychological
distress, respectively. The Brief Symptom Inventory yields a measure of overall
distress, the Global Severity Index, which was used in data analyses.
Demographic and medical characteristics taken from hospital records
included maternal race, age, parity, number of prenatal care visits, type
of medical insurance, infant gestational age, birth weight, length, head circumference,
and Apgar scores. The Hobel Neonatal Risk score33
was computed to obtain a measure of neonatal risk condition.
At 6.5, 12, and 24 months (corrected ages), infants were tested at the
research laboratory and given the Bayley Scales of Infant Development,34 widely used standardized assessments of infant development.
The scales yield a Mental Development Index, a standard score reflecting memory,
language, and problem-solving abilities. The Psychomotor Development Index
measures gross and fine motor control and coordination. Normative data from
the scales yield a mean of 100 and SD of 15. All assessors were master's level
psychology assistants or the equivalent and masked to infant drug exposure.
Interrater reliabilities (agreement percentage) for examiners for the scales
averaged 93% (range, 77%-100%) for the Mental Development Index and 94% (range,
79%-100%) for the Psychomotor Development Index.
At each visit, the infant's placement (with biological mother or relative
or foster parent) was noted, and the current caregiver data were updated to
provide concurrent measures of psychological distress and tobacco, alcohol,
marijuana, and cocaine use. If the infant was placed with a new caregiver,
intellectual measures were also updated. At the 2-year visit, for half the
sample, the Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment test (HOME)35 was administered to the caregiver in an interview
format as a measure of the quality of the caregiving environment. There were
no significant differences on demographic, medical, or drug-use characteristics
between children who were given the HOME and those who were not (all P>.10). By exposure group, cocaine users who received the
HOME were older (30.4 vs 28.7 years; P = .02). For
nonusers, those administered the HOME had more prenatal care visits (9.5 vs
7.6; P = .01), and their children had lower Apgar
scores at 5 minutes (8.7 vs 8.9; P = .02).
This study was approved by the institutional review boards of the participating
hospitals, and maternal written informed consent was obtained. All caregivers
were given $35 for participation at each visit, with transportation costs
and lunch provided.
Sample size, estimated for the outcomes of the Mental Development Index
and Psychomotor Index, was calculated according to our pilot data and with
the 2-sample t test. To detect a 10% difference,
a sample size of 98 subjects per group was needed, assuming 80% power and
a significance level of .01. However, we recruited additional subjects to
account for attrition.
Before analysis, drug self-report measures, the Global Severity Index,
and meconium quantification variables, all positively skewed, were normalized
by log x + 1 transformation. Means and SDs are reported in terms of the original
distribution, with transformations used in analyses.
Groups were compared on demographic variables, frequency and severity
of drug use, and infant birth outcomes by using t
tests for continuous data and χ2 analyses for categorical variables.
Outcomes over time were compared by using mixed model analyses (SAS PROC MIXED
version 8.2; SAS Institute Inc, Cary, NC), first considering only cocaine
status and then, if significant, by controlling for appropriate confounders.
Spearman rank order correlations were used to assess the relationship
of severity of prenatal drug exposure measures to infant outcomes at each
age. Confounding variables were entered into the MIXED model stepwise if they
were significantly different between exposed and unexposed groups and related
to the outcome at P<.2. The order of entry was
designed to account for demographic, environmental, and medical factors before
drug exposure factors, consistent with a teratologic model, and to reduce
the number of correlated variables in the statistical model. Demographic and
prenatal factors were considered first, followed by infant caregiving and
environmental variables and drug exposure variables in the following order:
maternal age; parity; number of prenatal care visits; maternal years of education;
marital status; socioeconomic status; biological and current caregiver (time
dependent) Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test—Revised, Wechsler Adult Intelligence
Scale—Revised block design, and picture completion scores; nonmaternal
care status; biological maternal and current caregiver (time dependent) psychological
distress; and prenatal and current caregiver measures of cigarette, alcohol,
and marijuana exposure. Because many of the confounding variables were expected
to be correlated, they were entered stepwise into the model separately and
retained only if they added significantly to the prediction of outcome. Global
Severity Index was tested last because maternal psychological distress can
be a precipitant and an effect of cocaine use.
Because infant birth characteristics or medical condition can be an
effect of cocaine exposure, birth outcomes that differed between groups were
assessed as possible mediating variables by entering them into the model after
all other variables if there was a significant group effect.36
Infant race and sex, which did not differ between exposure groups, were considered
moderator variables, with their effects tested through interaction terms.
Maternal characteristics are listed in Table 1a. The majority of both groups were black, had a high school
education or lower, had low income, and were unemployed. For all trimesters,
cocaine-using women used alcohol, marijuana, and tobacco more frequently and
copiously than nonusers. Cocaine users reported a mean (SD) of 23.3 (44.0)
rocks of cocaine used weekly throughout pregnancy, while the median use was
7.9 units. The highest individual use throughout pregnancy in the sample was
80 cigarettes daily, 30 joints weekly, 111 drinks weekly, and 386 rocks of
cocaine weekly. More detailed measures reflecting daily use by trimester have
been reported.10 The means (ranges) of the
concentration of cocaine metabolites in meconium were as follows: cocaine,
142 (0-3112) ng/g; cocaethylene, 18 (0-419) ng/g; benzoylecgonine, 552 (0-9998)
ng/g; and meta-hydroxybenzoylecgonine, 264 (0-9998) ng/g. Cocaine-using women
were older, had more children, and were less likely to have had prenatal care.
They were less likely to be married and had a less advanced vocabulary, lower
block design and picture completion scores, and higher psychological distress
scores.
Infant birth characteristics are listed in Table 2. Cocaine-exposed infants had a lower gestational age, birth
weight, head circumference, and length than unexposed infants. In the exposed
group, there were more infants who were preterm, had a low birth weight, and
were small for gestational age. More exposed infants were in nonmaternal care
just after birth (34% vs 1.5%; P<.001), at 1 year
(46% vs 4%; P<.001), and at 2 years of age (50%
vs 4%; P<.001). Exposed infants averaged 0.61
nonmaternal care placements by 2 years vs 0.09 for unexposed infants (P<.001). There were no differences in HOME scores between
the exposed and unexposed groups at 2 years.
From birth to 2 years, there were 11 deaths, 8 in cocaine-positive children
and 3 in cocaine-negative children (χ2 = 1.9; P = .17). Causes of death for the cocaine-exposed group were sudden
infant death syndrome (4), cardiopulmonary arrest (1), pneumonia (1), accidental
asphyxia (1), and respiratory distress syndrome (1). Causes of death for the
unexposed children were sudden infant death syndrome (2) and respiratory distress
syndrome (1). Retention rates were 84% (339) at 6.5 months, 90% (364) at 12
months, and 94% (379) at 2 years, with 100% (404) of survivors having at least
1 follow-up visit.
To assess the effects of cocaine while controlling for confounding variables
and to test for mediational effects, drug, demographic, and birth variables
that related to cognitive and motor outcome within each age were examined
(Table 3, Table 4, and Table 5).
Of these, the following variables that differed by exposure group were significant
predictors of longitudinal outcome (at P = .20) in
the mixed model for Mental Development Index and thus were entered as covariates:
current caregiver picture completion score (P<.09)
and block design score (P = .16), birth mother picture
completion score (P<.08) and block design score
(P = .13), birth-mother parity (P = .14), and educational level (P = .05).
In the stepwise model, current caregiver and birth mother picture completion
scores and birth mother block design score were no longer significant once
the current caregiver block design was controlled, and for the parsimony of
the model, they were removed from the analysis. Similarly, once the HOME score
was included in the model, the number of placements was no longer significant.
Birth mother parity and educational level added significantly to the model,
but after these demographic factors were considered, the only drug exposure
to predict outcome was cocaine. When the effects of these variables were controlled
in the mixed model, cocaine exposure remained a significant predictor of cognitive
outcome (Table 6, model 1).
Cocaine-exposed children performed more poorly on the Bayley Mental
Scale than unexposed children after adjustment for significant confounding
variables (Table 7). Although
Mental Development Index scores for both groups decreased over time, children
prenatally exposed to cocaine had scores that decreased faster. From 6.5 to
24 months, the average Mental Development Index score for infants exposed
to cocaine declined by 14 points (SE, 1.1); for those not exposed to cocaine,
by 9 points (SE, 1.1). Using a contrast of the means to test for a difference
in the change from 6.5 to 24 months between the groups yielded a significant
result (|Δ|, 4.44; SE, 1.52; t345
= 2.92; P = .004). At 2 years, the adjusted means
difference of 6 points yielded a moderate effect size of 0.44.
These effects remained significant and had the same magnitude when race,
sex, and HOME score at 2 years were controlled in the model (Table 6, model 2). Further, the effects of cocaine remained significant
when all birth outcomes, Apgar scores (P = .44),
Hobel risk (P<.001), and maternal Global Severity
Index (P = .91) scores were controlled, indicating that birth outcomes were
not mediators of cocaine's effects. Only the addition of birth head circumference
to the model partially reduced the effect of cocaine at 2 years (from 6 points
to 5.6 points), but it still did not reduce the significance of the cocaine
effect (F = 9.8; P = .002), indicating that some
of the negative effects of cocaine exposure on cognitive outcome were mediated
through smaller head circumference at birth. Interaction (effect modifiers)
was also tested with significant confounding variables, as well as race, sex,
and HOME score, but no interaction effects were found.
When incidence of scores in the range of mental retardation (Mental
Development Index <70) at 2 years of age was compared, cocaine-exposed
children were significantly more likely to be so classified, namely, 13.7%
(27) of the exposed vs 7.1% (13) of the unexposed (χ21 = 4.3; P = .04). Mild delays (Mental Development
Index <80) were present in 37.6% (74) of exposed children vs 20.9% (38)
of the unexposed group (χ21 = 12.6; P = .001). Adjusted for the same covariates in the linear model, the
odds ratio for scores in the range of mental retardation was 1.87 (95% confidence
interval [CI], 0.92-3.80; χ2 = 3.0; P
= .08) and for scores less than 80 was 1.98 (95% CI, 1.21-3.24; χ2 = 7.5; P = .006).
For the Psychomotor Development Index, significant effects of age, sex,
and prenatal tobacco exposure, but not cocaine, were found (Table 6). Scores for both groups of children increased significantly
from 6.5 months to 2 years of age (Table
7), tobacco exposure predicted lower motor scores, and boys had
lower motor scores than girls.
Correlations of Meconium Metabolites With Outcomes
Spearman rank correlations of the concentration of meconium metabolites
of cocaine and cognitive and motor outcomes are listed in Table 8. The concentration level of benzoylecgonine was negatively
related to the Mental Development Index score at 6.5 months, and all cocaine
metabolites except cocaethylene were related negatively to Mental Development
Index scores at 1 and 2 years. Higher levels of benzoylecgonine and meta-hydroxybenzoylecgonine
were also related to a lower Psychomotor Development Index score at 2 years.
Our study found significant cognitive deficits, with cocaine-exposed
children twice as likely to have significant delay throughout the first 2
years of life. The 13.7% rate of mental retardation is 4.89 times higher than
that expected in the population at large, and the percentage of children with
mild or greater delays requiring intervention was 38%, almost double the rate
of the high-risk noncocaine but polydrug-exposed comparison group. Because
2-year Mental Development Index scores are predictive of later cognitive outcomes,
it is possible that these children will continue to have learning difficulties
at school age.37,38
Cognitive delays could not be attributed to exposure to other drugs
or to a large number of potentially confounding variables. Further, poorer
cognitive outcomes were related to higher amounts of cocaine metabolites in
infant meconium as well as to maternal self-reported measures of amount and
frequency of cocaine use during pregnancy, providing further support for a
teratologic model. Developing neural systems of the fetal brain may be directly
affected by cocaine's adverse effects on monoaminergic system development
or indirectly affected through vascular constriction with subsequent decreases
in placental blood flow and generalized hypoxemia.4,5,39
In nonhuman primate models, fetal cocaine exposure has significant permanent
effects on cortical morphology, including a reduction in the number of cortical
cells, inappropriate positioning of cortical neurons, and altered glial morphology.40-42 Cognitive impairments
have been demonstrated in preclinical and human studies of cocaine exposure
as well, especially in assessments of learning and memory.12-16,24,42,43
Selective attention in particular appears to be affected and may contribute
to overall slower rates of learning in rat and human studies.25,43-45
In this sample, cocaine effects on cognitive outcomes were also partially
mediated by lower birth head circumference, which we have demonstrated to
be independently related to the level of prenatal cocaine exposure in a previous
study,10 further suggesting a direct biological
linkage of cocaine exposure to child behavioral outcomes.
Some limitations to this study should be considered. Although examiners
were masked to infant drug status, it may have been possible to identify drug
exposure through maternal or caregiver characteristics, since all children
were assessed with the caregiver present. The sample was also recruited according
to hospital screening measures and reflects outcomes only of more heavily
exposed infants. Also, the drug assessments were conducted retrospectively,
making reliability of maternal report problematic. In a separate study, we
found that biological measures were confirmatory of self-reports, and validity
of these measures was indicated by their relationship to differences in exposure
groups in prior studies of this sample and to perinatal and child developmental
outcomes.10,12,45
Despite these limitations, the prospective enrollment, large sample
size, excellent retention rates, and careful control for confounding variables
support the validity of the findings. Maternal drug status was determined
through biological and clinical means, enhancing reliability of classification.23 Moreover, tobacco, alcohol, marijuana, and other
drug use was quantified, reducing the likelihood that the effects of other
drugs are undercontrolled. These findings indicate that prenatal cocaine exposure
is associated with increased risk for cognitive impairment at 2 years of age
and suggest the need for public health initiatives for substance abuse prevention
and treatment of pregnant women.
1.Kandel DB, Warner LA, Kessler RC. Epidemiology of drug use and abuse among women. In: Wetherington CL, Roman AB, eds. Drug Addiction
Research and the Health of Women. Rockville, Md: National Institutes
of Health; 1998:105-130.
2.National Institute on Drug Abuse. National Pregnancy and Health Survey: Drug Use Among
Women Delivering Live Births. Rockville, Md: National Institutes of Health; 1992. Publication 96-3819.
3.Cregler LL, Mark H. Medical complications of cocaine abuse.
N Engl J Med.1986;315:1495-1500.Google Scholar 4.Mayes LC. Developing brain and in utero cocaine exposure: effects on neural ontogeny.
Dev Psychopathol.1999;11:685-714.Google Scholar 5.Volpe J. Effects of cocaine on the fetus.
N Engl J Med.1992;327:399-407.Google Scholar 6.Chouteau M, Namerow PB, Leppert P. The effect of cocaine abuse on birthweight and gestational age.
Obstet Gynecol.1988;72:351-354.Google Scholar 7.Singer L, Arendt R, Song LY, Warshawsky E, Kliegman R. Direct and indirect interactions of cocaine with childbirth outcomes.
Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med.1994;148:959-964.Google Scholar 8.Zuckerman B, Frank DA, Hingson R.
et al. Effects of maternal marijuana and cocaine use on fetal growth.
N Engl J Med.1989;320:762-768.Google Scholar 9.Little BB, Snell LM. Brain growth among fetuses exposed to cocaine in utero: asymmetrical
growth retardation.
Obstet Gynecol.1991;77:361-364.Google Scholar 10.Singer LT, Salvator A, Arendt RE, Minnes S, Farkas K, Kliegman R. Effects of cocaine/polydrug exposure and maternal psychological distress
on infant birth outcomes.
Neurotoxicol Teratol.In press.Google Scholar 11.Richardson GA, Hamel SC, Goldschmidt L, Day NL. The effects of prenatal cocaine use on neonatal neurobehavioral status.
Neurotoxicol Teratol.1996;18:519-528.Google Scholar 12.Singer LT, Arendt RE, Minnes S, Farkas K, Salvator A. Neurobehavioral outcomes of cocaine-exposed infants.
Neurotoxicol Teratol.2000;22:653-666.Google Scholar 13.Frank DA, Augustyn M, Knight WG, Pell T, Zuckerman B. Growth, development, and behavior in early childhood following prenatal
cocaine exposure: a systematic review.
JAMA.2001;285:1613-1625.Google Scholar 14.Singer LT, Arendt R, Farkas K.
et al. Relationship of prenatal cocaine exposure and maternal postpartum psychological
distress to child developmental outcome.
Dev Psychopathol.1997;9:473-489.Google Scholar 15.Alessandri SM, Bendersky M, Lewis M. Cognitive functioning in 8- to 18-month-old drug exposed infants.
Dev Psychol.1998;34:565-573.Google Scholar 16.Arendt R, Angelopoulos J, Salvator A, Singer L. Motor development of cocaine-exposed children at age two years.
Pediatrics.1999;103:86-92.Google Scholar 17.Hurt H, Brodsky NL, Betancourt L.
et al. Cocaine-exposed children: follow-up through 30 months.
J Dev Behav Pediatr.1995;16:29-35.Google Scholar 18.Lester BM, Lagasse LL, Seifer R. Cocaine exposure and children: the meaning of subtle effects.
Science.1998;282:633-634.Google Scholar 19.Azuma SD, Chasnoff JJ. Outcome of children prenatally exposed to cocaine and other drugs:
a path analysis of three-year data.
Pediatrics.1993;92:396-402.Google Scholar 20.Richardson GA, Conroy ML, Day NL. Prenatal cocaine exposure: effects on the development of school-age
children.
Neurotoxicol Teratol.1996;18:627-634.Google Scholar 21.Singer LT. Advances and redirections in understanding effects of fetal drug exposure.
J Drug Issues.1999;29:253-262.Google Scholar 22.Jacobson SW, Jacobson JL. Methodological considerations in behavioral toxicology in infants and
children.
Dev Psychol.1991;32:390-403.Google Scholar 23.Arendt RA, Singer LT, Minnes S, Salvator A. Accuracy in detecting prenatal drug exposure.
J Drug Issues.1999;29:203-214.Google Scholar 24.Jacobson SW, Jacobson JL, Sokol RJ, Martier SS, Chiodo LM. New evidence for neurobehavioral effects of in utero cocaine exposure.
J Pediatr.1996;129:581-590.Google Scholar 25.Singer LT, Arendt RE, Fagan JF.
et al. Neonatal visual information processing in cocaine-exposed and non-exposed
infants.
Infant Behav Dev.1999;22:1-15.Google Scholar 26.Bayley N. Manual for the Bayley Scales of Infant Development. New York, NY: Psychological Corp; 1969.
27.Lewis D, Moore C, Leikin J. Cocaethylene in meconium specimens.
J Toxicol Clin Toxicol.1994;32:697-703.Google Scholar 28.Ostrea Jr EM, Brady MJ, Parks PM, Asensio DC, Naluz A. Drug screening of meconium in infants of drug dependent mothers.
J Pediatr.1989;115:474-477.Google Scholar 29.Streissguth AP. The Behavioral Teratology of Alcohol: Performance,
Behavioral, and Intellectual Deficits in Prenatally Exposed Children. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 1986.
30.Dunn LM, Dunn LM, Robertsong GJ, Eisenberg JL. Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test—Revised. Circle Pines, Minn: American Guidance Service; 1981.
31.Wechsler D. Weschler Adult Intelligence Scale—Revised. San Antonio, Tex: Psychological Corp; 1989.
32.Derogatis L. The Brief Symptom Inventory: Administration, Scoring,
and Procedures Manual. 2nd ed. Baltimore, Md: Clinical Psychometric Research Inc; 1992.
33.Hobel CJ, Hyvarinen MA, Okada DM, Oh W. Prenatal and intrapartum high risk screening, I: prediction of the
high risk neonate.
Am J Obstet Gynecol.1973;117:1-9.Google Scholar 34.Bayley N. Bayley Scales of Infant Development. 2nd ed. San Antonio, Tex: Psychological Corp; 1993.
35.Caldwell B, Bradley R. Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment. Little Rock: University of Arkansas Press; 1984.
36.Barron RM, Kenny DA. The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological
research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations.
J Pers Soc Psychol.1986;51:1173-1182.Google Scholar 37.Fagan JF, Singer LT. Infant recognition memory as a measure of intelligence. In: Lipsitt L, ed. Advances in Infancy Research. Vol. 2. Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing Corp; 1983:31-78.
38.Molfese VJ, Acheson S. Infant and preschool mental and verbal abilities: how are infant scores
related to preschool scores?
Int J Behav Dev.1997;20:595-607.Google Scholar 39.Wood Jr JR, Plessinger MA, Clark KE. Effects of cocaine on uterine blood flow and fetal oxygenation.
JAMA.1987;257:957-961.Google Scholar 40.Lidow MS. Prenatal cocaine exposure adversely affects development of the primate
cerebral cortex.
Synapse.1995;21:332-341.Google Scholar 41.Wilkins AS, Maroba JJ, Tabit E, Kosfsky BE. Transplacental cocaine exposure, III: mechanisms underlying altered
brain development.
Neurotoxicol Teratol.1998;20:239-249.Google Scholar 42.Kosofsky BE, Wilkins AS. A mouse model of transplacental cocaine exposure: clinical implications
for exposed infants and children.
Ann N Y Acad Sci.1998;846:248-261.Google Scholar 43.Johns JM, Means MJ, Anderson DR, Bass EW, Means LW, McMillan BA. Prenatal exposure to cocaine, II: effects on open-field activity and
cognitive behavior in Sprague-Dawley rats.
Neurotoxicol Teratol.1992;14:343-349.Google Scholar 44.Strupp BJ, Morgan RE, Garavan H, Mactutus CF, Booze RM. Prenatal cocaine exposure: an emerging cognitive profile.
Neurotoxicol Teratol.1998;20:355.Google Scholar 45.Singer LT, Arendt RA, Minnes S, Salvator A, Siegel AC, Lewis BA. Developing language skills of cocaine exposed infants.
Pediatrics.2001;107:1057-1064.Google Scholar