Context Evidence consistently indicates that depression has adversely affected
work productivity. Estimates of the cost impact in lost labor time in the
US workforce, however, are scarce and dated.
Objective To estimate the impact of depression on labor costs (ie, work absence
and reduced performance while at work) in the US workforce.
Design, Setting, and Participants All employed individuals who participated in the American Productivity
Audit (conducted August 1, 2001–July 31, 2002) between May 20 and July
11, 2002, were eligible for the Depressive Disorders Study. Those who responded
affirmatively to 2 depression-screening questions (n = 692), as well as a
1:4 stratified random sample of those responding in the negative (n = 435),
were recruited for and completed a supplemental interview using the Primary
Care Evaluation of Mental Disorders Mood Module for depression, the Somatic
Symptom Inventory, and a medical and treatment history for depression. Excess
lost productive time (LPT) costs from depression were derived as the difference
in LPT among individuals with depression minus the expected LPT in the absence
of depression projected to the US workforce.
Main Outcome Measure Estimated LPT and associated labor costs (work absence and reduced performance
while at work) due to depression.
Results Workers with depression reported significantly more total health-related
LPT than those without depression (mean, 5.6 h/wk vs an expected 1.5 h/wk,
respectively). Eighty-one percent of the LPT costs are explained by reduced
performance while at work. Major depression accounts for 48% of the LPT among
those with depression, again with a majority of the cost explained by reduced
performance while at work. Self-reported use of antidepressants in the previous
12 months among those with depression was low (<30%) and the mean reported
treatment effectiveness was only moderate. Extrapolation of these survey results
and self-reported annual incomes to the population of US workers suggests
that US workers with depression employed in the previous week cost employers
an estimated $44 billion per year in LPT, an excess of $31 billion per year
compared with peers without depression. This estimate does not include labor
costs associated with short- and long-term disability.
Conclusions A majority of the LPT costs that employers face from employee depression
is invisible and explained by reduced performance while at work. Use of treatments
for depression appears to be relatively low. The combined LPT burden among
those with depression and the low level of treatment suggests that there may
be cost-effective opportunities for improving depression-related outcomes
in the US workforce.
Health conditions that affect ability to work are costly to employers
(unpublished data; an electronic manuscript, Lost Productive
Work Time Costs From Health Conditions in the US: Results From the American
Productivity Audit, is available from W.F.S. on request). Evidence
consistently indicates that common conditions including migraine,1-8 low
back pain,9,10 diabetes,11,12 allergic rhinitis,5,13-18 gastroesophageal
reflux,19-21 and
depression5,10,22-25 dominate
health-related lost labor time costs. Among these, depression is among the
most costly because it is highly prevalent and comorbid with other conditions.
Furthermore, although workers with depression are usually present at work,
their performance can be substantially reduced.
Model-based estimates indicate that depression costs US employers $24
billion annually in lost productive work time.23 However,
some notable limitations challenge the relevance of this and other estimates.
Using a human capital approach, this model makes important assumptions regarding
the prevalence of depression in the workforce, the duration of depressive
episodes, their imputed impact on productive time at work, and the cost to
employers. Furthermore, although stated in 1990 terms, the cost estimate is
based on data collected in the early to mid-1980s. The management and treatment
of depression has changed substantially since the 1980s; use of pharmaceutical
care and, more generally, access to care have increased26 and
may have influenced disability status and how work time is lost.
The American Productivity Audit was initiated to more directly understand
the relation between health and lost productive time (LPT) in the US workforce
(W.F.S., unpublished data). We describe the results of a supplemental study
to the productivity audit that focused on depression in the US workforce.
The productivity audit was completed using the Work and Health Interview
(Ricci et al27 and unpublished data; 2 electronic
manuscripts, The Work and Health Interview and Validation of the Work and Health Phone Interview, are
available from W.F.S. on request). A supplemental study, the Depressive Disorders
Study, was conducted in a random sample of audit participants to more accurately
estimate the LPT costs due to depression.
Work and Health Interview
The Work and Health Interview, a computer-assisted telephone interview,
captures data on work absence, reduced performance while at work, and health-related
causes. The recall period is 2 weeks. The interview comprises 8 modules. The
first 3 capture detailed data on employment status, occupation and usual work
time, and the presence of 22 different health conditions. The health assessment
includes 1 of 2 depression screening questions (ie, "In the past 2 weeks,
did you feel sad, blue, or down in the dumps?") that were used to identify
random samples of individuals with and without probable depression for the
Depressive Disorders Study. A module for missed days of work captured missed
workdays and the related cause. A module for job visualization asked about
activities performed at work and about job demand and control.28 The
module for LPT on days at work asked about missed hours and reduced performance
on workdays and the related cause. The demographics module gathered additional
information, including annual salary.
Health-related LPT, described in detail elsewhere (Ricci et al27 and unpublished data, available from W.F.S. on request),
was defined as the sum of hours per week absent from work for a health-related
reason ("absenteeism") and the hour-equivalents per week of health-related
reduced performance on workdays ("presenteeism"). Absenteeism was calculated
as the sum of missed workdays (ie, multiplied by average number of hours worked
per day) and reduced work hours on workdays (ie, late start, early departure,
or missed time during the workday) during the recall period. Presenteeism
was defined as reduced work performance during the recall period. It was quantified
by responses to 6 questions on specific work behaviors.
For 5 of the 6 questions, respondents were asked how often on average
during the recall period they lost concentration, repeated a job, worked more
slowly than usual, felt fatigued at work, and did nothing at work on days
when they were at work not feeling well. Responses were "all of the time,"
"most of the time," "half of the time," "some of the time," and "none of the
time." A sixth question asked respondents about the average amount of time
it took them to start working after arriving at work on days not feeling well
during the recall period. The aggregate measure of reduced performance was
then derived in 4 steps: (1) convert the categorical response options for
5 of the 6 questions into percentages as follows: all of the time (100%),
most of the time (75%), half of the time (50%), some of the time (25%), and
none of the time (0%); (2) average the responses to the 5 categorical behavior
questions to yield the average percentage of lost productive work time and
multiply this percentage by the number of hours worked per day to yield its
hour equivalent; (3) add the hours of lost productive work time to the reported
average amount of time it took to start working after arriving at work; and
(4) divide by the number of weeks per recall period for the hours per week
of LPT on days at work.
American Productivity Audit
The productivity audit, the parent survey for the Depressive Disorders
Study, is a national survey of the US population, with 30 523 interviews
completed between August 1, 2001, and July 31, 2002 (W.F.S., unpublished data).
The Depressive Disorders Study and all related estimates are based on the
subsample of 3351 productivity audit interviews completed between May 20 and
July 11, 2002, and the 1190 individuals selected from this subsample to complete
the supplemental interview.
Audit households were selected as a random sample of residences within
the continental United States with a telephone and at least 1 permanent adult
(ie, aged 18-65 years) resident. Residents who reported affirmatively to the
Current Population Survey (CPS)29 question
on employment status (ie, "Last week, did you do any work for either pay or
profit?"), and a 10% random sample of those who responded in the negative,
were invited to participate. Up to 2 eligible members were interviewed per
household. Oral informed consent was obtained from each participant before
initiating the interview. Audit participation was 66.2%.
A 2-step weighting method accounted for selective participation. One
weight was applied to individuals as the inverse of the number of phone numbers
available for incoming calls to account for the unequal probability of selecting
households. Second, a population weighting adjustment accounted for selection
bias due to incomplete coverage of the US population and ensured that estimates
of certain sample demographic subgroups' totals conformed to known values.
The CPS was used as the external reference database because it provides high-quality
data on a nationally representative sample of the US workforce. A raking method
was used for population weighting adjustment, benchmarking to 4 variables
common to both the productivity audit and the CPS. Raking uses an iterative
proportional fitting procedure to ensure that the weights assigned to individual
respondents lead to marginal distributions on auxiliary variables that are
equivalent to the CPS.30 Wesvar version 4 statistical
software (Westat, Rockville, Md), was used to perform the raking adjustments.
The Depressive Disorders Study
Figure 1 describes participant
identification and selection for the Depressive Disorders Study. Between May
20 and July 11, 2002, 3351 audit participants were asked 2 questions: "In
the past 2 weeks, did you feel sad, blue, or down in the dumps?" and "In the
past 2 weeks, did you have little interest or pleasure in doing things?" All
participants who responded affirmatively to at least 1 of these 2 questions
were invited to participate (n = 865) in the Depressive Disorders Study. A
group-matched stratified (by age, sex, occupation eligibility, and date of
interview) random sample (1:4) of those reporting "no" to both questions was
also invited to participate (n = 602). A total of 733 (692 met employment
criteria) respondents who screened positive and 457 (435 met employment criteria)
who screened negative completed the extended interview. Respondents received
a $10 incentive. Participation in the extended interview was 86%. The study
protocol and informed consent statement were approved by the Essex institutional
review board, Lebanon, NJ.
The extended interview included the Primary Care Evaluation of Mental
Disorders (PRIME-MD) Mood Module31 along with
the module for most recent depressive episode (ie, time since last depressive
episode and the duration of episode). The PRIME-MD is a validated diagnostic
interview. The mood module contained 9 items to identify individuals with
depression using Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders, Revised Third Edition (DSM-III-R)
criteria.32 Subsequently, the 26-item Somatic
Symptom Inventory (SSI)33 was administered.
For each of 26 physical symptoms, respondents reported the extent to which
they were bothered by each symptom in the past month ("not at all," "a little
bit," "moderately," "quite a bit," and "a great deal"). The association between
physical symptoms and depression was assessed by calculating the prevalence
of depression for each symptom cluster and for respondents who did not meet
criteria for any symptom cluster. Finally, 11 questions were asked about medical
care and treatment for depression (ie, frequency of talking with a physician
about depression, physician's diagnosis, whether or not a medication was prescribed,
which medication was prescribed, use and effectiveness of the medication,
and rating of medication adverse effects).
PRIME-MD diagnostic criteria based on the DSM-III-R were used to assign the diagnosis of a specific depressive disorder.
A total of 29.8% (n = 206) of those who screened positive for depression met
diagnostic criteria for major depressive disorder, dysthymia, or partial remission
or recurrence of major depressive disorder. Only 3.0% (n = 13) of those who
answered in the negative to both screening questions met diagnostic criteria
for depression. All 13 met criteria for dysthymia and 1 also met criteria
for major depressive disorder.
Clustering of physical symptoms using factor analysis was evaluated
to provide a structured summary of SSI data. Factor solutions differed for
respondents with and without depression; we used the factor solutions for
those with depression. Orthogonal and oblique rotation models did not differ;
we relied on the oblique models. An item was included in a factor if its absolute
value was greater than 0.4 and it did not load significantly on more than
1 factor. The number of factors was defined from scree plots and limited to
those with an eigenvalue >1.0. A 7-factor solution was deemed optimal among
respondents with depression: (1) pain, weakness, and fatigue (7 items, 27.3%
of variance); (2) gastrointestinal complaints (3 items, 13.5% of variance);
(3) panic or anxiety (3 items, 15.4% of variance); (4) faintness or dizziness
(4 items, 15.6% of variance); (5) autonomic instability with anxiety (2 items,
12.4% of variance); (6) ringing in the ears, or head or nose fullness (2 items,
9.4% of variance); and (7) sensory or nerve impairment (2 items, 9.4% of variance).
Only gastrointestinal complaints and panic or anxiety were common to individuals
with and without depression. A dichotomous variable defined the presence of
a factor-based symptom cluster. For each factor, the cutpoint was defined
at the 10th percentile of respondents without depression.
Analyses were completed to estimate the prevalence of depression in
the US workforce and to estimate LPT and associated costs among individuals
with depression compared with those without depression. Three mutually exclusive
categories were defined: major depressive disorder (ie, major depressive disorder
only and major depressive disorder plus dysthymia), dysthymia (ie, any dysthymia
excluding major depressive disorder with dysthymia), and partial remission
or recurrence of major depressive disorder (ie, excluding partial remission
or recurrence of major depressive disorder with dysthymia). Depression prevalence
was estimated in 2 steps. First, age- and sex-specific prevalences were calculated
based on the sampling fraction of those responding in the positive and in
the negative to the 2 depression screening questions. Second, using direct
adjustment, age and sex stratum–specific prevalence estimates were multiplied
times the corresponding age- and sex-specific population size of the US workforce.
Lost productive time in respondents with and without depressions were calculated
as total LPT for any health-related reason. Excess LPT was defined as the
difference in mean LPT in respondents with depression compared with an expected
value in those without depression. The expected value was estimated by applying
rates of LPT from specific age and sex groups without depression to the same
demographic subgroups of individuals who met criteria for depression. This
same method was used to estimate mean LPT for depression with and without
a specific symptom cluster and for the corresponding expected value among
respondents without depression. Lost labor costs were estimated from lost
productive hours and self-reported annual income (ie, hourly wage equaled
annual income divided by the mean number of hours worked per week times 52
weeks). Lost dollars were calculated by multiplying lost hours by the hourly
wage.
Benchmarking and weighting variables with missing data (ie, 0.9%) were
imputed using the age- and sex-specific mode for categorical variables, and
the age- and sex-specific median for continuous variables. If only 1 of the
5 variables used to calculate presenteeism was missing, the mean value of
the remaining 4 variables was substituted, reducing the proportion with missing
presenteeism estimates from 4.5% to 3.3%. Salary information was missing for
18.7% of all respondents. Missing salary data were modeled using multiple
linear regression. SAS version 8.2 was used for all analyses (SAS Institute
Inc, Cary, NC) and P<.05 was used to determine
statistical significance.
Participation in the American Productivity Audit has been described
in detail elsewhere (unpublished data available from W.F.S. on request). Among
audit participants, women made up 56.1% of the sample and respondents were
equally distributed across the 4 age groups. A majority of respondents were
white (77.0%), formally educated beyond high school (66.6%), and working more
than 30 hours per week (82.9%) with an annual income less than $40 000
(51.3%). During the 2-week recall period, 10.0% of workers were absent from
work for a personal health reason and 38.1% reported unproductive time due
to personal health on at least 1 workday. Overall, workers lost a mean of
1.89 hours per week of productive work time for either a personal or family
health reason. Reduced performance at work due to personal health accounted
for 65.3% (1.32 h/wk) of the lost time.
Respondents who met PRIME-MD criteria for any depressive disorder with
a treatment indication (ie, major depressive disorder, dysthymia, or partial
remission or recurrence of major depressive disorder) were of similar sex,
age, race, annual salary, and employment status as those without a depressive
disorder (Table 1). Among all
participants, the majority were women (65.6%), between 35 and 65 years of
age (66.0%), white (76.2%), earning less than $40 000 annually (66.2%),
and working more than 30 hours per week (80.7%). In contrast, respondents
with a depressive disorder were significantly more likely than those without
depression to have a lower education level (43.0% vs 33.7% with a high school
education or less; P = .01), and to have at least
1 of the 7 physical symptom clusters derived from factor analysis (78.1% vs
41.4%; P<.001) (Table 1).
Compared with those with other depressive disorders, those with major
depression were significantly more likely to have a lower educational level
(P<.01 for all comparisons), earn less than $20 000
annually (P<.01), and report physical symptoms
associated with pain, weakness, and fatigue (P<.001),
panic or anxiety (P<.001), and autonomic instability
(P<.001) (Table
1). In contrast, those with dysthymia were, on average, significantly
more likely to have attained a higher level of formal education (P<.01), report a higher annual salary (P<.01),
and work more than 30 hours per week (Table
1).
Prevalence of Depressive Disorders
The 2-week prevalence of any depressive disorder in the US workforce
was estimated at 9.4% (Table 2).
Dysthymia was the most prevalent (3.6%), followed by major depression (3.4%),
and partial remission or recurrence of major depressive disorder (2.4%) (Table 2). Any depression was close to 2
times more prevalent in women than in men, with a marked difference in the
prevalence of major depression (women, 5.3%; men, 1.6%). Other notable patterns
included a strong inverse gradient with increasing education level and, in
general, higher prevalence of any depression in relation to lower annual salary
levels. Prevalence appears to be lowest among those working 20 to 30 hours
per week (6.8%) compared with those working more (9.5%) or fewer (10.9%) hours.
The greatest difference in prevalence of depression was observed in relation
to physical symptom status. Prevalence of major depression was particularly
elevated among those reporting symptoms of autonomic instability (19.8%),
pain, weakness, or fatigue (14.9%), and panic or anxiety (14.1%) (Table 2).
Average LPT and National Cost Estimates
Lost productive time was expressed as an average across all individuals
who met criteria for depression (Table 3). On average, workers with depression reported significantly more
total health-related LPT than those without depression (mean, 5.6 h/wk vs
an expected value of 1.5 h/wk in the absence of depression) (Table 3). A total of 77.1% of individuals with depression reported
some LPT during the 2-week recall period. The expected number of LPT hours
was estimated by applying rates of LPT for those without depression from specific
age and sex groups to the same demographic subgroups of individuals who met
criteria for a depressive disorder. Overall, LPT among depressed individuals
was primarily explained by LPT while at work (82.1%). Average total LPT per
week was considerably higher for major depression (mean [SE], 8.4 [1.3] h/wk),
followed by total LPT for partial remission of major depression (5.3 [1.1]
h/wk), and dysthymia (3.3 [0.6]h/wk) (Table
3).
Physical symptom clusters were common among individuals with depression.
Pain, weakness, or fatigue was the most common cluster (49%), followed by
sensory or nerve impairment (40%), and ringing ears or head fullness (38%).
Individuals with major depression consistently reported the most LPT when
it co-occurred with a physical symptom cluster—in particular, when it
co-occurred with pain, weakness, or fatigue (mean [SE], 10.0 [1.2] h/wk),
gastrointestinal complaints (10.7 [1.5] h/wk), and sensory or nerve impairment
(10.0 [1.4] h/wk) (Table 3). In
the absence of depression, autonomic instability was associated with the most
LPT (6.5 h/wk), and gastrointestinal complaints were associated with the least
LPT (2.0 h/wk) (Table 3).
Physical symptom clusters often co-occurred and were moderately correlated.
We used ordinary least-squares regression to simultaneously estimate the association
of each symptom cluster with LPT, adjusting for depression status, sex, and
age. In this model, significant associations were observed for only 3 symptom
clusters, the most common being pain, weakness, or fatigue (β = 3.0;
SE = 0.5); the least common being faintness or dizziness (β = 2.1; SE
= 0.7); and autonomic instability (β = 2.9; SE = 0.5). Coefficients for
the other symptom clusters were close to zero.
United States workers with depression are estimated to cost employers
$44.01 billion per year in LPT, an excess of $30.94 billion per year when
compared with an expected cost in workers without depression (Table 4). A total of 81.1% of the LPT costs are explained by reduced
performance while at work. Major depression accounts for almost half (48.5%)
of the LPT among workers with depression, again with the majority of the cost
explained by reduced performance while at work (Table 4).
We examined self-reported treatment (Table 5) for depression in the 12 months prior to interview. Individuals
with depression were dichotomized by symptom burden (ie, 2 or 3 vs 0 or 1
of the symptom clusters significantly associated with LPT [pain, weakness,
or fatigue; autonomic instability; faintness or dizziness]). For any depression,
we observed overall that less than one third of workers with depression reported
receiving a prescription drug in the past 12 months for depression or anxiety.
Most workers reported taking the medication in the past 12 months, with 69%
to 81% reporting taking it in the past 2 days. Overall, self-reported treatment
effectiveness was moderate (5 on a 0-10 anchored continuous scale) and appeared
to be lower for workers with a high symptom burden compared with those with
a low symptom burden. The differences, however, were not statistically significant
(P>.05).
Our estimate of the LPT cost due to depression offers unique information
regarding hidden costs that is consistent with the widely held notion that
depression is a leading cause of disability.34 Our
estimate of $31 billion in excess LPT refers to time lost among individuals
actively engaged in work (ie, worked at least 1 day in the previous week).
It does not include labor costs associated with disability leave.
Previous studies consistently indicate that the lost work-time cost
from depression is substantial,22,23,35-42 exceeding
direct medical costs. However, estimates based on studies of specific employers38,42 pose challenges in extrapolating
to the US workforce. Prevalence and impact of depression appear to vary by
occupation.43 Moreover, employer-specific studies
often underestimate lost labor costs because they usually focus only on absence
time and rely on medical claims data to identify employees with depression.
Costs of LPT are not captured for depressed individuals who have not sought
care or who have sought care for other reasons (eg, physical symptoms).
Previous national projections of the labor cost of depression were based
primarily on the Epidemiologic Catchment Area (ECA) studies23,35-37,39 completed
in the 1980s. Greenberg et al23 estimated lost
labor costs of $24 billion in 1990 and of $33.5 billion in 2002 after adjusting
for inflation. Five important differences distinguish our study from that
by Greenberg et al. First, Greenberg et al captured some of the costs due
to disability (ie, hospitalization, bed-days), but the LPT cost from reduced
performance at work is incomplete. Second, the estimate of Greenberg et al
includes major depression (1-year prevalence), dysthymia (lifetime prevalence),
and bipolar disorders, but not partial remission of major depression. Third,
in using ECA data, Greenberg et al made assumptions about the average number
of hospital (ie, treated patients) and bed-ridden days (untreated individuals),
the number of days used for outpatient care, the average impact of depression
outside of inpatient care and days at work, and other factors. In contrast,
our questionnaire specifically captured LPT due to both work absence and reduced
performance while at work. Fourth, in the ECA study by Greenberg et al, details
regarding episodes of depression were recalled over a 1-year period35 vs the 2-week period in our study. Finally, clinical
care and management of depression has changed substantially since the 1980s.26,44-46
Our study indicates that 81% of the LPT costs from depression were explained
by reduced performance while at work. This finding is consistent with observations
for numerous other conditions. A substantial share of the LPT costs are explained
by reduced work performance, not work absence.3,4,7,12,16,17,20,23,47-51
Using the PRIME-MD, we estimated depression prevalence to be 9.4%. Comparing
our estimate with those from other studies is difficult. For example, the
ECA studies used the Diagnostic Interview Schedule. There is no formal link
to the PRIME-MD. Prevalence estimates are not usually confined to individuals
working for pay. Nonetheless, for comparison with other studies we have focused
on major depression because the criteria have not changed and because major
depression accounts for a substantial share of the LPT costs from depressive
disorders.
Based on ECA data, the 1-year prevalence of major depression among working
populations is 4%, excluding symptoms attributable to alcohol, drugs, physical
injury, and illness.43 This prevalence is comparable
with our prevalence estimates of 3.4% for major depression and 2.4% for partial
remission. We followed existing criteria and did not make the same exclusions
as Eaton et al.43 Broader population-based
estimates of the 1-year prevalence of major depression in adults aged 18 years
to 54 years range from 6.5% (ECA studies) to 11.1%.52 Our
estimate of major depression prevalence should be lower than previous estimates
(ie, those with a 12-month time frame) since we capture data only from individuals
who are currently experiencing a depressive episode and are actively working.
We do not capture data from individuals who experience recurrent episodes
of depression53-56 but
from those who are between episodes. Enumerating these cases is not essential
to an accurate estimate of LPT from depression. In contrast, we are likely
to have relatively complete capture of dysthymia, since it is an inherently
chronic condition by definition, lasting 2 years or longer.
Workers with major depression and physical symptoms account for a disproportionate
share of the LPT due to depression. While physical symptoms in some individuals
with depression are due to other conditions (eg, diabetes) comorbid with depression,
there is growing recognition that physical symptoms are often directly associated
with depression. More than 80% of patients with depression who seek care present
with physical symptoms.57 Moreover, disability
from depression appears to be correlated with number of physical symptoms.
The strong relation between depression and physical symptoms is thought to
be a common product of dysfunctional serotonergic and noradrenergic pathways
that project throughout the central nervous system and spinal cord.58 It is noteworthy that our data suggest that individuals
with depression and an elevated symptom burden (ie, at least 2 symptom clusters
significantly associated with LPT) appear to report the lowest treatment effectiveness.
If this relationship is real, workers with depression and a high physical
symptom burden are likely to be an important target for intervention to reduce
both direct medical costs and LPT. However, larger studies of depression treatment
status in the US workforce are required to accurately determine whether this
relationship is real and whether the relatively low proportion of workers
prescribed a treatment is an indication of unmet need.
The association of physical symptoms and mood disorders also may be
sustained because individuals who are impaired by an illness are entitled
to the dispensations of the "sick role"59 that
includes a reduction in expected performance of normal role functions. To
establish the sick role, the patient must be perceived as having a legitimate
medical condition beyond their control. Compared with physical symptoms, it
is more difficult to establish the sick role for depression. Stigmatization
is associated with mental disorders, physicians often fail to detect mood
disorders, and individuals may doubt whether depression is truly beyond personal
control. Therefore, even when role impairment is linked to a mood disorder,
the sick role is often constructed on the basis of physical symptoms that
also may have a direct pathophysiological relationship with the mood disorder,58 even though the symptoms themselves may not cause
impairment.
Our study has several potential limitations that could influence the
accuracy of estimated LPT attributable to depression. First, the Work and
Health Interview was designed to focus only on estimating work loss incurred
by individual workers reporting a health condition during the recall period.
Although this is the primary driver of employer costs associated with lost
productive work time, we recognize that health-related LPT estimates could
be refined by considering other factors such as the hiring and training of
replacement workers or the concomitant impact among coworkers.60 These
other factors could increase, decrease, or have no net effect on health-related
LPT cost estimates. Second, depression-related LPT costs could be overestimated
because of the predisposition of individuals to overstate work impairment
when in the acute phase of a depressive episode.61 While
data on this issue are limited, we cannot exclude the possibility of reporting
bias leading to an overestimate of LPT costs among individuals with depression.
Finally, our US population estimate of LPT is based on a strategically selected
but modest sample size of 219 employed individuals who met PRIME-MD criteria
for depression, of whom 87 had symptoms of major depression. The uncertainties
inherent in the sample size for this study must be considered when interpreting
our cost estimates and especially when considering self-reported treatment
data.
1.Dasbach EJ, Carides GW, Gerth WC, Santanello NC, Pigeon JG, Kramer MS. Work and productivity loss in the rizatriptan multiple attack study.
Cephalalgia.2000;20:830-834.http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/htbin-post/Entrez/query?db=m&form=6&Dopt=r&uid=11167913
Google Scholar 2.Davies GM, Santanello N, Gerth W, Lerner D, Block GA. Validation of a migraine work and productivity loss questionnaire for
use in migraine studies.
Cephalalgia.1999;19:497-502.http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/htbin-post/Entrez/query?db=m&form=6&Dopt=r&uid=10403065
Google Scholar 3.Gerth WC, Carides GW, Dasbach EJ, Visser WH, Santanello NC. The multinational impact of migraine symptoms on healthcare utilisation
and work loss.
Pharmacoeconomics.2001;19:197-206.http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/htbin-post/Entrez/query?db=m&form=6&Dopt=r&uid=11284383
Google Scholar 4.Hu XH, Markson LE, Lipton RB, Stewart WF, Berger ML. Burden of migraine in the United States: disability and economic costs.
Arch Intern Med.1999;159:813-818.http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/htbin-post/Entrez/query?db=m&form=6&Dopt=r&uid=10219926
Google Scholar 5.McCunney RJ. Health and productivity: a role for occupational health professionals.
J Occup Environ Med.2001;43:30-35.http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/htbin-post/Entrez/query?db=m&form=6&Dopt=r&uid=11201767
Google Scholar 6.Schulman EA, Cady RK, Henry D.
et al. Effectiveness of sumatriptan in reducing productivity loss due to migraine:
results of a randomized, double-blind placebo-controlled clinical trial.
Mayo Clin Proc.2000;75:782-789.http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/htbin-post/Entrez/query?db=m&form=6&Dopt=r&uid=10943230
Google Scholar 7.Stang PE, Osterhaus JT. Impact of migraine in the United States: data from the national health
interview survey.
Headache.1993;33:29-35.http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/htbin-post/Entrez/query?db=m&form=6&Dopt=r&uid=8436495
Google Scholar 8.Stang P, Cady R, Batenhorst A, Hoffman L. Workplace productivity: a review of the impact of migraine and its
treatment.
Pharmacoeconomics.2001;19:231-244.http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/htbin-post/Entrez/query?db=m&form=6&Dopt=r&uid=11303412
Google Scholar 9.Fautrel B, Guillemin F. Cost of illness studies in rheumatic diseases.
Curr Opin Rheumatol.2002;14:121-126.http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/htbin-post/Entrez/query?db=m&form=6&Dopt=r&uid=11845016
Google Scholar 10.Riedel JE, Baase C, Hymel P, Lynch W, Dreisbach S, Peterson KW. Preventing Disease and Promoting Health: Impact on
Productivity. Scottsdale, Ariz: Institute for Health and Productivity Management;
2001.
11.Burton WN, Connerty CM. Evaluation of a worksite-based patient education intervention targeted
at employees with diabetes mellitus.
J Occup Environ Med.1998;40:702-706.http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/htbin-post/Entrez/query?db=m&form=6&Dopt=r&uid=9729753
Google Scholar 12.Ng YC, Jacobs P, Johnson JA. Productivity losses associated with diabetes in the US.
Diabetes Care.2001;24:257-261.http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/htbin-post/Entrez/query?db=m&form=6&Dopt=r&uid=11213875
Google Scholar 13.Blaiss MS. Cognitive, social, and economic costs of allergic rhinitis.
Allergy Asthma Proc.2000;21:7-13.http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/htbin-post/Entrez/query?db=m&form=6&Dopt=r&uid=10748946
Google Scholar 14.Blanc PD, Trupin L, Eisner L.
et al. The work impact of asthma and rhinitis: findings from a population-based
survey.
J Clin Epidemiol.2001;54:610-618.http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/htbin-post/Entrez/query?db=m&form=6&Dopt=r&uid=11377122
Google Scholar 15.Burton WN, Conti DJ, Chen C-Y, Schultz AB, Edington DW. The impact of allergies and allergy treatment on worker productivity.
J Occup Environ Med.2001;43:64-71.http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/htbin-post/Entrez/query?db=m&form=6&Dopt=r&uid=11201771
Google Scholar 16.Crystal-Peters J, Crown WH, Goetzel RZ, Schutt DC. The cost of productivity losses associated with allergic rhinitis.
Am J Manage Care.2000;6:373-378.http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/htbin-post/Entrez/query?db=m&form=6&Dopt=r&uid=10977437
Google Scholar 17.Kessler RC, Almeida DM, Berglund P, Stang P. Pollen and mold exposure impairs the work performance of employees
with allergic rhinitis.
Ann Allergy Asthma Immunol.2001;87:289-295.http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/htbin-post/Entrez/query?db=m&form=6&Dopt=r&uid=11686420
Google Scholar 18.Slavin RG. Occupational and allergic rhinitis: impact on worker productivity and
safety.
Allergy Asthma Proc.1998;19:277-284.http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/htbin-post/Entrez/query?db=m&form=6&Dopt=r&uid=9801741
Google Scholar 19.Borchardt PJ. Employee productivity and gastroesophageal reflux disease: the payer's
viewpoint.
Am J Gastroenterol.2001;96(suppl 8):S62-S63.http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/htbin-post/Entrez/query?db=m&form=6&Dopt=r&uid=11510774
Google Scholar 20.Henke CJ, Levin TR, Henning JM, Potter LP. Work loss costs due to peptic ulcer disease and gastroesophageal reflux
disease in a health maintenance organization.
Am J Gastroenterol.2000;95:788-792.http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/htbin-post/Entrez/query?db=m&form=6&Dopt=r&uid=10710076
Google Scholar 21.Wahlqvist P. Symptoms of gastroesophageal reflux disease, perceived productivity,
and health-related quality of life.
Am J Gastroenterol.2001;96(suppl 8):S57-S61.http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/htbin-post/Entrez/query?db=m&form=6&Dopt=r&uid=11510773
Google Scholar 22.Berndt ER, Bailit HL, Keller MB, Verner JC, Finkelstein SN. Health care use and at-work productivity among employees with mental
disorders.
Health Aff (Millwood).2000;19(4):244-256.http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/htbin-post/Entrez/query?db=m&form=6&Dopt=r&uid=10916980
Google Scholar 23.Greenberg PE, Stiglin LE, Finkelstein SN, Berndt ER. The economic burden of depression in 1990.
J Clin Psychiatry.1993;54:405-418.http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/htbin-post/Entrez/query?db=m&form=6&Dopt=r&uid=8270583
Google Scholar 24.Simon GE, Revicki D, Heiligenstein J.
et al. Recovery from depression, work productivity, and health care costs
among primary care patients.
Gen Hosp Psychiatry.2000;22:153-162.http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/htbin-post/Entrez/query?db=m&form=6&Dopt=r&uid=10880708
Google Scholar 25.Simon GE, Barber C, Birnbaum HG.
et al. Depression and work productivity: the comparative costs of treatment
versus nontreatment.
J Occup Environ Med.2001;43:2-9.http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/htbin-post/Entrez/query?db=m&form=6&Dopt=r&uid=11201765
Google Scholar 26.Olfson M, Marcus SC, Druss B, Elinson L, Tanielian T, Pincus HA. National trends in the outpatient treatment of depression.
JAMA.2002;287:203-209.http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/htbin-post/Entrez/query?db=m&form=6&Dopt=r&uid=11779262
Google Scholar 27.Ricci JA, Stewart WF, Leotta C, Chee E. A comparison of six phone interviews designed to measure health-related
lost productive work time.
Value Health.2001;4:460.Google Scholar 28.Karasek R, Gordon G, Pietrokovsky C, Frese M, Pieper C. Job Content Instrument: Questionnaire and User's
Guide, Version 1.1. Los Angeles: University of Southern California; 1986.
30.Brick JM, Kalton G. Handling missing data in survey research.
Stat Methods Med Res.1996;5:215-238.http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/htbin-post/Entrez/query?db=m&form=6&Dopt=r&uid=8931194
Google Scholar 31.Spitzer RL, Williams JBW, Kroenke K.
et al. Utility of a new procedure for diagnosing mental disorders in primary
care: the PRIME-MD 1000 study.
JAMA.1994;272:1749-1756.http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/htbin-post/Entrez/query?db=m&form=6&Dopt=r&uid=7966923
Google Scholar 32.American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders,
Revised Third Edition. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 1987.
33.Barsky AG, Wyshak G, Herman GL. Hypochondriasis: an evaluation of the DSM-III criteria in medical outpatients.
Arch Gen Psychiatry.1986;43:493-500.http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/htbin-post/Entrez/query?db=m&form=6&Dopt=r&uid=3964028
Google Scholar 34.Murray CJL, Lopez AD. The Global Burden of Disease: A Comprehensive Assessment
of Mortality and Disability From Diseases, Injuries, and Risk Factors in 1990
and Projected to 2020. Cambridge, Mass: Harvard University Press; 1996.
35.Stoudemire A, Frank R, Hedemark N, Kamlet M, Blazer D. The economic burden of depression.
Gen Hosp Psychiatry.1986;8:387-394.http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/htbin-post/Entrez/query?db=m&form=6&Dopt=r&uid=3491775
Google Scholar 36.Broadhead WE, Blazer DG, George LK, Tse CK. Depression, disability days, and days lost from work in a prospective
epidemiologic survey.
JAMA.1990;264:2524-2550.http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/htbin-post/Entrez/query?db=m&form=6&Dopt=r&uid=2146410
Google Scholar 37.Johnson J, Weissman MM, Klerman GL. Service utilization and social morbidity associated with depressive
symptoms in the community.
JAMA.1992;267:1478-1483.http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/htbin-post/Entrez/query?db=m&form=6&Dopt=r&uid=1538538
Google Scholar 38.Conti DJ, Burton WN. The economic impact of depression in a workplace.
J Occup Med.1994;36:983-988.http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/htbin-post/Entrez/query?db=m&form=6&Dopt=r&uid=7823220
Google Scholar 39.Judd FK, Mijch AM. Depressive symptoms in patients with HIV infection.
Aust N Z J Psychiatry.1996;30:104-109.http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/htbin-post/Entrez/query?db=m&form=6&Dopt=r&uid=8724332
Google Scholar 40.Finkelstein SN, Berndt ER, Greenberg PE, Parsley RA, Russell JM, Keller MB.Chronic Depression Study Group. Improvement in subjective work performance after treatment of chronic
depression: some preliminary results.
Psychopharmacol Bull.1996;32:33-40.http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/htbin-post/Entrez/query?db=m&form=6&Dopt=r&uid=8927672
Google Scholar 41.Zhang M, Rost KM, Fortney JC, Smith GR. A community study of depression treatment and employment earnings.
Psychiatric Services.1999;50:1209-1213.http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/htbin-post/Entrez/query?db=m&form=6&Dopt=r&uid=10478909
Google Scholar 42.Goetzel RZ, Hawkins K, Ozminkowski RJ, Wang S. The health and productivity cost burden of the "Top 10" physical and
mental health conditions affecting six large US employers in 1999.
J Occup Environ Med.2003;45:5-14.http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/htbin-post/Entrez/query?db=m&form=6&Dopt=r&uid=12553174
Google Scholar 43.Eaton WW, Anthony JC, Mandel W, Garrison R. Occupations and the prevalence of major depressive disorder.
J Occup Med.1990;32:1079-1087.http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/htbin-post/Entrez/query?db=m&form=6&Dopt=r&uid=2258762
Google Scholar 44.Goff VV. Depression: A Decade of Progress, More to Do. Washington, DC: National Health Policy Forum; November 22, 2002.
Issue brief 786.
45.Graves EJ. Detailed diagnoses and procedures, National Hospital Discharge Survey
1987.
National Center for Health Statistics, Vital Health Stat 13.1989;100:1-304.http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/htbin-post/Entrez/query?db=m&form=6&Dopt=r&uid=2711615
Google Scholar 46.Kozak LJ, Hall MJ, Owings MF. National Hospital Discharge Survey: 2000 annual summary with detailed
diagnosis and procedure data.
Vital Health Stat 13.2002;153:1-194.http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/htbin-post/Entrez/query?db=m&form=6&Dopt=r&uid=12518555
Google Scholar 47.Cockburn IM, Bailit HL, Berndt ER, Finkelstein SN. When antihistamines go to work.
Bus Health.1999;17:49-50.http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/htbin-post/Entrez/query?db=m&form=6&Dopt=r&uid=10387170
Google Scholar 48.Cull RE, Wells NEJ, Miocevich ML. The economic cost of migraine.
Br J Med Econ.1992;2:103-115.Google Scholar 49.Kessler RC, Barber C, Birnbaum HG.
et al. Depression in the workplace: effects on short-term disability.
Health Aff (Millwood).1999;18(5):163-171.http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/htbin-post/Entrez/query?db=m&form=6&Dopt=r&uid=10495604
Google Scholar 50.Merkesdal S, Ruof J, Schoffski O, Bernitt K, Zeidler H, Mau W. Indirect medical costs in early rheumatoid arthritis: composition of
and changes in indirect costs within the first three years of disease.
Arthritis Rheum.2001;44:528-534.http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/htbin-post/Entrez/query?db=m&form=6&Dopt=r&uid=11263766
Google Scholar 51.Osterhaus JT, Guterman DL, Piachetka JR. Healthcare resource and lost labour costs of migraine headache in the
US.
Pharmacoeconomics.1992;2:67-76.http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/htbin-post/Entrez/query?db=m&form=6&Dopt=r&uid=10146980
Google Scholar 52.Kessler RC, McGonagle KA, Zhao S.
et al. Lifetime and 12-month prevalence of DSM-III-R psychiatric disorder
in the United States.
Arch Gen Psychiatry.1994;51:8-19.http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/htbin-post/Entrez/query?db=m&form=6&Dopt=r&uid=8279933
Google Scholar 53.Keller MB, Lavori PW, Mueller TI.
et al. Time to recovery, chronicity, and levels of psychopathology in major
depression: a 5-year prospective follow-up of 431 subjects.
Arch Gen Psychiatry.1992;49:809-816.http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/htbin-post/Entrez/query?db=m&form=6&Dopt=r&uid=1417434
Google Scholar 54.Frank E, Prien RF, Jarrett RB.
et al. Conceptualization and rationale to consensus definitions of terms in
major depressive disorders: response, remission, recovery, relapse, and recurrence.
Arch Gen Psychiatry.1991;48:851-855.http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/htbin-post/Entrez/query?db=m&form=6&Dopt=r&uid=1929776
Google Scholar 55.Katon W, Von Korff M, Lin E.
et al. Collaborative management to achieve treatment guidelines: impact on
depression in primary care.
JAMA.1995;273:1026-1031.http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/htbin-post/Entrez/query?db=m&form=6&Dopt=r&uid=7897786
Google Scholar 56.Schulberg HC, Block MR, Madonia MJ.
et al. Treating major depression in primary care practice: eight-month clinical
outcomes.
Arch Gen Psychiatry.1996;53:913-919.http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/htbin-post/Entrez/query?db=m&form=6&Dopt=r&uid=8857868
Google Scholar 57.Kirmayer LJ, Robbins JM, Dworkind M, Yaffe MJ. Somatization and the recognition of depression and anxiety in primary
care.
Am J Psychiatry.1993;150:734-741.http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/htbin-post/Entrez/query?db=m&form=6&Dopt=r&uid=8480818
Google Scholar 58.Stahl SM. Does depression hurt?
J Clin Psychiatry.2002;63:273-274.http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/htbin-post/Entrez/query?db=m&form=6&Dopt=r&uid=12000200
Google Scholar 59.Parsons T. The Social System. New York, NY: The Free Press; 1950.
60.Berger ML, Murray JF, Xu J, Pauly M. Alternative valuations of work loss and productivity.
J Occup Environ Med.2001;43:18-24.http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/htbin-post/Entrez/query?db=m&form=6&Dopt=r&uid=11201764
Google Scholar 61.Morgado A, Raoux N, Smith M, Allilaire JF, Widlocher D. Subjective bias in reports of poor work adjustment in depressed patients.
Acta Psychiatr Scand.1989;80:541-547.http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/htbin-post/Entrez/query?db=m&form=6&Dopt=r&uid=2618776
Google Scholar