[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Figure. Estimated Pooled Gender Effect Sizes for Categories of Patient-Physician Communication
Image description not available.
Error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals.
Table 1. Number of Physicians and Studies by Physician Specialty and Training Level*
Image description not available.
Table 2. Mean Number (Range) of Participants and Visits per Study
Image description not available.
Table 3. Summary of Effects of Physician Gender on Categories of Patient-Physician Communication
Image description not available.
1.
Lipkin Jr M, Quill TE, Napodano RJ. The medical interview: a core curriculum for residencies in internal medicine.  Ann Intern Med.1984;100:277-284.Google Scholar
2.
Hall JA, Roter DL, Katz NR. Meta-analysis of correlates of provider behavior in medical encounters.  Med Care.1988;26:657-675.Google Scholar
3.
Stewart MA. Effective physician-patient communication and health outcomes: a review.  CMAJ.1996;152:1423-1433.Google Scholar
4.
Weisman CS, Teitelbaum MA. Women and health care communication.  Patient Educ Couns.1989;13:183-199.Google Scholar
5.
Lurie N, Margolis KL, McGovern PG, Mink PJ, Slater JS. Why do patients of female physicians have higher rates of breast and cervical cancer screening?  J Gen Intern Med.1997;12:34-43.Google Scholar
6.
Steingart RM, Packer M, Hamm P.  et al.  Sex differences in the management of coronary artery disease.  N Engl J Med.1991;325:226-230.Google Scholar
7.
Verbrugge LM, Steiner RP. Physician treatment of men and women patients: sex bias or appropriate care?  Med Care.1981;19:609-632.Google Scholar
8.
Hall JA. Nonverbal Sex Differences: Communication Accuracy and Expressive StyleBaltimore, Md: Johns Hopkins University Press; 1984.
9.
Aries E. Men and Women in Interaction: Reconsidering the Differences. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 1996.
10.
Dindia K, Allen M. Sex differences in self-disclosure: a meta-analysis.  Psychol Bull.1992;112:106-124.Google Scholar
11.
Eagly AH, Johnson BT. Gender and leadership style: a meta-analysis.  Psychol Bull.1990;108:233-256.Google Scholar
12.
Kurtz RA, Chalfant HP. The Sociology of Medicine and Illness2nd ed. Boston, Mass: Allyn & Bacon; 1991.
13.
Mechanic D. Medical SociologyNew York, NY: Free Press; 1978.
14.
Jonas HS, Etzel S. Undergraduate medical education.  JAMA.1988;260:1063-1071.Google Scholar
15.
Klass P. A Not Entirely Benign Procedure: Four Years as a Medical StudentNew York, NY: Signet; 1987.
16.
Roter DL, Hall JA, Katz NR. Patient-physician communication: a descriptive summary of the literature.  Patient Educ Couns.1988;12:99-119.Google Scholar
17.
Rosenthal R. Meta-analytic Procedures for Social ResearchBeverly Hills, Calif: Sage; 1991.
18.
Hall JA, Rosenthal R. Interpreting and evaluating meta-analysis.  Eval Health Prof.1995;18:393-407.Google Scholar
19.
Cohen J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences2nd ed. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; 1988.
20.
 STATA Statistical Software: Release 6.0 . College Station, Tex: STATA Corp; 1999.
21.
Sharp S, Sterne J. New syntax and output for the meta analysis command. In: Newton HJ, ed. STATA Technical Bulletin Reprints. Vol 7. College Station, Tex: STATA Corp; 1998:106-108.
22.
Bernzweig J, Takayama JI, Phibbs C, Lewis C, Pantell RH. Gender differences in physician-patient communication: evidence from pediatric visits.  Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med.1997;151:586-591.Google Scholar
23.
Bertakis KD, Helms LJ, Callahan EJ, Azari R, Robbins JA. The influence of gender on physician practice style.  Med Care.1995;33:407-416.Google Scholar
24.
Charon R, Greene MG, Adelman R. Women readers, women doctors: a feminist reader-response theory for medicine. In: More ES, Milligan MA, eds. The Empathic Practitioner: Empathy, Gender, and Medicine. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press; 1994:205-221.
25.
Colliver JA, Nu VV, Marcy ML, Travis TA, Robbs RS. Effects of examinee gender, standardized-patient gender, and their interaction on standardized patients' ratings of examinees' interpersonal and communication skills.  Acad Med.1993;68:153-157.Google Scholar
26.
Cypress BK. Characteristics of visits to female and male physicians.  Vital Health Stat 13.1980;13:12-16.Google Scholar
27.
Hall JA, Irish JT, Roter DL, Ehrlich CM, Miller LH. Gender in medical encounters: an analysis of physician and patient communication in a primary care setting.  Health Psychol.1994;13:384-392.Google Scholar
28.
Irish JT, Hall JA. Interruptive patterns in medical visits: the effects of role, status and gender.  Soc Sci Med.1995;41:874-881.Google Scholar
29.
Harrigan JA, Gramata JF, Lucic KS, Margolis C. It's how you say it: physicians' vocal behavior.  Soc Sci Med.1989;28:87-92.Google Scholar
30.
Huston S, Sleath B, Rubin RH. Physician gender and hormone replacement therapy discussion.  J Womens Health Gend Based Med.2001;10:279-287.Google Scholar
31.
Jarski RW, Gjerde CL, Bratton BD, Brown DD, Matthes SS. A comparison of four empathy instruments in simulated patient-medical student interactions.  J Med Ed.1985;60:545-551.Google Scholar
32.
Law SA, Britten N. Factors that influence the patient centredness of a consultation.  Br J Gen Pract.1995;45:520-524.Google Scholar
33.
Marteau TM, Humphrey C, Matoon G, Kidd J, LLoyd M, Horder J. Factors influencing the communication skills of first-year clinical medical students.  Med Educ.1991;25:127-134.Google Scholar
34.
Marvel K, Epstein RM, Flowers J, Beckman HB. Soliciting the patient's agenda: have we improved?  JAMA.1999;281:283-287.Google Scholar
35.
Meeuwesen L, Schaap C, van der Staak C. Verbal analysis of doctor-patient communication.  Soc Sci Med.1991;10:1143-1150.Google Scholar
36.
Mendez A, Shymansky JA, Wolraich M. Verbal and non-verbal behavior of doctors while conveying distressing information.  Med Educ.1986;20:437-443.Google Scholar
37.
Meuleman JR, Harward MP. Assessing medical interview performance: effect of interns' gender and month of training.  Arch Intern Med.1992;152:1677-1680.Google Scholar
38.
Roter D, Lipkin Jr M, Korsgaard A. Sex differences in patients' and physicians' communication during primary care medical visits.  Med Care.1991;29:1083-1093.Google Scholar
39.
Roter DL, Geller G, Bernhardt BA, Larson SM, Doksum T. Effects of obstetrician gender on communication and patient satisfaction.  Obstet Gynecol.1999;93:635-641.Google Scholar
40.
Saunders DG, Kindy Jr P. Predictors of physicians' responses to woman abuse: the role of gender, background, and brief training.  J Gen Intern Med.1993;8:606-609.Google Scholar
41.
Shapiro J. Patterns of psychosocial performance in the doctor-patient encounter: a study of family practice residents.  Soc Sci Med.1990;31:1035-1041.Google Scholar
42.
Shapiro J, Schiermer DD. Resident psychosocial performance: a brief report.  Fam Pract.1991;8:10-13.Google Scholar
43.
van den Brink-Muinen A, de Bakker DH, Bensing JM. Consultations for women's health problems: factors influencing women's choice of sex of general practitioner.  Br J Gen Pract.1994;44:205-210.Google Scholar
44.
Bensing JM, Van den Brink-Muinen A, de Bakker DH. Gender differences in practice style: a Dutch study of general practitioners.  Med Care.1993;31:219-229.Google Scholar
45.
van den Brink-Muinen A, Bensing JM, Kerssens JJ. Gender and communication style in general practice: differences between women's health care and regular health care.  Med Care.1998;36:100-106.Google Scholar
46.
van den Brink-Muinen A. Gender, Health and Health Care in General PracticeUtrecht, the Netherlands: Nivel; 1996.
47.
van Dulmen AM, Bensing JM. Gender differences in gynecologist communication.  Women Health.2000;30:49-61.Google Scholar
48.
Wasserman RC, Inui TS, Barriatua RD, Carter WB, Lippincott P. Pediatric clinicians' support for parents makes a difference: an outcome-based analysis of clinician-parent interaction.  Pediatrics.1984;74:1047-1053.Google Scholar
49.
West C. Routine Complications: Troubles With Talk Between Doctors and PatientsBloomington: Indiana University Press; 1984:51-70.
50.
Labov W, Fanshel D. Therapeutic Discourse: Psychotherapy as ConversationNew York, NY: Academic Press; 1977.
51.
Skelton JR, Hobbs FR. Descriptive study of cooperative language in primary care consultations by male and female doctors.  BMJ.1999;318:576-579.Google Scholar
52.
Roter DL. The enduring and evolving nature of the patient-physician relationship.  Patient Educ Couns.2000;39:5-15.Google Scholar
53.
Hall JA, Roter DL. Do patients talk differently to male and female physicians? a meta-analytic review.  Patient Educ Couns.In press.Google Scholar
54.
Mechanic D, McAlpine DD, Rosenthal M. Are patients' office visits with physicians getting shorter?  N Engl J Med.2001;344:198-204.Google Scholar
55.
Carr-Hill R, Jenkins-Clarke S, Dixon P, Pringle M. Do minutes count? consultation lengths in general practice.  J Health Serv Res Policy.1998;3:207-213.Google Scholar
56.
Stafford RS, Saglam D, Causino N.  et al.  Trends in adult visits to primary care physicians in the United States.  Arch Fam Med.1999;8:26-32.Google Scholar
57.
Henderson JT, Weisman CS. Physician gender effects on preventive screening and counseling: an analysis of male and female patients' health care experiences.  Med Care.2001;39:1281-1292.Google Scholar
58.
Mechanic D. Changing medical organization and the erosion of trust.  Milbank Q.1996;74:171-189.Google Scholar
59.
Roter DL, Hall JA. Why physician gender matters in the shaping of the patient-physician relationship.  J Womens Health.1998;7:1093-1097.Google Scholar
60.
US Department of Health and Human Services.  Mental Health: A Report of the Surgeon GeneralRockville, Md: US Dept of Health and Human Services; 1999.
61.
Elstad J. Women's priorities regarding physician behavior and their preference for a female physician.  Women Health.1994;21:1-19.Google Scholar
62.
Pearse WH. The Commonwealth Fund Women's Health Survey: selected results and comments.  Womens Health Issues.1994;4:38-47.Google Scholar
63.
Roter DL, Hall JA. How physician gender shapes the communication and evaluation of medical care.  Mayo Clin Proc.2001;76:673-676.Google Scholar
64.
Roter DL, Hall JA, Kern DE, Barker LR, Cole KA, Roca RP. Improving physicians' interviewing skills and reducing patients' emotional distress: a randomized clinical trial.  Arch Intern Med.1995;155:1877-1884.Google Scholar
65.
Davis DA, Thomson MA, Oxman AD, Haynes RB. Changing physician performance: a systematic review of the effect of continuing medical education strategies.  JAMA.1995;274:700-705.Google Scholar
66.
Smith RC, Marshall-Dorsey AA, Osborn GG.  et al.  Evidence-based guidelines for teaching patient-centered interviewing.  Patient Educ Couns.2000;39:27-36.Google Scholar
67.
Hall JA, Roter DL. Medical communication and gender: a summary of research.  J Gend Specif Med.1998;1:39-42.Google Scholar
68.
Lipsey MW, Wilson DB. The efficacy of psychological, educational, and behavioral treatment: confirmation from meta-analysis.  Am Psychol.1993;48:1181.Google Scholar
69.
Meyer GJ, Finn SE, Eyde LD.  et al.  Psychological testing and psychological assessment: a review of evidence and issues.  Am Psychol.2001;56:128-165.Google Scholar
The Patient-Physician Relationship
August 14, 2002

Physician Gender Effects in Medical Communication: A Meta-analytic Review

Author Affiliations

Author Affiliations: Departments of Health Policy and Management (Dr Roter) and Environmental Health Sciences (Mr Aoki), Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore, Md; and Department of Psychology, Northeastern University, Boston, Mass (Dr Hall).

 

The Patient-Physician Relationship Section Editor: Richard M. Glass, MD, Deputy Editor.

JAMA. 2002;288(6):756-764. doi:10.1001/jama.288.6.756
Abstract

Context Physician gender has been viewed as a possible source of variation in the interpersonal aspects of medical practice, with speculation that female physicians facilitate more open and equal exchange and a different therapeutic milieu from that of male physicians. However, studies in this area are generally based on small samples, with conflicting results.

Objective To systematically review and quantify the effect of physician gender on communication during medical visits.

Data Sources Online database searches of English-language abstracts for the years 1967 to 2001 (MEDLINE, AIDSLINE, PsycINFO, and Bioethics); a hand search was conducted of reprint files and the reference sections of review articles and other publications.

Study Selection Studies using a communication data source, such as audiotape, videotape, or direct observation, and large national or regional studies in which physician report was used to establish length of visit, were identified through bibliographic and computerized searches. Twenty-three observational studies and 3 large physician-report studies reported in 29 publications met inclusion criteria and were rated.

Data Extraction The Cohen d was computed based on 2 reviewers' (J.A.H. and Y.A.) independent extraction of quantitative information from the publications. Study heterogeneity was tested using Q statistics and pooled effect sizes were computed using the appropriate effects model. The characteristics of the study populations were also extracted.

Data Synthesis Female physicians engage in significantly more active partnership behaviors, positive talk, psychosocial counseling, psychosocial question asking, and emotionally focused talk. There were no gender differences evident in the amount, quality, or manner of biomedical information giving or social conversation. Medical visits with female physicians are, on average, 2 minutes (10%) longer than those with male physicians. Obstetrics and gynecology may present a different pattern than that of primary care, with male physicians demonstrating higher levels of emotionally focused talk than their female colleagues.

Conclusions Female primary care physicians engage in more communication that can be considered patient centered and have longer visits than their male colleagues. Limited studies exist outside of primary care, and gender-related practice patterns in some subspecialties may differ from those evident in primary care.

Studies have linked physicians' communication skills to a variety of positive outcomes, including patient and physician satisfaction, higher levels of adherence to therapeutic recommendations, improved physiological indicators of disease control, and enhanced physical and mental health status.1-3 Within this context, gender has stimulated a good deal of interest as a possible source of variation in the interpersonal aspects of medical practice, with speculation that female physicians facilitate more open and equal exchange and a different therapeutic milieu from that of male physicians.4-7

Outside of the medical context, differences in the interpersonal style of women compared with men are well documented.8-11 Women disclose more information about themselves in conversation,10 they have a warmer and more engaged style of nonverbal communication,8 and they encourage and facilitate others to talk to them more freely and in a warmer and more intimate way.8 There is also evidence that women take greater pains to downplay their own status in an attempt to equalize status with a partner, in contrast with men's tendency to assert status differences.11 Despite gender differences in routine conversation, it is not known whether "female-linked" conversational styles are evident among medical students in light of selection criteria and, if present, survive the long medical training process.12-15 The purpose of this article is to quantitatively summarize empirical studies relating physician gender to physicians' communication with patients during medical visits.

Methods
Search Procedure and Criteria for Study Inclusion

To be included in the review, a study had to (1) involve physicians, physicians in training (interns or residents), or medical students; (2) involve actual or standardized patients; (3) measure communication using neutral observers (including standardized patients as observers), audiotape, or videotape, with the exception of inclusion of physician-reported length of the medical visit; (4) test for an association between physician gender and at least 1 interpretable physician communication variable; (5) deal with nonpsychiatric medical visits; and (6) be published in an English-language book or journal.

Studies were identified through online database searches of MEDLINE (1967-2001), AIDSLINE, PsycINFO, and Bioethics using the key words doctor-patient interaction, patient-interaction, physician-patient interaction, and doctor-patient relationship. These key words were combined with other key words: female, gender effects, female physicians, female doctors, and effect of sex of doctor. In addition, a hand search was conducted of our own reprint files and the reference sections of review articles and other publications.

Analytic Approach

A systematic review of the studies produced more than 150 different communication variables related to physician gender. Starting with a conceptual framework developed in an earlier meta-analysis evaluating correlates of physician communication in medical visits,2,16 the majority of variables were easily classified into several subsuming categories. The remaining variables were assigned to groupings based on an agreement between the first 2 authors, as reflected in the . (A listing of the individual variables, the study citations from which the variables were drawn, their assignment to each category, and a notation indicating the variables for which ≥1 studies showed a significant physician gender effect is available on request from the authors.)

For measures reflecting differences in length of visit, effect size was expressed as the mean difference between male and female physicians measured in minutes. For the other communication variables, however, the scale of measurement often varied across studies so that direct comparison of mean values across studies, even when applied to conceptually equivalent variables, was not useful. Consequently, gender differences were expressed as a Cohen d, defined as the difference between the male and female means divided by their pooled within-group SD.17,18 For this measure of effect size, a positive Cohen d value indicates that female physicians scored higher on the variable in question than male physicians did, and a negative Cohen d value indicates the reverse. In the studies summarized herein, the Cohen d was never reported directly but, rather, was calculated from the published information using standard formulas (eg, means and SDs, frequencies or percentages, correlation coefficients, or the χ2 test, t test, or F test).17 Two investigators (J.A.H. and Y.A.) independently abstracted the quantitative information from the publications and calculated the Cohen d. If a study reported a result as nonsignificant and gave no other useful data for calculation of the Cohen d, a conservative approach was taken and a 0 was used in the calculation. A final consideration in the presentation of results is the magnitude of the effect size. Cohen's rule of thumb for effect size considers 0.2 as small, 0.5 as medium, and 0.8 or greater as large.19

When there was substantial heterogeneity (P>.10 for heterogeneity based on Q statistics) in the effect sizes within a given category, a random-effects model was used in the calculation of the Cohen d (pooled effect size). When the test for heterogeneity was nonsignificant, a fixed-effects model was used. In addition to the Cohen d, a standard normal deviate (z score, the statistic associated with a P value; eg, the z score associated with P = .05 by a 2-tailed test is 1.96) was derived for each result, and these were added and divided by the square root of the total number of studies to obtain a z score and an associated combined P value (Stouffer method).17 If an author reported a result as nonsignificant and gave no other useful data for calculation of a z score, a z of 0 was used in the calculation of the combined probability.17 Calculating a combined value that includes all studies captures information that is often embedded in null results and generally lost, and provides a commonly understood probability measure to compare results across variables of interest. In this analysis, some studies provided more than 1 result for a given communication category. For example, a hypothetical study might measure "shared decision making," "lets patient state concerns," and "encourages patient paraphrasing," all of which would have been placed in the category of partnership building. Because of conceptual redundancy, only the strongest single association for a given category in a given study was entered in summaries of effect size and z score. STATA statistical software20 and user-written modules21 were used for all analyses. Throughout this article, P<.05 is considered statistically significant.

Descriptive Summary of the Literature

Twenty-six studies, reported in 29 publications, were included in the meta-analytic calculations; 23 of these were direct observation studies and 3 were based on physician report of visit length.5,22-49 (References 27 and 28 are publications reporting data from the same study. This is also the case for references 43 and 44 as well as 45 and 46.)

As reflected in Table 1, most studies were conducted in primary care settings, and physicians at all levels of training were represented in the studies. The average number of physicians in the observational studies was 40, with male physicians substantially outnumbering female physicians (n = 25 and 15, respectively; Table 2). The average number of visits per study was 157; this reflected an average of 97 visits to male physicians and 65 visits to female physicians in each study. There was wide variation in the number of patients observed by each physician; the average was 4, with a range of 1 to 32.

Three large databases were used to supplement the observational data on length of visit by physician report. The US analysis was based on the National Ambulatory Medical Care Survey and was used to derive separate estimates of physician gender effects for length of visits in 5 specialties: general and family practice, internal medicine, pediatrics, obstetrics and gynecology, and dermatology.26 A similar Dutch analysis derived estimates of length of visit from the Dutch National Study on Morbidity and Interventions in General Practice for general practitioners, with a subanalysis of general practitioners treating patients for "women's health problems".43,44 The third report represents a regional US study.5

The majority of studies were based on complete patient visits; however, 3 studies limited evaluation to a specific portion of the visit and 6 studies were of a standardized patient interview. Ten of the studies were based on all-female patient populations, which included mothers with their children seeking pediatric care (children were of both sexes), standardized patients (all of whom were female), and women seeking obstetrical care. There were no studies of exclusively male patient populations.

Eighteen different systems were used to measure communication variables. Only 2 of these systems were used in more than 1 study; the Beckman and Frankel method for assessment of physician probing of patient concerns was used in 2 studies34,37 and the Roter Interaction Analysis System was used in 5 studies.27,38,39,45,47 Eighteen of the 23 observational studies reported intercoder reliability; all were in the mid to high range (0.59-1.0) based on Pearson correlation coefficients or κ statistics.

Results

Details of the results reviewed below relating effects of physician gender to categories of patient-physician communication are displayed in Table 3. Figure 1 summarizes these results graphically.

Information Giving

Informational Content Nine studies addressed the relationship of biomedical information giving and physician gender. Two of these studies reported significant results; one45,46 reported significantly higher levels for male physicians, but the other reported the opposite.38 Neither the combined z score nor the Cohen d was statistically significant.

A more consistent picture of gender effect emerged for psychosocial discussion. Five of the 10 studies addressing psychosocial discussion reported significantly higher levels for female rather than male physicians23,24,40,41,45,46; only 1 study (a study of gynecologists47) reported higher (but nonsignificant) levels of psychosocial discussion for male physicians. Both the combined z score and the pooled Cohen d were significant, as shown in Table 3.

Informational Manner Information can be given in a more or less directive manner; for instance, giving specific instructions is an example of the former, while proposing alternatives is an example of the latter.50 Nine studies assessed variables that could be considered directive; 2 of these found higher levels of directiveness for male physicians35,39 and 1 reported higher levels for female pediatricians.22 Five studies assessed variables consistent with nondirective information giving. Of these, only 1 study reported a significant result indicating higher levels for female physicians.35 Neither the overall combined z score summarizing these studies nor the pooled effect size was significant.

Informational Quality Measures of information-giving quality include such variables as clear communication and avoidance of jargon. Six studies investigated this issue and none reported statistically significant results.

Question Content and Form

Questions are broadly concerned with data gathering and may reflect general question asking (simply defined as requesting information) or content-specific inquiries (questions with biomedical, psychosocial, compliance-related content). Questions were sometimes also specified by form, either as open-ended or closed-ended. (Questions with the intent of furthering patient introspection or clarifying patient expectations were classified as partnership building and are discussed later.)

There was little evidence of a gender effect for general question asking, for biomedical questions, or for compliance-related questions. Five studies identified questions generally; 2 of the 3 studies that reported significant results reported significantly higher levels of question asking among female physicians,22,41 while another found the opposite,23 yielding a nonsignificant pooled effect. For biomedical questions, 2 studies reported significant results; one reported significantly higher levels of question asking among male physicians,47 while the other found the opposite,28 with a nonsignificant pooled effect. Two studies specified questions related to patient compliance and neither reported a significant gender effect (results not shown).41,42

As was evident in the content analysis of information giving, women showed higher levels of psychosocial question asking than men; 3 of 6 studies reported significant results indicating higher levels for female physicians.28,40,41 No studies reported higher levels of psychosocial questioning by male physicians. The combined z score was statistically significant and the Cohen d showed a trend toward significance (P = .06).

One of 4 studies that assessed closed questions reported significantly higher levels among women38 and none reported higher levels for men. The pooled findings reflect a statistically significant Cohen d and a trend toward a significant combined z score (P = .09). In contrast, we found little evidence of gender effect on open-ended questions; 1 study of 6 found significantly higher levels for female physicians,42 but neither the combined z score nor the Cohen d was statistically significant.

Partnership Building

By our definition, partnership building occurs when a physician actively facilitates patient participation in the medical visit or attempts to equalize status by assuming a less dominating stance within the relationship. The 2 classes of partnering behavior can be distinguished as reflecting (1) an active facilitation of partnership, or "enlistment," or (2) a more passive or "lowered-dominance" approach to partnership (see the for examples). Twelve studies included the active, enlistment-type variables in their assessments. Six of these studies reported significantly higher levels of enlistment on the part of female physicians,24,28,32,38,41,47 and 2 studies showed the reverse.37,39 Heterogeneity analysis indicated substantial variation among the studies. Both the combined z score and the pooled Cohen d were statistically significant, as shown in Table 3. (We became aware late in this article's production of an overlooked study that examined communication elements that would have been included in the active partnership category.51 The study assessed facilitative phrases with question tags such as "isn't it?" and "don't you?" The study reported significantly higher use of the question tag "don't you?" by female compared with male physicians [P = .03; Cohen d = 0.32]. The question tag "isn't it?" did not show a significant gender effect.)

Five studies assessed variables reflecting the passive, lowered-dominance approach and 1 of these reported a statistically significant result indicating lowered dominance among female physicians.24 Neither the Cohen d nor the combined z score summarizing these studies was significant.

Social Conversation

Social conversation is defined as nonmedical exchanges, largely social pleasantries and greetings. Seven studies included measures of social conversation. One study reported a statistically significant result reflecting higher levels of social conversation by female pediatricians.22 The measures summarizing these studies were not significant, as shown in Table 3.

Positive Talk

Positive talk assessment captures the generally positive atmosphere created in the visit through verbal behaviors such as agreements, encouragement, and reassurance. Social conversation was not included in this category, with the exception of 2 studies in which it was embedded in a composite variable composed of positive elements. Fourteen studies included some measure of positive talk. Six of these studies reported significantly higher levels of positive talk for female physicians.22,28,33,38,42,47 No studies reported higher levels of positive talk by male physicians. The Cohen d was significant and the combined z score summarizing these studies was large, as shown in Table 3.

Eight of the studies measuring positive talk were based on exclusively female patient populations, while 6 studies were based on both male and female patients. Inspection of the pattern of results (data not shown) shows evidence of a physician gender effect for all-female patient populations (pooled effect size, 0.25; 95% confidence interval [CI], −0.01 to 0.52; P = .06) as well as mixed-gender populations (pooled effect size, 0.59; 95% CI, 0.41-0.77; P<.001). Comparison of the pooled effect size estimates from these 2 populations suggests a significantly greater effect in mixed-gender populations than in exclusively female populations (P = .01).

Negative Talk

The negative talk category included explicit verbal expressions of criticism or disapproval. Nine studies assessed some element of negative talk. Two of these studies reported a significant result, one of which indicated that female physicians were more likely to avoid being critical of a patient than male physicians41 and the other of which reflected higher levels of disagreement by female compared with male obstetricians.39 Neither the Cohen d nor the overall combined z score summarizing these studies was significant, as shown in Table 3.

Emotionally Focused Talk

Emotionally focused talk included explicit inquiry about feelings and emotions, exploration of emotional concerns, and statements of empathy and concern. This category is distinguished from psychosocial exchange by its direct link to feelings and emotions. Thirteen studies assessed emotional talk in some manner; 4 of these found significantly higher levels for female compared with male physicians.33,36,42,48 Both gynecology studies in the review found higher levels of emotional talk for male physicians; one of these reported a significant result39 and the other was marginally significant (P = .06).47 The combined z score was statistically significant but the pooled Cohen d was not, as shown in Table 3.

The studies showed a high degree of heterogeneity, which was almost entirely explained by the 2 obstetrics and gynecology studies. Estimates based on the pooling of the 11 primary care studies showed a significant and consistent gender effect favoring female physicians (effect size, 0.16; 95% CI, 0.04-0.29; P = .008), with a low degree of heterogeneity (P for heterogeneity = .19). Analysis of the 2 obstetrics and gynecology studies showed a significant gender effect favoring male physicians (effect size, −0.27; 95% CI, −0.49 to −0.07; P = .01), with a low degree of heterogeneity (P = .41 for heterogeneity).

Nonverbal Communication

Nonverbal communication includes positive nonverbal behaviors (smiles, nods, friendly voice tone, relaxed hands), and a variety of behaviors that can have ambiguous, neutral, or negative meaning depending on the context of use (eg, touches patient, folds hands, gestures while speaking, points at the patient, speech is disturbed, voice tone reflects anxiety or boredom). To avoid the difficulties associated with ambiguous interpretation, we limited our analysis to positive nonverbal behaviors.

Six studies assessed positive nonverbal behavior in some manner, and 2 of these studies reported significant results showing that female physicians demonstrate higher levels of smiling and head nods27 and awareness of nonverbal communication.41 No studies reported higher levels of positive nonverbal behavior for male physicians. The combined z score was significant (P = .02) and the Cohen d trended toward significance (P = .07), as shown in Table 3.

Length of Visit

Five of the 10 studies that directly measured length of visit reported women to conduct significantly longer visits than men22,24,35,38,47 and only 1 found statistically longer visits for male physicians (a study of obstetricians39). Length of visit averaged 21 minutes (range, 7.4-36.7 minutes) for male physicians and 23 minutes (range, 10.5-37.0 minutes) for female physicians. The 3 studies of physician-reported length of visit similarly found that female physicians' visits were significantly longer than male physicians' visits, with an average of 14.8 minutes for male physicians and 17.0 minutes for female physicians. (Visits in the Netherlands are substantially shorter than visits in the United States, regardless of physician gender.)

There was no statistically significant difference between the observational and physician-reported estimates of length of visit attributed to physician gender; based on the 10 observational studies, visits with female physicians averaged 2.0 minutes longer than visits with male physicians (95% CI, 0.5-3.5) and were 2.1 minutes longer (95% CI, −0.15 to 4.3) based on the 7 physician-reported studies. The overall difference in length of visit attributed to physician gender for all 17 studies was 2.05 minutes (95% CI, 0.65-3.4).

Comment

Despite widespread interest in the effects of physician gender on the care process, the literature describing these effects is small. We identified only 23 observational studies relating the communication process to physician gender. Nevertheless, the pattern of results was almost entirely consistent with what one might expect from the nonmedical literature regarding gender differences in communication. Female physicians engage in communication that more broadly relates to the larger life context of patients' conditions by addressing psychosocial issues through related questioning and counseling, greater use of emotional talk, more positive talk, and more active enlistment of patient input. When taken together, these elements comprise a pattern that can be broadly considered "patient-centered" interviewing.52 In contrast with the higher levels of psychosocial and socioemotional exchange, there is little evidence that physician gender is related to the more task-specific communication elements of care. Physician gender was not related to the provision of biomedical information (including discussion of the diagnosis, prognosis, and medical treatment), the manner in which information was given, or the quality of the information that was given.

Behavioral differences in the communication styles of male and female physicians would be especially important if they produce corresponding gender differences in patients' behavior directed back at them. Indeed, a separate meta-analysis investigating the effects of physician gender on patient communication suggested that patient behavior largely reciprocates the gender-linked physician behavior reported in the current analysis.53 Like female physicians, their patients talk more overall, make more positive statements, discuss more psychosocial information, and express more partnership-building than patients of male physicians. Patient behaviors that showed little or no difference in relation to physician gender were patient questions, social conversation, and negative statements. Some behaviors were indirectly reciprocated. Even though male and female physicians did not differ in how much biomedical information they provided to their patients, patients of female physicians provided more biomedical information to them than to male physicians. Since female physicians ask more psychosocial questions than their male counterparts, it may be that this type of question stimulates more patient disclosure of both a psychosocial and biomedical nature. More patient disclosure may also be fostered by female physicians' more active efforts to build partnership through inviting patients' opinions and through the use of facilitators and back channels, such as saying "uh-huh" and "go on," and other indicators of verbal attentiveness. Interestingly, though female physicians made more emotionally focused statements than male physicians, patients did not direct more emotional discussion to their female compared with male physicians. Patient and physician gender concordance appears to strengthen many of the gender effects observed. The 2 studies of which we are aware that have investigated the impact of gender concordance on communication27,28 both found that female gender concordant visits were characterized by longer length and more equal patient and physician contributions to the medical dialogue than all other gender combinations. Hall et al27 also reported more positive statements, nodding, and back channels in female-concordant visits compared with others.

Female physicians also spent more time with their patients than male physicians did, an average difference of about 2 minutes, or 10%, per visit. With the increasing time and productivity pressures that plague all physicians, a 2-minute-per-visit increase represents a substantial burden, easily putting a female physician an hour behind her male colleagues at the end of a busy day. Mechanic et al54 have reported that the average medical visit has increased by 1 to 2 minutes in the last 10 years. Despite this increase, the widespread perception of a shrinking visit may be fueled by the time-pressured atmosphere within which physicians may be providing more preventive and counseling services than in the past.54-56 In this light, female physicians may be at even greater risk of falling behind their male colleagues in daily scheduling. The analysis by Henderson and Weisman57 of the Commonwealth survey of patient-reported screening and counseling services concluded that female physicians provided more preventive counseling to both their male and female patients, and more gender-specific screening to their female patients than did male physicians.

Pressure to do more in limited time may act to further amplify the communication differences between physicians of different genders. While male physicians may respond to time pressures by dispensing with socioemotional and psychosocial tasks, as suggested by Mechanic,58 female physicians may find this more difficult to do.59 We suggest this because female physicians currently record proportionately more diagnoses of a psychosocial nature than their male colleagues do,26,44 and the demand for diagnosis and treatment of mental health problems in primary care is expected to grow.60

The results from the 2 obstetrics and gynecology studies deviate from those of the primary care studies. As several studies have documented especially strong patient preferences for female physicians in gynecologic and obstetric care,61,62 male physicians may feel pressured to meet the increasing competitive challenge of growing numbers of female physicians by enhancing their interpersonal skills.63 If this is the case, it would suggest that physicians are capable of modifying their communication style given sufficient motivation and incentive. The training literature is also optimistic in this regard; there is ample evidence that instruction in communication skills is associated with improvement in skills, with some studies showing these improvements to be long-lasting.64-66

The literature on which the current analysis depends is small and limited to published studies raising the possibility that relevant, unpublished material was missed. However, the fairly high number of null results in the review indicates that many studies lacking statistically significant findings were published. This is consistent with the fact that gender effects were often of secondary and even tertiary importance in terms of the main theme of an article, making it unlikely that unpublished studies would have produced a different pattern of results.17 Furthermore, we have no reason to believe that results favoring one gender over the other would be preferentially published. It is also noteworthy that more than half of the communication measures showed significant heterogeneity across studies. While we do not know what the source of unmeasured variability may be, it is likely to be some aspect of the institutional cultures of the study sites, as well as case mix and delivery system.44,54

What can we conclude about the consequences of these gender-related communication effects in terms of the variety of patient outcomes so valued in health services research? The reviewed studies did not systematically address patient outcomes, and no direct conclusion can be drawn. It seems likely that the effects found are an indication of a relatively more health-promoting therapeutic milieu produced by female physicians. Such a conclusion, however, can only be speculative, since no study has directly investigated whether patients of female physicians fare better on clinical measures. Furthermore, while physician communication behaviors similar to those reviewed here have been positively related to patient satisfaction, compliance, and patient recall and comprehension of information,2 as well as a variety of patient health outcomes,3 this is not always the case. On average, female physicians do not win out in popularity, as indicated by a review of studies that compare the satisfaction of patients seeing male vs female physicians.67 Some studies show patients to be more satisfied with male physicians, some show more satisfaction with female physicians, and some show no difference. It is premature to offer an explanation for this variation, and we can only speculate that patients' satisfaction depends on what the physician actually does, as well as stereotypes and expectations held by patients and differences in patient characteristics, such as health status or sociodemographics. Future studies of physician gender and communication will need to focus greater attention on the assessment of patient health outcomes and other indices of care quality.

In summary, the review produced a pattern of effects associated with physician gender that goes beyond a listing of individual elements of medical exchange. Taken together, the differences reflect a patient-centered communication style that inspires patient reciprocation and is likely to reflect a more intimate therapeutic milieu of heightened engagement, comfort, and partnership. Although the magnitude of the effects attributable to gender for any given communication element were usually small, the effects are comparable with those of many well-established medical, psychological, behavioral, and educational interventions.17,68,69

What might these results mean for male physicians? We do not suggest that all or even most female physicians are patient-centered and male physicians are not; there is far more common ground than difference in the communication behaviors of male and female physicians. Moreover, physicians, both male and female, who are skillful communicators may achieve time efficiencies that allow the delivery of quality, patient-centered care in even restricted time frames.64 Physicians have the capacity to improve their communication skills in meaningful ways through self-awareness, self-monitoring, and training. The potentially powerful impact of patient reciprocation of both communication style and affect in the medical visit is especially important to recognize, as recognition could help create positive exchanges and defuse negatively spiraling interaction patterns. Both male and female physicians, as well as their patients, are entitled to no less.

References
1.
Lipkin Jr M, Quill TE, Napodano RJ. The medical interview: a core curriculum for residencies in internal medicine.  Ann Intern Med.1984;100:277-284.Google Scholar
2.
Hall JA, Roter DL, Katz NR. Meta-analysis of correlates of provider behavior in medical encounters.  Med Care.1988;26:657-675.Google Scholar
3.
Stewart MA. Effective physician-patient communication and health outcomes: a review.  CMAJ.1996;152:1423-1433.Google Scholar
4.
Weisman CS, Teitelbaum MA. Women and health care communication.  Patient Educ Couns.1989;13:183-199.Google Scholar
5.
Lurie N, Margolis KL, McGovern PG, Mink PJ, Slater JS. Why do patients of female physicians have higher rates of breast and cervical cancer screening?  J Gen Intern Med.1997;12:34-43.Google Scholar
6.
Steingart RM, Packer M, Hamm P.  et al.  Sex differences in the management of coronary artery disease.  N Engl J Med.1991;325:226-230.Google Scholar
7.
Verbrugge LM, Steiner RP. Physician treatment of men and women patients: sex bias or appropriate care?  Med Care.1981;19:609-632.Google Scholar
8.
Hall JA. Nonverbal Sex Differences: Communication Accuracy and Expressive StyleBaltimore, Md: Johns Hopkins University Press; 1984.
9.
Aries E. Men and Women in Interaction: Reconsidering the Differences. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 1996.
10.
Dindia K, Allen M. Sex differences in self-disclosure: a meta-analysis.  Psychol Bull.1992;112:106-124.Google Scholar
11.
Eagly AH, Johnson BT. Gender and leadership style: a meta-analysis.  Psychol Bull.1990;108:233-256.Google Scholar
12.
Kurtz RA, Chalfant HP. The Sociology of Medicine and Illness2nd ed. Boston, Mass: Allyn & Bacon; 1991.
13.
Mechanic D. Medical SociologyNew York, NY: Free Press; 1978.
14.
Jonas HS, Etzel S. Undergraduate medical education.  JAMA.1988;260:1063-1071.Google Scholar
15.
Klass P. A Not Entirely Benign Procedure: Four Years as a Medical StudentNew York, NY: Signet; 1987.
16.
Roter DL, Hall JA, Katz NR. Patient-physician communication: a descriptive summary of the literature.  Patient Educ Couns.1988;12:99-119.Google Scholar
17.
Rosenthal R. Meta-analytic Procedures for Social ResearchBeverly Hills, Calif: Sage; 1991.
18.
Hall JA, Rosenthal R. Interpreting and evaluating meta-analysis.  Eval Health Prof.1995;18:393-407.Google Scholar
19.
Cohen J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences2nd ed. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; 1988.
20.
 STATA Statistical Software: Release 6.0 . College Station, Tex: STATA Corp; 1999.
21.
Sharp S, Sterne J. New syntax and output for the meta analysis command. In: Newton HJ, ed. STATA Technical Bulletin Reprints. Vol 7. College Station, Tex: STATA Corp; 1998:106-108.
22.
Bernzweig J, Takayama JI, Phibbs C, Lewis C, Pantell RH. Gender differences in physician-patient communication: evidence from pediatric visits.  Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med.1997;151:586-591.Google Scholar
23.
Bertakis KD, Helms LJ, Callahan EJ, Azari R, Robbins JA. The influence of gender on physician practice style.  Med Care.1995;33:407-416.Google Scholar
24.
Charon R, Greene MG, Adelman R. Women readers, women doctors: a feminist reader-response theory for medicine. In: More ES, Milligan MA, eds. The Empathic Practitioner: Empathy, Gender, and Medicine. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press; 1994:205-221.
25.
Colliver JA, Nu VV, Marcy ML, Travis TA, Robbs RS. Effects of examinee gender, standardized-patient gender, and their interaction on standardized patients' ratings of examinees' interpersonal and communication skills.  Acad Med.1993;68:153-157.Google Scholar
26.
Cypress BK. Characteristics of visits to female and male physicians.  Vital Health Stat 13.1980;13:12-16.Google Scholar
27.
Hall JA, Irish JT, Roter DL, Ehrlich CM, Miller LH. Gender in medical encounters: an analysis of physician and patient communication in a primary care setting.  Health Psychol.1994;13:384-392.Google Scholar
28.
Irish JT, Hall JA. Interruptive patterns in medical visits: the effects of role, status and gender.  Soc Sci Med.1995;41:874-881.Google Scholar
29.
Harrigan JA, Gramata JF, Lucic KS, Margolis C. It's how you say it: physicians' vocal behavior.  Soc Sci Med.1989;28:87-92.Google Scholar
30.
Huston S, Sleath B, Rubin RH. Physician gender and hormone replacement therapy discussion.  J Womens Health Gend Based Med.2001;10:279-287.Google Scholar
31.
Jarski RW, Gjerde CL, Bratton BD, Brown DD, Matthes SS. A comparison of four empathy instruments in simulated patient-medical student interactions.  J Med Ed.1985;60:545-551.Google Scholar
32.
Law SA, Britten N. Factors that influence the patient centredness of a consultation.  Br J Gen Pract.1995;45:520-524.Google Scholar
33.
Marteau TM, Humphrey C, Matoon G, Kidd J, LLoyd M, Horder J. Factors influencing the communication skills of first-year clinical medical students.  Med Educ.1991;25:127-134.Google Scholar
34.
Marvel K, Epstein RM, Flowers J, Beckman HB. Soliciting the patient's agenda: have we improved?  JAMA.1999;281:283-287.Google Scholar
35.
Meeuwesen L, Schaap C, van der Staak C. Verbal analysis of doctor-patient communication.  Soc Sci Med.1991;10:1143-1150.Google Scholar
36.
Mendez A, Shymansky JA, Wolraich M. Verbal and non-verbal behavior of doctors while conveying distressing information.  Med Educ.1986;20:437-443.Google Scholar
37.
Meuleman JR, Harward MP. Assessing medical interview performance: effect of interns' gender and month of training.  Arch Intern Med.1992;152:1677-1680.Google Scholar
38.
Roter D, Lipkin Jr M, Korsgaard A. Sex differences in patients' and physicians' communication during primary care medical visits.  Med Care.1991;29:1083-1093.Google Scholar
39.
Roter DL, Geller G, Bernhardt BA, Larson SM, Doksum T. Effects of obstetrician gender on communication and patient satisfaction.  Obstet Gynecol.1999;93:635-641.Google Scholar
40.
Saunders DG, Kindy Jr P. Predictors of physicians' responses to woman abuse: the role of gender, background, and brief training.  J Gen Intern Med.1993;8:606-609.Google Scholar
41.
Shapiro J. Patterns of psychosocial performance in the doctor-patient encounter: a study of family practice residents.  Soc Sci Med.1990;31:1035-1041.Google Scholar
42.
Shapiro J, Schiermer DD. Resident psychosocial performance: a brief report.  Fam Pract.1991;8:10-13.Google Scholar
43.
van den Brink-Muinen A, de Bakker DH, Bensing JM. Consultations for women's health problems: factors influencing women's choice of sex of general practitioner.  Br J Gen Pract.1994;44:205-210.Google Scholar
44.
Bensing JM, Van den Brink-Muinen A, de Bakker DH. Gender differences in practice style: a Dutch study of general practitioners.  Med Care.1993;31:219-229.Google Scholar
45.
van den Brink-Muinen A, Bensing JM, Kerssens JJ. Gender and communication style in general practice: differences between women's health care and regular health care.  Med Care.1998;36:100-106.Google Scholar
46.
van den Brink-Muinen A. Gender, Health and Health Care in General PracticeUtrecht, the Netherlands: Nivel; 1996.
47.
van Dulmen AM, Bensing JM. Gender differences in gynecologist communication.  Women Health.2000;30:49-61.Google Scholar
48.
Wasserman RC, Inui TS, Barriatua RD, Carter WB, Lippincott P. Pediatric clinicians' support for parents makes a difference: an outcome-based analysis of clinician-parent interaction.  Pediatrics.1984;74:1047-1053.Google Scholar
49.
West C. Routine Complications: Troubles With Talk Between Doctors and PatientsBloomington: Indiana University Press; 1984:51-70.
50.
Labov W, Fanshel D. Therapeutic Discourse: Psychotherapy as ConversationNew York, NY: Academic Press; 1977.
51.
Skelton JR, Hobbs FR. Descriptive study of cooperative language in primary care consultations by male and female doctors.  BMJ.1999;318:576-579.Google Scholar
52.
Roter DL. The enduring and evolving nature of the patient-physician relationship.  Patient Educ Couns.2000;39:5-15.Google Scholar
53.
Hall JA, Roter DL. Do patients talk differently to male and female physicians? a meta-analytic review.  Patient Educ Couns.In press.Google Scholar
54.
Mechanic D, McAlpine DD, Rosenthal M. Are patients' office visits with physicians getting shorter?  N Engl J Med.2001;344:198-204.Google Scholar
55.
Carr-Hill R, Jenkins-Clarke S, Dixon P, Pringle M. Do minutes count? consultation lengths in general practice.  J Health Serv Res Policy.1998;3:207-213.Google Scholar
56.
Stafford RS, Saglam D, Causino N.  et al.  Trends in adult visits to primary care physicians in the United States.  Arch Fam Med.1999;8:26-32.Google Scholar
57.
Henderson JT, Weisman CS. Physician gender effects on preventive screening and counseling: an analysis of male and female patients' health care experiences.  Med Care.2001;39:1281-1292.Google Scholar
58.
Mechanic D. Changing medical organization and the erosion of trust.  Milbank Q.1996;74:171-189.Google Scholar
59.
Roter DL, Hall JA. Why physician gender matters in the shaping of the patient-physician relationship.  J Womens Health.1998;7:1093-1097.Google Scholar
60.
US Department of Health and Human Services.  Mental Health: A Report of the Surgeon GeneralRockville, Md: US Dept of Health and Human Services; 1999.
61.
Elstad J. Women's priorities regarding physician behavior and their preference for a female physician.  Women Health.1994;21:1-19.Google Scholar
62.
Pearse WH. The Commonwealth Fund Women's Health Survey: selected results and comments.  Womens Health Issues.1994;4:38-47.Google Scholar
63.
Roter DL, Hall JA. How physician gender shapes the communication and evaluation of medical care.  Mayo Clin Proc.2001;76:673-676.Google Scholar
64.
Roter DL, Hall JA, Kern DE, Barker LR, Cole KA, Roca RP. Improving physicians' interviewing skills and reducing patients' emotional distress: a randomized clinical trial.  Arch Intern Med.1995;155:1877-1884.Google Scholar
65.
Davis DA, Thomson MA, Oxman AD, Haynes RB. Changing physician performance: a systematic review of the effect of continuing medical education strategies.  JAMA.1995;274:700-705.Google Scholar
66.
Smith RC, Marshall-Dorsey AA, Osborn GG.  et al.  Evidence-based guidelines for teaching patient-centered interviewing.  Patient Educ Couns.2000;39:27-36.Google Scholar
67.
Hall JA, Roter DL. Medical communication and gender: a summary of research.  J Gend Specif Med.1998;1:39-42.Google Scholar
68.
Lipsey MW, Wilson DB. The efficacy of psychological, educational, and behavioral treatment: confirmation from meta-analysis.  Am Psychol.1993;48:1181.Google Scholar
69.
Meyer GJ, Finn SE, Eyde LD.  et al.  Psychological testing and psychological assessment: a review of evidence and issues.  Am Psychol.2001;56:128-165.Google Scholar
×