Context Childhood asthma is strongly associated with allergic sensitization.
Studies have suggested that animal exposure during infancy reduces subsequent
allergic sensitization.
Objective To evaluate the relationship between dog and cat exposure in the first
year of life and allergic sensitization at 6 to 7 years of age.
Design, Setting, and Subjects Prospective birth cohort study of healthy, full-term infants enrolled
in a health maintenance organization in suburban Detroit, Mich, who were born
between April 15, 1987, and August 31, 1989, and followed up yearly to a mean
age of 6.7 years. Of 835 children initially in the study at birth, 474 (57%)
completed follow-up evaluations at age 6 to 7 years.
Main Outcome Measures Atopy, defined as any skin prick test positivity to 6 common aeroallergens
(dust mites [Dermatophagoides farinae, D pteronyssinus],
dog, cat, short ragweed [Ambrosia artemisiifolia],
and blue grass [Poa pratensis]); seroatopy, defined
as any positive allergen-specific IgE test result for the same 6 allergens
or for Alternaria species.
Results The prevalence of any skin prick test positivity (atopy) at age 6 to
7 years was 33.6% with no dog or cat exposure in the first year of life, 34.3%
with exposure to 1 dog or cat, and 15.4% with exposure to 2 or more dogs or
cats (P = .005). The prevalence of any positive allergen-specific
IgE test result (seroatopy) was 38.5% with no dog or cat exposure, 41.2% with
exposure to 1 dog or cat, and 17.9% with exposure to 2 or more dogs or cats
(P = .003). After adjustment for cord serum IgE concentration,
sex, older siblings, parental smoking, parental asthma, bedroom dust mite
allergen levels at 2 years, and current dog and cat ownership, exposure to
2 or more dogs or cats in the first year of life was associated with a significantly
lower risk of atopy (adjusted odds ratio, 0.23; 95% confidence interval, 0.09-0.60)
and seroatopy (adjusted odds ratio, 0.33; 95% confidence interval, 0.13-0.83).
Conclusion Exposure to 2 or more dogs or cats in the first year of life may reduce
subsequent risk of allergic sensitization to multiple allergens during childhood.
The increasing prevalence of asthma in the United States and other developed
countries over the last few decades has been a cause for concern.1,2 While many factors appear to be involved
in the development of childhood asthma, allergic sensitization to common allergens
has consistently been shown to be related to the risk of developing asthma
and to the risk of asthma persisting from childhood into adulthood.3-6 Many
studies have attempted to elucidate relationships between environmental exposures,
especially during infancy, and the risk of allergic sensitization in later
life.7,8 These studies are based
on the theory that an individual's genetic predisposition to allergic disease
is activated or enhanced by early allergen exposure.4,7,9
The outcome of interactions between genetic influences and allergen exposures
may be influenced by other environmental exposures, such as passive exposure
to environmental tobacco smoke.7,9
If these relationships were better understood it might become possible to
reduce the prevalence of allergic sensitization and perhaps asthma in children.
Exposure to dogs and cats during infancy has been thought to increase
the risk of subsequent allergy to these animals.8,10-12
This assumption is primarily based on a few retrospective studies reporting
an increased likelihood of allergic sensitization following exposure during
infancy.10-12
Some studies, however, have suggested that exposure to dogs or cats during
infancy is associated with reduced risk of allergic disease.13-18
Others have shown that children growing up on farms, especially farms with
animals, were less likely to be allergic than were children growing up in
urban environments.19,20
This analysis is part of the Childhood Allergy Study, a prospective
birth cohort study designed to simultaneously evaluate multiple relationships
between early environmental exposures and subsequent allergic sensitization
and asthma.21-30
Among the variables considered were parental allergy histories, parental smoking,
IgE levels in cord blood, month of birth, concentrations of dust mite and
cat allergen in the child's bedroom at age 2 years, and pet exposure. In this
analysis, we specifically examined exposure to dogs or cats in the first year
of life and a child's risk of later allergic sensitization to common allergens
after adjusting for potential confounding associations. We also examined relationships
between early dog and cat exposure and allergen-specific serum IgE concentrations,
lung function, methacholine airway responsiveness, and asthma.
The selection of children for the Childhood Allergy Study has previously
been described.21 Briefly, all pregnant women
living in an area of northern, suburban Detroit, defined by contiguous ZIP
codes, and belonging to a health maintenance organization, were eligible to
participate if their infants were born between April 15, 1987, and August
31, 1989. Only infants born at term (36 or more weeks' gestational age) with
valid measurements of cord serum IgE concentration were entered into the study.
The study was approved by the institutional human rights committee, and written
informed consent was obtained when the mothers were enrolled, at the time
of the first home visit, and prior to the clinical evaluations.
Study nurses interviewed mothers prior to delivery to obtain information
concerning each parent's level of education; presence of allergies in general
and of hay fever and asthma specifically; and parental smoking habits. The
number of siblings was also noted along with other data about the home. Cord
serum IgE concentrations were measured for all infants as previously described.21
We contacted parents by telephone when infants were aged 1 year to obtain
information on prespecified variables of interest, including the presence
and number of pets in the home during the first year. The number of dogs and
cats reported at this time was used for this analysis. When children were
aged 2 years, nurses visited each child's home to obtain information about
the home environment and to collect dust samples from the child's bedroom,
as well as urine samples from the child for measurement of urinary cotinine
as a biomarker of passive cigarette smoke exposure. The dust samples were
analyzed for concentrations of mite (Der p 1 and Der f 1) and cat (Fel d 1)
allergens using a monoclonal antibody-based enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay
as previously described.28 We have documented
the validity of parental smoking histories in this cohort with reference to
children's urinary cotinine concentrations.26
Questionnaire-based parental smoking histories from the first year of the
child's life were used for these analyses because there were fewer missing
values than for urinary cotinine concentrations. Follow-up telephone interviews
also were conducted when the children were aged 3, 5, and 6 years, and a second
home visit was conducted when the children were aged 4 years.
Evaluations for Allergic Sensitization and Asthma
Clinical evaluations for allergic sensitization and asthma were performed
when the children were aged 6 to 7 years. In addition to general medical histories
and physical examinations, these evaluations included skin prick testing with
commercial extracts of dust mites (Dermatophagoides farinae, D pteronyssinus), dog, cat, short ragweed
(Ambrosia artemisiifolia), and blue grass (Poa pratensis), along with saline and histamine controls
(all extracts and controls, Pharmaceutical Division, Bayer Inc, Spokane, Wash).
Skin prick test results were considered positive if the product of perpendicular
wheal diameters was 4 mm or more associated with a flare of at least 10 mm,
and if there was no response to the negative control. Atopy was defined as
a positive skin prick test result with any of the 6 allergens tested. Blood
samples obtained during the evaluation were assayed for total serum IgE concentration
and concentrations of allergen-specific IgE antibodies using a commercial
assay (AlaSTAT, Diagnostic Products Corp, Los Angeles, Calif). Allergen-specific
IgE testing included the same 6 allergens used for skin prick testing in addition
to Alternaria species. Total and allergen-specific
serum IgE levels were expressed in international units per milliliter (1 IU/mL
corresponds to 2.4 ng/mL). Allergen-specific IgE levels of 0.35 IU/mL or higher
were considered to be a positive test result in accordance with the manufacturer's
recommendation. Seroatopy was defined as any positive test result for an allergen-specific
IgE concentration. Numbers of children with seroatopy may have been slightly
higher than those with atopy because one additional allergen, Alternaria, was also used to define seroatopy. Because a study published
after the start of this study suggested that cockroach sensitization may be
associated with asthma,31 a random sample of
100 sera were assayed for cockroach-specific IgE and only 2 sera were positive.24 Given the low prevalence of detectable cockroach
sensitization, no further testing for cockroach-specific IgE was performed.
At the time of skin prick testing, children were defined as having current
asthma if a parent reported that they had been diagnosed by a physician as
having asthma and that they had asthma symptoms or used asthma medications
in the preceding 12 months. Pulmonary function tests were performed as previously
described, and the results are presented as the percentage of predicted using
standard equations.29 Methacholine airway responsiveness
was determined as previously described.29 After
baseline spirometry and no response to a control saline challenge, 5 doses
of methacholine (0.025-25 mg/mL) were administered through a dosimeter (Pulmonary
Data Services, Louisville, Colo). Methacholine airway responsiveness was defined
as a fall in forced expiratory volume in 1 second (FEV1) of 20%
or more from the postsaline challenge value at a concentration of administered
methacholine of 10 mg/mL or less.29
The power of this study was originally based on the ability to detect
a small to medium effect (0.2) for a χ2 test as defined by
Cohen.32 With this assumption, and assuming
an α level of .05, the power to detect significant associations between
outcomes in 3 exposure groups is greater than 90% with an overall sample size
of 470. Given the same assumptions, it also is possible to stratify the data
for a variable with approximately equal prevalences (ie, sex) and still have
power greater than 70% with 235 in each group. If the prevalence of a variable
is low in the cohort, such as current asthma, the power is much lower.
The collected data were first examined for potential imbalances between
those children lost from the study and those who were retained. χ2 Tests were used to compare the relative percentages.33
Pet exposure in the first year of life was defined as an ordinal variable
with 3 categories: no dog or cat exposure, exposure to 1 dog or cat, and exposure
to 2 or more dogs or cats. The highest strata was truncated at 2 or more because
of the small sample size above this level. Information about pet exposure
at age 6 to 7 years was used to create the same 3 categories of pet exposure
as was used for the first year of life. Binary variables of interest (eg,
atopy [yes or no], specific skin prick test positivity) were analyzed according
to pet exposure category using a χ2 test for 2 × 3 contingency
tables.33 We did not have a preconceived hypothesis
concerning a relationship between exposure to varying number of dogs and cats
and the risk of allergic sensitization. Therefore, we tested the general hypothesis
that outcomes differed across categories of dog and cat exposure rather than
testing for trends with increasing exposure.
The relationships between pet exposure categories and continuous variables,
such as percent predicted forced vital capacity (FVC) or total serum IgE,
were evaluated with a 1-way analysis of variance technique.34
Each continuous variable was transformed to natural logarithmic equivalents
to reduce positive skewing prior to analysis. If the range of the variable
to be logarithmically transformed included zero, 1 was added to the variable
prior to transformation. When logarithmic data transformation did not result
in a near normal distribution, such as for dust mite concentrations, a Kruskal-Wallis
test was used. The number of children in each analysis varied slightly because
of missing data.
Atopy and seroatopy were each used as a dependent binary variable in
a linear logistic regression assessing 2 indicator variables for pet exposure:
exposure to 1 dog or cat or exposure to 2 or more dogs or cats.35
These models were fitted without other variables and with other potentially
confounding variables, including cord serum IgE concentration, child's sex,
having older siblings, parental smoking, mother or father with a history of
asthma, and total bedroom dust mite allergen levels at child age 2 years.
The logistic model is appropriate for modeling binary dependent variables.
It makes minimal assumptions about the distributional properties of the independent
variables and the exponentiation of the coefficient allows for estimation
of the odds ratio (OR). We chose to include the number of dogs and cats as
2 binary indicator variables avoiding assumptions concerning the direction
of any associations. Using the Hosmer-Lemeshow test, we found no evidence
to doubt the validity of the models.35
We analyzed the entire data set and data sets defined by sex. In all
analyses, an α = .05 criteria was used to determine statistical significance.
There was no attempt to impute data; all analyses were performed on all available
data. SAS v8.0 (SAS Institute Inc, Cary, NC) was used for all analyses.36
A total of 1194 pregnant women were potentially eligible for entry into
this study, and consent for participation was obtained from 953 women. Infants
of 106 of these women were not enrolled in the study because a cord blood
sample was not obtained, leaving 847 eligible newborns. Six of the cord blood
samples were thought to be contaminated by maternal blood,21,23
and an additional 6 children were found to be ineligible when each child's
data were examined and verified prior to the 6- to 7-year evaluations, yielding
835 eligible children enrolled at birth. Of the 835 children initially enrolled,
235 had been lost to follow-up by age 6 years, and 126 of those contacted
at age 6 years declined participation in the clinical evaluation. Thus, 474
(57%) of the 835 eligible children initially enrolled completed the clinical
evaluation for allergic sensitization and asthma at an average age of 6.7
(SD, 0.17) years. Characteristics of children who were evaluated at 6 to 7
years did not differ significantly from those of children who did not undergo
clinical evaluation at age 6 to 7 years, including whether the parent had
a history of asthma or hay fever or whether there were dogs or cats in the
household (Table 1). Also, interactions
between each variable, any exposure to dogs and cats in the home in the first
year of life, and whether the child participated in the clinical evaluation
were not statistically significant (Table
1). When the relationship between maternal and paternal histories
of asthma, allergies, and hay fever and presence of 2 or more dogs or cats
in the household was evaluated, no significant associations were found.
The parents of the children in this study were relatively well educated
and almost all (804 [96.3%]) described themselves as white, non-Hispanic.
Characteristics of the children completing the study are presented in Table 2. Boys and girls were approximately
equally represented. The presence of a dog or cat in the home did not differ
significantly between parents with a history of asthma, allergies, or hay
fever and those who did not report these conditions.
To investigate the relationships between dog and cat exposure and allergic
sensitization, we initially compared the 184 children with any dog exposure
in their first year of life to the 220 children without either dog or cat
exposure. Children exposed to a dog were less likely to have a positive skin
test result to dog allergen (3.3% vs 8.6%, P = .03)
and detectable dog-specific IgE (3.7% vs 8.7%, P
= .06) at follow-up. Any exposure to a dog was also associated with lower
total serum IgE levels (geometric mean, 23.8 IU/mL vs 33.1 IU/mL for no dog
or cat exposure; P = .04). The inverse association
between dog exposure and allergic sensitization was further examined in relationship
to number of dogs (Table 3). The
reference group remained the 220 children with neither dog nor cat exposure
in the first year of life. An apparent dose-response effect for atopy and
seroatopy was found across the 3 exposure categories. Atopy was present in
33.6% of the children without dog or cat exposure, in 29.7% with exposure
to 1 dog, and in only 8.3% with exposure to 2 or more dogs (P = .009). The prevalence of seroatopy was 38.5% with no pet exposure,
36.7% with exposure to 1 dog, and 12.9% with exposure to 2 or more dogs (P = .02). The same analyses were performed with cat exposure
during the first year. Using the same reference group of 220 unexposed children,
the patterns toward less prevalent allergic sensitization with exposure to
cats were similar to those observed with dog exposure, but none of the associations
reached statistical significance. For example, the prevalence of atopy declined
from 33.6% to 31.4% to 23.1% with exposure to no dogs or cats, 1 cat, or 2
or more cats, respectively (P = .54 for comparison
across categories), while seroatopy declined from 38.5% to 34.5% to 26.1%
(P = .45).
Based on finding similar relationships between dog and cat exposures
in the first year of life and allergic sensitization at age 6 to 7 years,
relationships were further analyzed by simultaneously considering combined
dog and cat exposure. Combining dogs and cats increased the number of children
in each category, allowing further exploration of the relationships through
stratification of the data by sex. Atopy and seroatopy were each present in
about one third of children. When all children were considered, the prevalence
of skin prick test positivity to dog allergen, outdoor and indoor allergens,
atopy, and seroatopy were significantly different across the 3 exposure categories
and generally decreased with increasing pet exposure (Table 4). The pattern of decreasing skin prick test positivity to
cat allergen with increasing exposure was similar but the relationship(s)
failed to reach statistical significance.
When boys and girls were considered separately, different patterns emerged
from the data (Table 4). Exposure
to a single dog or cat in the first year of life was associated with an increased
prevalence of atopy and seroatopy in girls while both outcomes declined in
boys exposed to a single dog or cat. Lower prevalences of skin prick test
positivity to dog, cat, and indoor and outdoor allergens, and of methacholine
airway responsiveness were consistently found in association with exposure
to a single dog or cat with boys but not with girls. Measurements of lung
function were also related to dog and cat exposure for boys but not for girls.
The prevalence of methacholine airway responsiveness in boys was 25.5% when
there had been no dog or cat exposure, 20.3% with exposure to 1 dog or cat,
and 5.1% with exposure to 2 or more dogs or cats (P
= .03). In girls, the prevalence of methacholine responsiveness was unchanged
across pet exposure categories. Similarly, the mean percent predicted FVC
and FEV1 increased significantly across pet exposure categories
among boys but not among girls, and were highest with exposure to 2 or more
dogs or cats. Thirty-three (7%) of 473 children had current asthma. The prevalence
of current asthma was lower in boys who had been exposed to 2 or more dogs
or cats in infancy compared with no exposure (5.1% vs 11.8%, respectively),
but the difference across exposure categories was not statistically significant
(P = .43) and no difference was seen for girls.
For all children, exposure to 2 or more dogs and cats in the first year
of life was associated with a lower total serum IgE at age 6 to 7 years, but
the analysis across exposure categories was not statistically significant
(P = .09) (Table
5). In analyses stratified by sex, exposure to more dogs or cats
was associated with significantly decreased geometric mean IgE among boys
(P = .02) and among children with a parental history
of asthma (P = .03), but not among girls (P = .42) or among children without a parental history of asthma (P = .31).
When mean total dust mite concentrations in the child's bedroom at age
2 years were compared for homes with no pet, 1 dog or cat, and 2 or more dogs
or cats, the median (5th percentile, 95th percentile) dust mite concentrations
were not significantly different (2.0 [0.5, 37.1] µg/g dust; 1.8 [0.5,
16.6] µg/g dust; 1.1 [0.5, 43.8] µg/g dust; P = .27).
Logistic regression analysis was used to adjust for the effects of possible
confounding variables (cord serum IgE concentration, levels of house dust
mite allergen in the child's bedroom at age 2 years, child's sex, an older
sibling, passive exposure to parental tobacco smoke, and parental history
of asthma) on relationships between dog and cat exposure and risks of atopy
and seroatopy. After adjusting for all of these variables, exposure to 2 or
more dogs or cats was still associated with significantly lower risks of atopy
(OR, 0.31; 95% confidence interval [CI], 0.14-0.72) and seroatopy (OR, 0.43;
95% CI, 0.19-0.96) in all children (Table
6, model 1).
When the analysis was further adjusted for current exposure to dogs
or cats using the same exposure categories as in the first year of life (no
exposure; exposure to 1 dog or cat; exposure to 2 or more dogs or cats at
age 6-7 years), the risk of atopy and seroatopy associated with exposure in
the first year of life remained significantly decreased (atopy: OR, 0.23;
95% CI, 0.09-0.60; seroatopy: OR, 0.33; 95% CI, 0.13-0.83) (Table 6, model 2). When the variable for dog or cat exposure at
age 6 to 7 years replaced the variable for exposure in the first year of life,
no statistically significant associations were found. For example, with all
children included in the analysis, risks of atopy and seroatopy among children
with 2 or more dogs or cats at age 6 to 7 years were not significantly different
than those among children with no current pet exposure (for atopy: OR, 0.79;
95% CI, 0.44-1.85; P = .79; for seroatopy: OR, 0.81;
95% CI, 0.43-1.94; P = .81).
In this prospective study we found that exposure to 2 or more dogs or
cats in the first year of life was associated with a lower prevalence of allergic
sensitization at age 6 to 7 years regardless of exposure to dogs and cats
at age 6 years. This inverse relationship was consistent whether skin prick
tests for 6 common aeroallergens or tests for 7 allergen-specific IgE concentrations
were considered as primary outcomes. The inverse relationship was present
for both indoor (dust mites, dog, and cat) and outdoor (ragweed, grass, and Alternaria) allergens. The relationships remained significant
after adjusting for a number of variables that may be risk factors for allergic
sensitization or that could have been associated with pet ownership, including
cord serum IgE concentration, house dust mite exposure, older siblings, parental
smoking, and parental history of asthma.4,37-42
Other studies have also reported lower prevalences of allergic sensitization
or symptoms related to allergic diseases in association with early exposure
to dogs and cats,13,15,16,43,44
but a systematic review of the literature concerning this question concluded
that previous exposure to dogs and cats increased the risk of asthma and wheezing
in children older than 6 years.45 The conclusions
of this systematic review differ from the results of 2 large prospective birth
cohort studies.43,44 Nafstad et
al43 found that after using logistic regression
to adjust for potential confounders, being exposed to pets in early life reduced
the risk of asthma (OR, 0.7; 95% CI, 0.5-1.1) and allergic rhinitis (OR, 0.6;
95% CI, 0.4-1.0) in a birth cohort of 2531 children followed to age 4 years.
In a birth cohort of 1246 children in Arizona followed up to age 13 years,
Remes et al44 reported that children who had
1 or more dogs in the home at birth were significantly less likely to develop
frequent wheeze than children without early dog exposure, but neither early
exposure to dogs or to cats was associated with skin prick test positivity
or total serum IgE concentrations. Remes et al did not find a difference between
children exposed to 1 dog compared with those exposed to 2 or more dogs. They
also found that the inverse relationship between dog exposure and frequent
wheeze was predominantly among children without a parental history of asthma.
Reasons for the differences in allergic sensitization outcomes between our
study and the study by Remes et al are not clear, but may include differences
in climate where the birth cohorts were located and differences in keeping
pets inside the home.
In a recent cross-sectional study in children by Platts-Mills et al,18 and also in a subsequent study in adults,46 a bell-shaped dose-response relationship between
cat allergen exposure and cat-specific sensitization was observed. Decreased
levels of cat-specific sensitization were associated with both the lowest
and the highest cat allergen exposure groups. Platts-Mills and colleagues
also found that cat-specific IgG antibody levels increased with increasing
cat exposure, and were highest in the highest cat exposure group. They suggested
that high levels of cat allergen exposure induced a modified T helper cell
type 2 (TH2) response with production of cat allergen–specific
IgG and IgG4 antibodies without allergic sensitization. This interesting hypothesis
is not entirely consistent with the data presented in our study because we
found that allergen-specific IgE antibodies to dust mites, ragweed, and grass
(allergens unrelated to dog and cat) were also less prevalent in children
exposed to dogs and cats in the first year of life.
Other researchers have suggested that the protective effect of dogs
and cats is not related to allergen exposure but rather to increased exposure
to bacterial endotoxin associated with household pets.15,47
Endotoxin exposure is hypothesized to shift the developing immune system away
from a TH2-type pattern of response, which favors development of
allergic sensitization, toward a TH1-type response. Studies in
animals have shown that concomitant exposure to endotoxin and allergen will
prevent allergic sensitization normally induced by the allergen.48
Recent studies have shown that endotoxin levels in homes are inversely related
to TH2-type cytokine production by lymphocytes of children residing
in the homes and that the presence of household dogs is related to higher
levels of indoor endotoxin.47,49
Our data are consistent with the hypothesis that exposure to 2 or more dogs
or cats, and therefore exposure to higher levels of endotoxin, is associated
with a TH1 pattern of immune response and less allergic sensitization.
There were several associations with dog and cat exposure that were
evident for boys but not for girls, including lower total serum IgE concentrations,
lower prevalence of methacholine airway responsiveness, and better lung function.
These differences in associations between boys and girls are puzzling, but
others have also observed differences between boys and girls in factors related
to asthma.50-52
Consistent with the pattern of the results of total serum IgE concentrations,
methacholine airway responsiveness, and FEV1 in boys, the prevalence
of asthma was also lower in those boys exposed to 2 or more dogs or cats compared
with those who were unexposed (5.1% vs 11.8%). This difference was not statistically
significant across pet exposure categories; however, only 39 boys were exposed
to 2 or more dogs or cats in the first year of life. Assuming prevalences
that we found in our cohort, a study designed to detect a statistically significant
difference in the prevalence of asthma among boys exposed to 2 or more dogs
or cats would have required a final cohort of at least 1327 children followed
up to age 6 to 7 years.
An important strength of this study is the prospective design using
a population-based cohort of children followed yearly from birth. The prevalences
of allergic sensitization, methacholine airway responsiveness, and asthma
found in our cohort were similar to those reported by others studying children
of similar ages.53-55
The mean values for total serum IgE were also similar to those reported by
others.55,56 Animal exposure was
ascertained when the child was 1 year old, not years later.10,14,15
Assessing animal exposure prior to assessing outcomes reduces concern of misclassification
of exposure and recall bias. Information on other factors potentially related
to risk of allergic sensitization, most importantly family history, was collected,
allowing adjustment for the potential confounding effects of these other variables.57 Another strength is the multiple objectively measured
outcomes.10,14 The association
between pet exposure and less allergic sensitization was found with both in
vivo (skin prick test reactivity) and in vitro (allergen-specific IgE levels)
tests. The persons performing the skin prick tests, allergen-specific IgE
tests, spirometry, and methacholine challenges were unaware of study hypotheses
at the time the tests were performed, making systematic measurement bias unlikely.
Bronchial hyperresponsiveness is frequently stated to be a major component
of asthma that can be objectively measured.58-60
Our findings of reduced methacholine airway responsiveness in boys with exposure
to 2 or more dogs or cats suggest that exposure to dogs and cats may be associated
with a reduced risk of asthma, at least in boys. While we did not find a statistically
significant association for current asthma in this study, the prevalence of
asthma in boys exposed to 2 or more dogs or cats was 57% lower than in unexposed
boys, a difference that would likely be significant in a larger population.
There are limitations to our study. As with most prospective studies,
some children did not complete the entire study. However, we found no important
differences between children examined at age 6 to 7 years and those who were
not examined. In addition, we could not detect differences in the relationship
between dog and cat ownership and parental history of asthma, allergy, or
hay fever among those examined and not examined. A second caveat is the limited
racial, socio-economic, and geographic diversity of our study population,
suggesting that our conclusions can only be applied to similar populations
of white children. Since our follow-up was limited to an average age of 6.7
years, we do not know if the associations we found will persist as the children
grow older, but others have found that the association between dog and cat
exposure and a lower risk of allergy-related symptoms persisted to age 12
to 13 years.15,44 Sample size
is another limitation of the study. A larger sample would have allowed more
reliable estimates and detailed examinations of the differences between boys
and girls and between children with and without parental histories of asthma.
A final caveat is that we did not consider exposure to dogs and cats outside
the child's home.
In this prospective study designed to examine multiple potential risk
factors for allergic sensitization, we found that exposure to 2 or more dogs
or cats in the first year of life was associated with a significantly lower
probability of subsequent allergic sensitization to common aeroallergens.
Exposure to 2 or more dogs or cats was also associated with significantly
lower serum IgE concentration, less methacholine airway responsiveness, and
better lung function in boys but not in girls. The association between pet
exposure and decreased prevalence of allergic sensitization remained unchanged
after adjustment for potentially confounding variables. These findings suggest
that exposure to more than 1 dog or cat in the first year of life may reduce
a child's risk of allergic disease.
1.Mannino DM, Homa DM, Akinbami LJ, Moorman JE, Gwynn C, Redd SC. Surveillance for asthma—United States, 1980-1999.
Mor Mortal Wkly Rep CDC Surveill Summ.2002;51:1-13.Google Scholar 2.Yunginger JW, Reed CE, O'Connell EJ, Melton III LJ, O'Fallon WM, Silverstein MD. A community-based study of the epidemiology of asthma: incidence rates,
1964-1983.
Am Rev Respir Dis.1992;146:888-894.Google Scholar 3.Sears MR, Burrows B, Flannery EM, Herbison GP, Holdaway MD. Atopy in childhood, I: gender and allergen related risks for development
of hay fever and asthma.
Clin Exp Allergy.1993;23:941-948.Google Scholar 4.Platts-Mills TA, Vervloet D, Thomas WR, Aalberse RC, Chapman MD. Indoor allergens and asthma: report of the third international workshop.
J Allergy Clin Immunol.1997;100:S2-S24.Google Scholar 5.Litonjua AA, Sparrow D, Weiss ST, O'Connor GT, Long AA, Ohman Jr JL. Sensitization to cat allergen is associated with asthma in older men
and predicts new-onset airway hyperresponsiveness.
Am J Respir Crit Care Med.1997;156:23-27.Google Scholar 6.Sporik R, Ingram JM, Price W, Sussman JH, Honsinger RW, Platts-Mills TA. Association of asthma with serum IgE and skin test reactivity to allergens
among children living at high altitude: tickling the dragon's breath.
Am J Respir Crit Care Med.1995;151:1388-1392.Google Scholar 7.Von Mutius E. The environmental predictors of allergic disease.
J Allergy Clin Immunol.2000;105:9-19.Google Scholar 8.Popp W, Rauscher H, Sertl K, Wanke T, Zwick H. Risk factors for sensitization to furred pets.
Allergy.1990;45:75-79.Google Scholar 9.Steerenberg PA, van JGC, Vanderbriel RJ, Vos JG, van Bree L, van Loveren H. Environmental and lifestyle factors may act in concert to increase
the prevalence of respiratory allergy including asthma.
Clin Exp Allergy.1999;29:1303-1308.Google Scholar 10.Suoniemi I, Björkstén F, Haahtela T. Dependence of immediate hypersensitivity in the adolescent period on
factors encountered in infancy.
Allergy.1981;36:263-268.Google Scholar 11.Vanto T, Koivikko A. Dog hypersensitivity in asthmatic children.
Acta Paediatr Scand.1983;72:571-575.Google Scholar 12.Lindfors A, van Hage-Hamsten M, Rietz H, Wickman M, Nordvall SL. Influence of interaction of environmental risk factors and sensitization
in young asthmatic children.
J Allergy Clin Immunol.1999;104:755-762.Google Scholar 13.Burr ML, Merrett TG, Dunstan FDJ, Maguire MJ. The development of allergy in high-risk children.
Clin Exp Allergy.1997;27:1247-1253.Google Scholar 14.Braback L, Breborowicz A, Julge K.
et al. Risk factors for respiratory symptoms and atopic sensitisation in the
Baltic area.
Arch Dis Child.1995;72:487-493.Google Scholar 15.Hesselmar B, Äberg N, Äberg B, Eriksson B, Björkstén B. Does early exposure to cat or dog protect against later allergy development?
Clin Exp Allergy.1999;29:611-617.Google Scholar 16.Reijonen TM, Kotaniemi-Syrjanen A, Korhonen K, Korppi M. Predictors of asthma three years after hospital admission for wheezing
in infancy.
Pediatrics.2000;106:1406-1412.Google Scholar 17.Roost H-P, Kunzli N, Schindler C.
et al. Role of current and childhood exposure to cat and atopic sensitization.
J Allergy Clin Immunol.1999;104:941-947.Google Scholar 18.Platts-Mills T, Vaughan J, Squillace S, Woodfolk J, Sporik R. Sensitization, asthma, and a modified Th2 response in children exposed
to cat allergen: a population-based cross-sectional study.
Lancet.2001;357:752-756.Google Scholar 19.Riedler J, Eder W, Oberfeld G, Schreuer M. Austrian children living on a farm have less hay fever, asthma and
allergic sensitization.
Clin Exp Allergy.2000;30:194-200.Google Scholar 20.Ernst P, Cormier Y. Relative scarcity of asthma and atopy among rural adolescents raised
on a farm.
Am J Respir Crit Care Med.2000;161:1563-1566.Google Scholar 21.Ownby DR, Johnson CC, Peterson EL. Maternal smoking does not influence cord serum IgE or IgD concentrations.
J Allergy Clin Immunol.1991;88:555-560.Google Scholar 22.Johnson CC, Ownby DR, Peterson EL. Parental history of atopic disease and concentration of cord blood
IgE.
Clin Exp Allergy.1996;26:624-629.Google Scholar 23.Ownby DR, McCullough J, Johnson CC, Peterson EL. Evaluation of IgA measurements as a method for detecting maternal blood
contamination of cord blood samples.
Pediatr Allergy Immunol.1996;7:125-129.Google Scholar 24.Joseph CLM, Ownby DR, Peterson EL, Johnson CC. Racial differences in physiologic parameters related to asthma among
middle-class children.
Chest.2000;117:1336-1344.Google Scholar 25.Ownby DR, Johnson CC, Peterson EL. Incidence and prevalence of physician-diagnosed asthma in a suburban
population of young adults.
Ann Allergy Asthma Immunol.1996;77:304-308.Google Scholar 26.Peterson EL, Johnson CC, Ownby DR. Use of urinary cotinine and questionnaires in the evaluation of infant
exposure to tobacco smoke in epidemiologic studies.
J Clin Epidemiol.1997;50:917-923.Google Scholar 27.Johnson CC, Peterson EL, Ownby DR. Gender differences in total and allergen-specific immunoglobulin E
(IgE) concentrations in a population-based cohort from birth to age four years.
Am J Epidemiol.1998;147:1145-1152.Google Scholar 28.Peterson EL, Ownby DR, Kallenbach L, Johnson CC. Evaluation of air and dust sampling schemes for Fel d 1, Der f 1, and
Der p 1 allergens in home in the Detroit area.
J Allergy Clin Immunol.1999;104:348-355.Google Scholar 29.Ownby DR, Peterson EL, Johnson CC. Factors related to methacholine airway responsiveness in children.
Am J Respir Crit Care Med.2000;161:1578-1583.Google Scholar 30.Ownby DR, Johnson CC, Peterson EL. Passive cigarette smoke exposure of infants.
Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med.2000;154:1237-1241.Google Scholar 31.Rosenstreich DL, Eggleston PA, Kattan M.
et al. The role of cockroach allergy and exposure to cockroach allergen in
causing morbidity among inner-city children with asthma.
N Engl J Med.1997;336:1356-1363.Google Scholar 32.Cohen J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. Revised ed. New York, NY: Academic Press; 1977.
33.Everitt BS. The Analysis of Contigency Tables. London, England: Chapman & Hall; 1977.
34.Neter J, Wasserman W, Kutner MH. Applied Statistical Models: Regression, Analysis
of Variance, and Experimental Design. 3rd ed. Boston, Mass: Irwin; 1990.
35.Hosmer DW, Lemeshow S. Applied Logistic Regression. New York, NY: Wiley & Sons; 1989.
36.SAS Institute Inc. SAS/STAT Users Guide. Version 8 ed. Cary, NC: SAS Institute Inc; 1999.
37.Bergmann RL, Edenharter G, Bergmann KC.
et al. Predictability of early atopy by cord blood-IgE and parental history.
Clin Exp Allergy.1997;27:752-760.Google Scholar 38.Kjellman NIM, Croner S. Cord blood IgE determination for allergy prediction: a follow-up to
seven years of age in 1,651 children.
Ann Allergy.1984;53:167-171.Google Scholar 39.Martinez FD, Antognoni G, Macri F.
et al. Parental smoking enhances bronchial responsiveness in nine-year-old
children.
Am Rev Respir Dis.1988;138:518-523.Google Scholar 40.Stoddard JJ, Miller T. Impact of parental smoking on the prevalence of wheezing respiratory
illness in children.
Am J Epidemiol.1995;141:96-102.Google Scholar 41.Sporik R, Holgate ST, Platts-Mills TA, Cogswell JJ. Exposure to house-dust mite allergen
(Der p I)
and the development of asthma in childhood.
N Engl J Med.1990;323:502-507.Google Scholar 42.Ball TM, Castro-Rodriguez JA, Griffith KA, Holberg CJ, Martinez FD, Wright AL. Siblings, day-care attendance, and the risk of asthma and wheezing
during childhood.
N Engl J Med.2000;343:538-543.Google Scholar 43.Nafstad P, Magnus P, Gaarder PI, Jaakkola JJK. Exposure to pets and atopy-related diseases in the first 4 years of
life.
Allergy.2001;56:307-312.Google Scholar 44.Remes ST, Castro-Rodriguez JA, Holberg CJ, Martinez FD, Wright AL. Dog exposure in infancy decreases the subsequent risk of frequent wheeze
but not atopy.
J Allergy Clin Immunol.2001;108:509-515.Google Scholar 45.Apelberg BJ, Aoki Y, Jaakkola JJK. Systematic review: exposure to pets and risk of asthma and asthma-like
symptoms.
J Allergy Clin Immunol.2001;107:455-460.Google Scholar 46.Custovic A, Hallam CL, Simpson BM, Craven M, Simpson A, Woodcock A. Decreased prevalence of sensitization to cats with high exposure to
cat allergen.
J Allergy Clin Immunol.2001;108:537-539.Google Scholar 47.Gereda JE, Leung DYM, Thatayatikom A.
et al. Relation between house-dust endotoxin exposure, type 1 T-cell development,
and allergen sensitisation in infants at high risk of asthma.
Lancet.2000;355:1680-1683.Google Scholar 48.Gold DR, Burge HA, Carey V, Milton DK, Platts-Mills T, Weiss S. Predictors of repeated wheeze in the first year of life: the relative
roles of cockroach, birth weight, acute lower respiratory illness, and maternal
smoking.
Am J Respir Crit Care Med.1999;160:227-236.Google Scholar 49.Park J-H, Gold DR, Spiegelman DL, Burge HA, Milton DK. House dust endotoxin and wheeze in the first year of life.
Am J Respir Crit Care Med.2001;163:322-328.Google Scholar 50.Horwood LJ, Fergusson DM, Hons BA, Shannon FT. Social and familial factors in the development of early childhood asthma.
Pediatrics.1985;75:859-868.Google Scholar 51.Skobeloff EM, Spivey WH, St Clair SS, Schoffstal JM. The influence of age and sex on asthma admissions.
JAMA.1992;268:3437-3440.Google Scholar 52.Osborne ML, Vollmer WM, Linton KLP, Buist AS. Characteristics of patients with asthma within a large HMO: a comparison by age and gender.
Am J Respir Crit Care Med.1998;157:123-128.Google Scholar 53.Peat JK, Salome CM, Sedgwick CS, Kerrebijn J, Woolcock AJ. A prospective study of bronchial hyperresponsiveness and respiratory
symptoms in a population of Australian school children.
Clin Exp Allergy.1989;19:299-306.Google Scholar 54.Peat JK, Britton WJ, Salome CM, Woolcock AJ. Bronchial hyperresponsiveness in two populations of Australian schoolchildren,
III: effect of exposure to environmental allergens.
Clin Allergy.1987;17:291-300.Google Scholar 55.Wright AL, Holberg CJ, Martinez FD, Halonen M, Morgan W, Taussig LM. Epidemiology of physician-diagnosed allergic rhinitis in childhood.
Pediatrics.1994;94:895-901.Google Scholar 56.Backer V, Ulrik CS, Wendelboe D.
et al. Distribution of serum IgE in children and adolescents aged 7 to 16
years in Copenhagen, in relation to factors of importance.
Allergy.1992;47:484-489.Google Scholar 57.Withers NJ, Low L, Holgate ST, Clough JB. The natural history of respiratory symptoms in a cohort of adolescents.
Am J Respir Crit Care Med.1998;158:352-357.Google Scholar 58.Avital A, Noviski N, Bar-Yishay E, Springer C, Levy M, Godfrey S. Nonspecific bronchial reactivity in asthmatic children depends on severity
but not on age.
Am Rev Respir Dis.1991;144:36-38.Google Scholar 59.Galdès-Sebaldt M, McLaughlin FJ, Levison H. Comparison of cold air, ultrasonic mist, and methacholine inhalations
as tests of bronchial reactivity in normal and asthmatic children.
J Pediatr.1985;107:526-530.Google Scholar 60.Takeda K, Shibasaki M, Takita H. Relation between bronchial responsiveness to methacholine and levels
of IgE antibody against
Dermatophagoides farinae
and serum IgE in asthmatic children.
Clin Exp Allergy.1993;23:450-454.Google Scholar