Importance
Cognitive decline is a major cause of disability in stroke survivors. The magnitude of survivors’ cognitive changes after stroke is uncertain.
Objective
To measure changes in cognitive function among survivors of incident stroke, controlling for their prestroke cognitive trajectories.
Design, Setting, and Participants
Prospective study of 23 572 participants 45 years or older without baseline cognitive impairment from the Reasons for Geographic and Racial Differences in Stroke (REGARDS) cohort, residing in the continental United States, enrolled 2003-2007 and followed up through March 31, 2013. Over a median follow-up of 6.1 years (interquartile range, 5.0-7.1 years), 515 participants survived expert-adjudicated incident stroke and 23 057 remained stroke free.
Exposure
Time-dependent incident stroke.
Main Outcomes and Measures
The primary outcome was change in global cognition (Six-Item Screener [SIS], range, 0-6). Secondary outcomes were change in new learning (Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer Disease Word-List Learning; range, 0-30), verbal memory (Word-List Delayed Recall; range, 0-10), and executive function (Animal Fluency Test; range, ≥0), and cognitive impairment (SIS score <5 [impaired] vs ≥5 [unimpaired]). For all tests, higher scores indicate better performance.
Results
Stroke was associated with acute decline in global cognition (0.10 points [95% CI, 0.04 to 0.17]), new learning (1.80 points [95% CI, 0.73 to 2.86]), and verbal memory (0.60 points [95% CI, 0.13 to 1.07]). Participants with stroke, compared with those without stroke, demonstrated faster declines in global cognition (0.06 points per year faster [95% CI, 0.03 to 0.08]) and executive function (0.63 points per year faster [95% CI, 0.12 to 1.15]), but not in new learning and verbal memory, compared with prestroke slopes. Among survivors, the difference in risk of cognitive impairment acutely after stroke, compared with immediately before stroke, was not statistically significant (odds ratio, 1.32 [95% CI, 0.95 to 1.83]; P = .10); however, there was a significantly faster poststroke rate of incident cognitive impairment compared with the prestroke rate (odds ratio, 1.23 per year [95% CI, 1.10 to 1.38]; P < .001). For a 70-year-old black woman with average values for all covariates at baseline, stroke at year 3 was associated with greater incident cognitive impairment: absolute difference of 4.0% (95% CI, −1.2% to 9.2%) at year 3 and 12.4% (95% CI, 7.7% to 17.1%) at year 6.
Conclusions and Relevance
Incident stroke was associated with an acute decline in cognitive function and also accelerated and persistent cognitive decline over 6 years.
Each year, 795 000 US residents experience a stroke.1 In 2010, almost 7 million adults were stroke survivors.1 Over the last 2 decades, age-standardized years lived with disability rates increased by 40% for stroke—the only major disease to show a significant increase in this important disability measure.2 Disability due to stroke is a major driver of health burden and costs for families, health care systems, and public programs such as Medicare and Medicaid.2 Cognitive impairment after stroke is a major contributor to this disability,3,4 and its prevalence has increased sharply in older adults.5,6 Despite its enormous social and economic burden, poststroke cognitive impairment has been called a “neglected consequence of stroke.”7
Quiz Ref IDAlthough stroke is associated with acute cognitive decline,3 it is unclear whether stroke survivors acquire a faster rate of cognitive decline over the years following the event (ie, slope) compared with the prestroke rate of cognitive decline, after accounting for the acute cognitive decline at the time of the event.8 While cognitive decline over the years before stroke is common9 and is associated with poststroke cognitive decline,10 most studies of stroke cannot measure actual changes in the rate of cognitive decline associated with stroke because they lack measures of patients’ prestroke cognitive changes or use proxy-reported measures.10-14 Moreover, most studies of stroke have not measured both the acute decline in cognitive function at the time of the stroke and the change in the rate of cognitive decline over the years after stroke simultaneously.15 One study9 suggests that stroke causes an acute decline in cognitive function at the time of the event but does not cause faster cognitive decline over the years following the event.
We hypothesized that stroke causes an acute decline in cognitive function at the time of the event and also faster cognitive decline during the years following the event.
Study Design, Participants, and Measurements
The Reasons for Geographic and Racial Differencs in Stroke (REGARDS) study is a prospective cohort study of 30 239 non-Hispanic black and white individuals examining regional and racial influences on stroke mortality.16 Details are described elsewhere.16 Briefly, participants were enrolled between 2003 and 2007 using commercially available lists and a combination of mail and telephone contacts to recruit English-speaking, community-dwelling adults 45 years or older who were living in the continental United States. Race and sex were balanced by design, with oversampling of the Southeastern United States. Race was self-reported. Baseline data collection included computer-assisted telephone interviews gathering demographic information, medical history, and health status. In-home examinations by trained health care professionals following standardized, quality-controlled protocols collected blood and urine samples, electrocardiograms, blood pressure, height and weight measurements, and medication use by pill bottle review. Blood and urine samples were centrally analyzed at the University of Vermont.
Participants or their proxies were followed up every 6 months by telephone with retrieval of medical records for reported hospitalizations. For this study, we followed up participants through March 31, 2013. To control for prestroke cognition, we required all participants to have a baseline measurement of each outcome. We excluded participants with baseline cognitive impairment, defined as a Six-Item Screener (SIS) score less than 5. This cut point is a valid measure of cognitive impairment in community-dwelling black and white adults.17 We required that participants with incident stroke have 1 or more cognitive measurement after stroke.
The study was approved by the institutional review boards of all participating institutions, and all participants provided written informed consent.
Cognitive Function Assessments
REGARDS technicians who underwent formal training and certification administered cognitive function tests longitudinally by telephone including: the SIS beginning in 2003 and measured annually and a battery of 3 cognitive tests measured biannually starting in 2006 that included the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer Disease (CERAD) Word List Learning (WLL), Word List Delayed Recall (WLD), and Animal Fluency Test (AFT).18,19 Research demonstrates that global cognition, word list, and verbal fluency can be measured reliably and precisely over the telephone in middle-aged and older adults, with scores virtually identical to those obtained in person.20-22 These cognitive measures are consistent with the Vascular Cognitive Impairment Harmonization Standards23 and have been validated for black and white individuals.17,24,25
The SIS assesses global cognitive function and can detect cognitive dysfunction in older patients experiencing acute medical illness.26 The SIS consists of 3-item recall and 3-item temporal orientation (score range, 0-6).17 The CERAD WLL measures new learning (score range, 0-30) and the WLD measures verbal memory (score range, 0-10). Quiz Ref IDThe AFT assesses executive function (complex cognitive processing used in problem-solving or complex action sequences), with scores representing number of animals generated in 1 minute. For all cognitive tests, higher scores indicate better performance. Cognitive data were provided only by self-respondents.
Measurement of Incident Stroke
Quiz Ref IDIncident strokes were adjudicated by a team of experts who used published guidelines and reviewed medical records.27,28 Stroke events were defined as “rapidly developing clinical signs of focal, at times global, disturbance of cerebral function, lasting more than 24 hours or leading to death with no apparent cause other than that of vascular origin” (World Health Organization).27,29 Events not meeting this definition but characterized by symptoms lasting less than 24 hours and with neuroimaging consistent with acute ischemia or hemorrhage were classified as “clinical strokes.” For fatal strokes, the medical history, hospital records, interviews with next of kin or proxies, and death certificate or National Death Index data were reviewed to adjudicate the cause of death.30 Strokes were further classified as ischemic or hemorrhagic. Cases were assigned to 2 physician adjudicators and disagreements were resolved by full committee review. To maintain high interrater reliability, an adjudicator underwent retraining if disagreement with other adjudicators was greater than 20% in ongoing review.27
Covariates were factors that could influence stroke and cognition and were measured at baseline. Demographics were age, sex, race, education, marital status, income, urban/rural residence, and region of residence. Vascular risk factors were systolic blood pressure, diabetes status, hyperlipidemia, atrial fibrillation, waist circumference, body mass index, alcohol intake, cigarette smoking, and physical activity. Clinical risk factors were baseline cognitive score for each cognitive outcome, glomerular filtration rate,31 history of stroke, history of myocardial infarction, self-reported health status, and depression symptoms.32
The primary outcome was global cognition measured by the SIS; secondary outcomes were new learning as measured by the WLL, verbal memory as measured by the WLD, and executive function as measured by the AFT. Each outcome measure was treated as a continuous variable. Continuous variables may better detect average intraindividual change and heterogeneity in intraindividual change in cognitive function.33 An additional secondary outcome was cognitive impairment as measured by SIS score (<5 [impaired] vs ≥5 [unimpaired])17 and allowing a participant’s status of cognitive impairment to vary over time. Incident stroke was treated as a time-dependent covariate that affects a participant’s cognitive test performance in all years after the stroke.15 The eFigure in the Supplement shows the conceptual model.
Descriptive characteristics were compared between participants who did and did not have an incident stroke during follow-up using 2-sample t test with equal variance or χ2 tests as appropriate. The association of baseline covariates with cognitive function was assessed using linear mixed-effects models that adjusted for baseline cognitive score and years since baseline.
We fit linear mixed-effects models to measure changes in cognitive function over time after adjusting for participant factors including baseline cognitive score. The models included random effects for intercept and slope to accommodate correlation of cognitive measures within participants over time and to allow participant-specific rates of cognitive change.34,35 We analyzed each dependent variable separately. Cognition was censored at the time of second incident stroke, death, loss to follow-up, or the end of follow-up. Time was expressed as the years from the date of the first measurement of the cognitive outcome. Generalized linear mixed-effects models for a binary outcome were used for estimating the odds of incident cognitive impairment (SIS score <5).
Model A included a time-varying incident stroke variable to estimate the effect of incident stroke on the acute decline in cognitive function at the time of the event (the value changes from 0 to 1 on the date of the incident stroke) because stroke is associated with an acute decline in cognitive function. The acute decline in cognitive function at the time of stroke was estimated based on the fitted model, which included the first set of routinely administered cognitive function tests after a stroke event as well as all other cognitive function tests administered before and after stroke.9,36 For this study, the first cognitive assessment after stroke is considered the acute component or early/mid-stage recovery.
Model B included the variables from Model A and added a time after stroke covariate to estimate the effect of incident stroke on the decline in cognitive function over the years following the event. This variable indicates the rate of change in cognitive function (slope) after incident stroke. Models included demographics, vascular risk factors, and clinical factors. Age, sex, race, education, region, and baseline cognitive score were retained in all models regardless of statistical significance. Other variables that did not reach statistical significance (defined as P < .05) were removed from the final models; these were marital status, urban/rural residence, hyperlipidemia, atrial fibrillation, body mass index, physical activity, and diastolic blood pressure.
After selecting the final, parsimonious model, we calculated participant-specific (conditional) predicted values for each cognitive score and participant-specific predicted probabilities of incident cognitive impairment (SIS score <5) over time for a 70-year-old black woman with the average values of all covariates at baseline (high school education, stroke belt residence, income <$20 000, never smoker, no alcohol use, systolic blood pressure 135 mm Hg, diabetes present, waist circumference 95 cm, no self-reported stroke, 4-item Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale score of 0.9 points, fair health status, and SIS score of 5 points) conditional on her experiencing or not experiencing an incident stroke midway through the follow-up period (at year 3). For our exemplar individual, we chose covariate values that were representative for the stroke belt population because it had a higher risk of cognitive decline relative to the remaining population. Random effects for this prediction were set to zero.
We included participants who self-reported a baseline history of stroke in the main analysis to allow comparison with a study15 that included adults with a self-reported history of physician-diagnosed stroke at baseline. We repeated analyses excluding participants who reported a stroke history at baseline and using multiple imputation for missing baseline values of covariates (eMethods in the Supplement).37
Statistical significance for all analyses was set as P < .05 (2-sided). All analyses were performed using Stata version 13.1 (StataCorp).
After excluding the 2639 individuals with baseline cognitive impairment, the 2072 with insufficient information on the primary outcome, the 1887 with missing covariate data, and the 69 with incident stroke before baseline outcome measurement, the study sample included 23 572 participants, 515 of whom experienced incident stroke (470 ischemic, 43 hemorrhagic, and 2 of undeterminable type) over a median follow-up of 6.1 years (interquartile range, 5.0-7.1 years) (Figure 1). There were 306 strokes in 14 632 white participants (2.1%) and 209 strokes in 8940 black participants (2.3%) (absolute difference, 0.2% [95% CI, 0.1% to 0.6%]; P = .2). Stroke incidence was stable over the duration of follow-up (eTable 1 in the Supplement). Excluded participants were more likely than included participants to be older, black, less educated, and current smokers and nondrinkers; excluded participants also were more likely to have lower incomes; diabetes; a history of stroke at baseline; fair or poor health status; higher baseline values of systolic blood pressure, waist circumference, and depressive symptoms; and lower baseline cognitive scores.
Table 1 presents baseline characteristics of study participants. Compared with participants who did not experience an incident stroke, those who did were more likely to be older, men, and current smokers and to have diabetes, less education, lower income, and worse health status. Adults who had incident stroke had higher baseline values of systolic blood pressure, waist circumference, and depressive symptom scores and more frequently reported a history of stroke at baseline than those who did not. Baseline prestroke SIS scores were slightly lower among those with than without incident stroke (5.7 vs 5.8 points; absolute difference, 0.04 points [95% CI, 0.002 to 0.08]; P = .04).
There were 61 deaths among the 515 individuals with incident stroke (11.8%) and 1812 deaths among the 23 056 without incident stroke (7.9%) (absolute difference, 4.0% [95% CI, 1.6% to 6.3%]; P = .001). Participants had undergone a mean of 3.0 (SD, 1.8) SIS tests and 1.4 (SD, 0.6) 3-test batteries before stroke. Stroke survivors had undergone a mean of 2.8 (SD, 1.8) SIS tests and 1.3 (SD, 0.5) 3-test batteries after stroke and a median follow-up of 2.5 years (interquartile range, 1.2 to 4.2 years). eTable 2 in the Supplement presents the time from incident stroke to the first poststroke measurement of each cognitive test.
Because the secondary outcome measures were introduced during follow-up and performed less frequently, the WLL analysis included 10 321 participants, 107 of whom had incident stroke; the WLD analysis included 10 053 participants, 102 of whom had incident stroke; and the AFT analysis included 11 214 participants, 120 of whom had incident stroke. eTable 3 in the Supplement presents the scores for each cognitive test at the end of follow-up by incident stroke status.
Change in Global Cognition After Stroke
Incident stroke was associated with a significant decline in global cognition acutely after stroke and also faster decline in global cognition over the years following the event. Table 2 and Figure 2 show that there was a slight increase in global cognition over time before stroke. Stroke survivors experienced an acute decline in global cognition after stroke (adjusted decline in SIS score, 0.10 points [95% CI, 0.04 to 0.17]; P = .001). In the years following stroke, global cognition declined significantly faster than it did before the stroke (decrease in slope after incident stroke, 0.06 points per year [95% CI, 0.03 to 0.08]; P < .001), resulting in a net negative slope after stroke (prestroke slope: 0.021; poststroke slope: −0.035). eTable 4 in the Supplement presents the unadjusted model.
We also assessed SIS as a binary outcome. Among survivors, the difference in risk of cognitive impairment acutely after stroke, compared with immediately before stroke, was not statistically significant (odds ratio, 1.32 [95% CI, 0.95 to 1.83]); however, there was a significantly faster rate of incident cognitive impairment after stroke compared with the prestroke rate (odds ratio, 1.23 per year [95% CI, 1.10 to 1.38]). For the exemplar individual, a 70-year-old black woman with average values of all covariates at baseline, stroke at year 3 was associated with a greater predicted probability of incident cognitive impairment compared with no stroke at year 3 (19.8% vs 15.7%; absolute difference, 4.0% [95% CI, −1.2% to 9.2%]; P < .10) and at year 6 (23.5% vs 11.1%; absolute difference, 12.4% [95% CI, 7.7% to 17.1%]; P < .001). At the end of follow-up, the frequency of incident cognitive impairment (noncumulative) was greater in stroke survivors than those without stroke (19.2% vs 8.7%; P < .001) (absolute difference, 10.6% [95% CI, 8.1% to 13.0%]; P < .001).
Changes in New Learning and Verbal Memory After Stroke
Table 3 and Figure 2 show that incident stroke was associated with significant acute declines in new learning and verbal memory after the event (WLL, 1.80 points [95% CI, 0.73 to 2.86]; P = .001; WLD, 0.60 points [95% CI, 0.13 to 1.07]; P = .01). New learning and verbal memory scores increased slightly over time before stroke but less so in black participants (P = .01 for WLL and P = .02 for WLD for race × time interaction term). We did not detect significant changes in the slopes of new learning or verbal memory after incident stroke compared with prestroke slopes (P = .91 for WLL and P = .70 for WLD for change in slope after incident stroke).
Changes in Executive Function After Stroke
Executive function declined significantly over time before stroke (0.31 points per year [95% CI, 0.27 to 0.35]; P < .001) (Table 3). Stroke was associated with an acute decline in executive function (0.90 points [95% CI, 0.23 to 1.57]; P = .009) in Model A but not in Model B (Table 3). In the years following an incident stroke, executive function declined significantly faster than it did before the stroke (change in slope after incident stroke, 0.63 points per year [95% CI, 0.12 to 1.15]; P = 0.02) (Figure 2) (prestroke slope, −0.312; poststroke slope, −0.944).
Results were similar in analyses excluding individuals with baseline history of stroke, imputing missing values of baseline covariates, adjusting for baseline renal function and history of myocardial infarction, and adjusting for death (eTables 5-8 in the Supplement). Cognitive changes after stroke persisted if participants were required to have 2 or more follow-up cognitive measures, but some changes in secondary outcomes were no longer statistically significant (eTable 9 in the Supplement). In analyses including stroke type, results for ischemic stroke were similar; results for hemorrhagic stroke were consistent, although some were no longer statistically significant (eTable 10 in the Supplement).
Quiz Ref IDIn this national cohort of black and white US residents 45 years or older, incident stroke was associated with accelerated and persistent declines in global cognition and executive function, after accounting for individuals’ cognitive changes before and acutely after the event. Stroke survivors had a significantly faster rate of incident cognitive impairment after stroke compared with the prestroke rate (odds ratio, 1.23 per year [95% CI, 1.10 to 1.38]; P < .001), controlling for the odds of developing cognitive impairment before or acutely after the event. We found significant, acute declines in new learning and verbal memory after stroke but no acceleration of prestroke rates of change in these functions.
Our data suggest that cognitive function declines both acutely and over the long term after incident stroke. Several studies9,13,14 have suggested that cognitive decline does not accelerate after stroke, unless a recurrent stroke occurs. Our results may differ because we had actual patient measures of prestroke cognition (not proxy-reported measures13,14), used expert-adjudicated strokes and dates (not self-report9), and used different cognitive measures.
The acute declines in global cognition, new learning, and verbal memory associated with stroke are likely clinically meaningful. A decline of 0.5 or more standard deviations from baseline has been defined as clinically meaningful decline,38 has been correlated with clinically meaningful decline in global cognition in a cohort of cognitively normal adults 50 years or older,39 and, for the CERAD battery, has been correlated with other established measures of cognitive decline in older adults with dementia.40 A 0.5-SD decrease from the baseline score for each outcome is approximately 0.2 points for the SIS, 2.4 points for the WLL, 1.0 points for the WLD, and 2.4 points for the AFT. The 95% confidence intervals for the acute cognitive declines in global cognition, new learning, and verbal memory after stroke include declines of this magnitude. Acute cognitive decline after stroke increases survivors’ risk of mortality,41 disability,3,4 and dependent living3,4 and decreases their quality of life.42 The long-term declines in global cognition and executive function parallel the long-term functional decline seen in stroke survivors.43 Moreover, declines in global cognition and executive function significantly increase the risk of mortality,44 dementia,17,45 depression,46 and accelerated functional decline,47,48 which in turn is associated with institutionalization and caregiver burden.
Quiz Ref IDIncident stroke or its risk factors may cause long-term cognitive decline through several mechanisms. Stroke may induce or exacerbate neurodegenerative disease,49,50 or neurodegenerative disease may amplify brain injury and cognitive deficits after stroke.51 Vascular risk factors52 or an immune response53 may cause ongoing cerebrovascular injury, inflammation, and oxidative stress. Moreover, stroke survivors may experience incident comorbidity (eg, cardiac disease).54 It is unlikely that clinically apparent recurrent strokes or baseline atrial fibrillation explain the long-term cognitive declines that we observed, because we censored cognitive information for participants at the time of recurrent stroke and adjusting for atrial fibrillation did not change our results. Still, stroke survivors may have had subclinical infarcts after their index stroke that contributed to subsequent cognitive decline.55 We did not have brain imaging data subsequent to the incident stroke. Our findings suggest a scientific need to determine whether the acute and also accelerated long-term cognitive decline are the result of incomplete rehabilitation from the initial stroke, subsequent vascular injury attributable to uncontrolled risk factors,56 behavioral changes, or other mechanisms.
Our study has several strengths. We had longitudinal cognitive assessments in a cohort and stroke subset of sufficient size to estimate before and after differences (and the acute change) in cognitive decline. Incident strokes were expert-adjudicated based on medical record review. REGARDS systematically measured cognitive domains commonly affected by stroke: global cognition, learning, memory, and executive function.3,57 We accounted for prestroke cognitive decline and acute cognitive declines after stroke to disentangle the association between stroke and longitudinal cognitive decline.
Our study has limitations. Results are generalizable only to community-dwelling stroke survivors not requiring a proxy respondent (eg, without aphasia). Although excluded participants had higher prevalence of stroke and dementia risk factors than included participants, these differences would reduce the ability to detect the cognitive effects of stroke. We were unable to control for stroke features (location, laterality, severity13), acute stroke treatments, or heart failure because these data were unavailable. Selective attrition may lead to underestimation of cognitive decline because participants with worse cognition at baseline or after stroke die, drop out, or require a proxy.58 Analyses that accounted for loss to follow-up or death did not change our results, consistent with research from Salthouse.59
Fewer incident strokes and cognitive observations potentially limited statistical power to detect changes in the secondary outcomes (eg, verbal memory14,15). The linear mixed-effects models perform well for sparse data with small numbers of repeated measures; still, the results of the secondary outcomes may require confirmatory analysis with more observations of cognition and incident stroke. Although stroke may exacerbate depression, we did not adjust for time-dependent depressive symptom scores because depressive symptoms are often comorbid with cognitive decline and therefore on the causal pathway. The slight increases in global cognition, new learning, and verbal memory over time before stroke may be attributable to practice effects.39,60 We did not have data on functional impairments or incident dementia. The approach taken to defining clinically meaningful changes, by using a threshold of change exceeding 0.5 or greater SD, is a common approach, but it does not provide a clear intuition of actual clinical impact, and a clinically meaningful change may vary by an individual’s age, education, sex, and baseline cognition.60 Measurement of poststroke cognition during the early to mid-stage recovery phase may lead to underestimation of acute cognitive decline.
Our study has potential implications for clinical practice, research, and health care policy. Although clinical practice guidelines and quality improvement programs recommend cognitive assessments be performed for patients with stroke before hospital discharge and also in the postacute settings,61,62 our results suggest that stroke survivors also warrant monitoring for mounting cognitive impairment over the years after the event. Moreover, our results suggest that long-term cognitive dysfunction is a potential domain for evaluating acute stroke therapies. As adults increasingly survive stroke,63 cases of poststroke cognitive impairment will multiply.5 Given that poststroke cognitive impairment increases mortality, morbidity, and health care costs,64 health systems and payers will need to develop cost-effective systems of care that will best manage the long-term needs and cognitive problems of this increasing and vulnerable stroke survivor population.
Incident stroke was associated with acute decline in cognitive function and also accelerated and persistent cognitive decline over 6 years.
Corresponding Author: Deborah A. Levine, MD, MPH, Division of General Medicine, University of Michigan, NCRC, 2800 Plymouth Rd, Bldg 16, Room 430W, Ann Arbor, MI 48109-2800 (deblevin@umich.edu).
Author Contributions: Dr Levine had full access to all of the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.
Study concept and design: Levine, Galecki, Kabeto, Giordani, Wadley.
Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: Levine, Langa, Unverzagt, Kabeto, Giordani, Wadley.
Drafting of the manuscript: Levine, Galecki, Giordani.
Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Levine, Langa, Unverzagt, Kabeto, Giordani, Wadley.
Statistical analysis: Levine, Galecki, Kabeto.
Obtained funding: Levine.
Administrative, technical, or material support: Levine, Unverzagt, Kabeto, Giordani.
Study supervision: Levine, Galecki, Unverzagt, Giordani.
Conflict of Interest Disclosures: All authors have completed and submitted the ICMJE Form for Disclosure of Potential Conflicts of Interest. Dr Levine reported receiving consulting fees from AstraZeneca and the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke for work on clinical trials; receiving a grant from the Michigan Alzheimer’s Disease Center; and serving as a member of the program advisory committee for the Kaiser Permanente Northern California (KPNC)–University of California, San Francisco (UCSF) Stroke Prevention/Intervention Research Program (SPIRP). No other authors reported disclosures.
Funding/Support: This work was supported by cooperative agreement U01 NS041588 from the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke, National Institutes of Health, Department of Health and Human Services. Additional funding was provided by a grant K23AG040278 from the National Institute on Aging (Dr Levine).
Role of the Funder/Sponsor: Representatives of the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke have been involved in the review of the manuscript but not directly involved in the collection, management, analysis, or interpretation of the data; or the decision to submit the manuscript for publication.
Disclaimer: The content of this article is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke, the National Institutes of Health, or the Department of Veterans Affairs.
Additional Contributions: We thank the other investigators, the staff, and the participants of the REGARDS study for their valuable contributions. A full list of participating REGARDS investigators and institutions can be found at http://www.regardsstudy.org.
1.Mozaffarian
D, Benjamin
EJ, Go
AS,
et al; American Heart Association Statistics Committee and Stroke Statistics Subcommittee. Heart disease and stroke statistics—2015 update: a report from the American Heart Association.
Circulation. 2015;131(4):e29-e322.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 2.Murray
CJ, Atkinson
C, Bhalla
K,
et al; U.S. Burden of Disease Collaborators. The state of US health, 1990-2010: burden of diseases, injuries, and risk factors.
JAMA. 2013;310(6):591-608.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 3.Tatemichi
TK, Desmond
DW, Stern
Y, Paik
M, Sano
M, Bagiella
E. Cognitive impairment after stroke: frequency, patterns, and relationship to functional abilities.
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1994;57(2):202-207.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 4.Patel
MD, Coshall
C, Rudd
AG, Wolfe
CD. Cognitive impairment after stroke: clinical determinants and its associations with long-term stroke outcomes.
J Am Geriatr Soc. 2002;50(4):700-706.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 5.Ukraintseva
S, Sloan
F, Arbeev
K, Yashin
A. Increasing rates of dementia at time of declining mortality from stroke.
Stroke. 2006;37(5):1155-1159.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 6.Edwards
JD, Koehoorn
M, Boyd
LA, Levy
AR. Is health-related quality of life improving after stroke? a comparison of health utilities indices among Canadians with stroke between 1996 and 2005.
Stroke. 2010;41(5):996-1000.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 7.Erkinjuntti
T, Gauthier
S. Diagnosing vascular cognitive impairment and dementia: concepts and controversies. In: Wahlund
L, Erkinjuntti
T, Gauthier
S, eds. Vascular Cognitive Impairment in Clinical Practice. Cambridge, United Kingdom: Cambridge University Press; 2009:3-10.
8.Rajan
KB, Aggarwal
NT, Wilson
RS, Everson-Rose
SA, Evans
DA. Association of cognitive functioning, incident stroke, and mortality in older adults.
Stroke. 2014;45(9):2563-2567.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 9.Wang
Q, Capistrant
BD, Ehntholt
A, Glymour
MM. Long-term rate of change in memory functioning before and after stroke onset.
Stroke. 2012;43(10):2561-2566.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 10.del Ser
T, Barba
R, Morin
MM,
et al. Evolution of cognitive impairment after stroke and risk factors for delayed progression.
Stroke. 2005;36(12):2670-2675.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 11.Tatemichi
TK, Desmond
DW, Mayeux
R,
et al. Dementia after stroke: baseline frequency, risks, and clinical features in a hospitalized cohort.
Neurology. 1992;42(6):1185-1193.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 12.Liman
TG, Heuschmann
PU, Endres
M, Flöel
A, Schwab
S, Kolominsky-Rabas
PL. Changes in cognitive function over 3 years after first-ever stroke and predictors of cognitive impairment and long-term cognitive stability: the Erlangen Stroke Project.
Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2011;31(4):291-299.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 13.Srikanth
VK, Quinn
SJ, Donnan
GA, Saling
MM, Thrift
AG. Long-term cognitive transitions, rates of cognitive change, and predictors of incident dementia in a population-based first-ever stroke cohort.
Stroke. 2006;37(10):2479-2483.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 14.Sachdev
PS, Lipnicki
DM, Crawford
JD, Wen
W, Brodaty
H. Progression of cognitive impairment in stroke/TIA patients over 3 years.
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2014;85(12):1324-1330.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 15.Knopman
DS, Mosley
TH, Catellier
DJ, Coker
LH; Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities Study Brain MRI Study. Fourteen-year longitudinal study of vascular risk factors, APOE genotype, and cognition: the ARIC MRI Study.
Alzheimers Dement. 2009;5(3):207-214.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 16.Howard
VJ, Cushman
M, Pulley
L,
et al. The reasons for geographic and racial differences in stroke study: objectives and design.
Neuroepidemiology. 2005;25(3):135-143.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 17.Callahan
CM, Unverzagt
FW, Hui
SL, Perkins
AJ, Hendrie
HC. Six-item screener to identify cognitive impairment among potential subjects for clinical research.
Med Care. 2002;40(9):771-781.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 18.Morris
JC, Heyman
A, Mohs
RC,
et al. The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD), part I: clinical and neuropsychological assessment of Alzheimer’s disease.
Neurology. 1989;39(9):1159-1165.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 19.Morris
JC, Mohs
RC, Rogers
H, Fillenbaum
G, Heyman
A. Consortium to establish a registry for Alzheimer’s disease (CERAD) clinical and neuropsychological assessment of Alzheimer’s disease.
Psychopharmacol Bull. 1988;24(4):641-652.
PubMedGoogle Scholar 20.Unverzagt
FW, Monahan
PO, Moser
LR,
et al. The Indiana University telephone-based assessment of neuropsychological status: a new method for large scale neuropsychological assessment.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2007;13(5):799-806.
PubMedGoogle Scholar 21.Rapp
SR, Legault
C, Espeland
MA,
et al; CAT Study Group. Validation of a cognitive assessment battery administered over the telephone.
J Am Geriatr Soc. 2012;60(9):1616-1623.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 22.Wilson
RS, Leurgans
SE, Foroud
TM,
et al; National Institute on Aging Late-Onset Alzheimer’s Disease Family Study Group. Telephone assessment of cognitive function in the late-onset Alzheimer’s disease family study.
Arch Neurol. 2010;67(7):855-861.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 23.Hachinski
V, Iadecola
C, Petersen
RC,
et al. National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke–Canadian Stroke Network vascular cognitive impairment harmonization standards [published correction appears in
Stroke. 2007;38(3):1118].
Stroke. 2006;37(9):2220-2241.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 24.Ferraro
FR, ed. Minority and Cross-Cultural Aspects of Neuropsychological Assessment (Studies on Neuropsychology, Neurology and Cognition). Lisse, the Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger; 2002.
25.Lucas
JA, Ivnik
RJ, Smith
GE,
et al. Mayo’s Older African Americans Normative Studies: norms for Boston Naming Test, Controlled Oral Word Association, Category Fluency, Animal Naming, Token Test, WRAT-3 Reading, Trail Making Test, Stroop Test, and Judgment of Line Orientation.
Clin Neuropsychol. 2005;19(2):243-269.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 26.Carpenter
CR, DesPain
B, Keeling
TN, Shah
M, Rothenberger
M. The Six-Item Screener and AD8 for the detection of cognitive impairment in geriatric emergency department patients.
Ann Emerg Med. 2011;57(6):653-661.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 27.Howard
VJ, Kleindorfer
DO, Judd
SE,
et al. Disparities in stroke incidence contributing to disparities in stroke mortality.
Ann Neurol. 2011;69(4):619-627.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 28.Safford
MM, Brown
TM, Muntner
PM,
et al; REGARDS Investigators. Association of race and sex with risk of incident acute coronary heart disease events.
JAMA. 2012;308(17):1768-1774.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 29. Stroke—1989: recommendations on stroke prevention, diagnosis, and therapy: report of the WHO Task Force on Stroke and other Cerebrovascular Disorders.
Stroke. 1989;20(10):1407-1431.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 30.Halanych
JH, Shuaib
F, Parmar
G,
et al. Agreement on cause of death between proxies, death certificates, and clinician adjudicators in the Reasons for Geographic and Racial Differences in Stroke (REGARDS) study.
Am J Epidemiol. 2011;173(11):1319-1326.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 31.Levey
AS, Stevens
LA, Schmid
CH,
et al; CKD-EPI (Chronic Kidney Disease Epidemiology Collaboration). A new equation to estimate glomerular filtration rate.
Ann Intern Med. 2009;150(9):604-612.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 32.Melchior
LA, Huba
GJ, Brown
VB, Reback
CJ. A short depression index for women.
Educ Psychol Meas. 1993;53(4):1117-1125.
Google ScholarCrossref 33.Hertzog
C, Dixon
R. Methodological issues in research on cognition and aging. In: Blanchard-Fields
F, Hess
TM, eds. Perspectives on Cognitive Change in Adulthood and Aging. New York, NY: Mcgraw-Hill College; 1996:66-121.
34.West
BG, Galecki
AT. Linear Mixed Models: A Practical Guide Using Statistical Software. Boca Raton, LA: Chapman & Hall/CRC; 2007.
36.Levine
DA, Davydow
DS, Hough
CL, Langa
KM, Rogers
MA, Iwashyna
TJ. Functional disability and cognitive impairment after hospitalization for myocardial infarction and stroke.
Circ Cardiovasc Qual Outcomes. 2014;7(6):863-871.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 37.He
Y. Missing data analysis using multiple imputation: getting to the heart of the matter.
Circ Cardiovasc Qual Outcomes. 2010;3(1):98-105.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 38.Wolinsky
FD, Unverzagt
FW, Smith
DM, Jones
R, Stoddard
A, Tennstedt
SL. The ACTIVE cognitive training trial and health-related quality of life: protection that lasts for 5 years.
J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2006;61(12):1324-1329.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 39.Unger
JM, van Belle
G, Heyman
A; Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease. Cross-sectional versus longitudinal estimates of cognitive change in nondemented older people: a CERAD study.
J Am Geriatr Soc. 1999;47(5):559-563.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 40.Rossetti
HC, Munro Cullum
C, Hynan
LS, Lacritz
LH. The CERAD Neuropsychologic Battery Total Score and the progression of Alzheimer disease.
Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2010;24(2):138-142.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 41.Tatemichi
TK, Paik
M, Bagiella
E, Desmond
DW, Pirro
M, Hanzawa
LK. Dementia after stroke is a predictor of long-term survival.
Stroke. 1994;25(10):1915-1919.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 42.Ankolekar
S, Renton
C, Sare
G,
et al; ENOS Trial Investigators. Relationship between poststroke cognition, baseline factors, and functional outcome: data from “efficacy of nitric oxide in stroke” trial.
J Stroke Cerebrovasc Dis. 2014;23(7):1821-1829.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 43.Dhamoon
MS, Moon
YP, Paik
MC,
et al. Long-term functional recovery after first ischemic stroke: the Northern Manhattan Study.
Stroke. 2009;40(8):2805-2811.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 44.Cosentino
S, Scarmeas
N, Albert
SM, Stern
Y. Verbal fluency predicts mortality in Alzheimer disease.
Cogn Behav Neurol. 2006;19(3):123-129.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 45.Clark
LJ, Gatz
M, Zheng
L, Chen
YL, McCleary
C, Mack
WJ. Longitudinal verbal fluency in normal aging, preclinical, and prevalent Alzheimer’s disease.
Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Demen. 2009;24(6):461-468.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 46.Unützer
J, Katon
W, Callahan
CM,
et al. Depression treatment in a sample of 1,801 depressed older adults in primary care.
J Am Geriatr Soc. 2003;51(4):505-514.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 47.Atchison
TB, Massman
PJ, Doody
RS. Baseline cognitive function predicts rate of decline in basic-care abilities of individuals with dementia of the Alzheimer’s type.
Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2007;22(1):99-107.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 48.Bennett
HP, Corbett
AJ, Gaden
S, Grayson
DA, Kril
JJ, Broe
GA. Subcortical vascular disease and functional decline: a 6-year predictor study.
J Am Geriatr Soc. 2002;50(12):1969-1977.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 49.Snowdon
DA, Greiner
LH, Mortimer
JA, Riley
KP, Greiner
PA, Markesbery
WR. Brain infarction and the clinical expression of Alzheimer disease: the Nun Study.
JAMA. 1997;277(10):813-817.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 50.Garcia-Alloza
M, Gregory
J, Kuchibhotla
KV,
et al. Cerebrovascular lesions induce transient β-amyloid deposition.
Brain. 2011;134(pt 12):3697-3707.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 51.Whitehead
SN, Cheng
G, Hachinski
VC, Cechetto
DF. Progressive increase in infarct size, neuroinflammation, and cognitive deficits in the presence of high levels of amyloid.
Stroke. 2007;38(12):3245-3250.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 52.Iadecola
C, Gorelick
PB. Converging pathogenic mechanisms in vascular and neurodegenerative dementia.
Stroke. 2003;34(2):335-337.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 53.Doyle
KP, Quach
LN, Solé
M,
et al. B-lymphocyte-mediated delayed cognitive impairment following stroke.
J Neurosci. 2015;35(5):2133-2145.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 54.Schaapsmeerders
P, Maaijwee
NA, van Dijk
EJ,
et al. Long-term cognitive impairment after first-ever ischemic stroke in young adults.
Stroke. 2013;44(6):1621-1628.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 55.Schneider
JA, Boyle
PA, Arvanitakis
Z, Bienias
JL, Bennett
DA. Subcortical infarcts, Alzheimer’s disease pathology, and memory function in older persons.
Ann Neurol. 2007;62(1):59-66.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 56.Douiri
A, McKevitt
C, Emmett
ES, Rudd
AG, Wolfe
CD. Long-term effects of secondary prevention on cognitive function in stroke patients.
Circulation. 2013;128(12):1341-1348.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 57.Reed
BR, Eberling
JL, Mungas
D, Weiner
MW, Jagust
WJ. Memory failure has different mechanisms in subcortical stroke and Alzheimer’s disease.
Ann Neurol. 2000;48(3):275-284.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 58.Desmond
DW, Bagiella
E, Moroney
JT, Stern
Y. The effect of patient attrition on estimates of the frequency of dementia following stroke.
Arch Neurol. 1998;55(3):390-394.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 59.Salthouse
TA. Selectivity of attrition in longitudinal studies of cognitive functioning.
J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2014;69(4):567-574.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 60.Stein
J, Luppa
M, Luck
T,
et al. The assessment of changes in cognitive functioning: age-, education-, and gender-specific reliable change indices for older adults tested on the CERAD-NP battery: results of the German Study on Ageing, Cognition, and Dementia in Primary Care Patients (AgeCoDe).
Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2012;20(1):84-97.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 62.Miller
EL, Murray
L, Richards
L,
et al; American Heart Association Council on Cardiovascular Nursing and the Stroke Council. Comprehensive overview of nursing and interdisciplinary rehabilitation care of the stroke patient: a scientific statement from the American Heart Association.
Stroke. 2010;41(10):2402-2448.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 63.Koton
S, Schneider
AL, Rosamond
WD,
et al. Stroke incidence and mortality trends in US communities, 1987 to 2011.
JAMA. 2014;312(3):259-268.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 64.Rockwood
K, Brown
M, Merry
H, Sketris
I, Fisk
J; Vascular Cognitive Impairment Investigators of the Canadian Study of Health and Aging. Societal costs of vascular cognitive impairment in older adults.
Stroke. 2002;33(6):1605-1609.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref