[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Figure 1.  Mean Test Score Difference Between Children With and Without Type 1 Diabetes—Linear Model
Mean Test Score Difference Between Children With and Without Type 1 Diabetes—Linear Model

For top and bottom panels, n = 608 655 children and n = 1 561 770 test scores (n = 1878 children with diabetes and n = 4234 test scores). For center panel, n = 2863 children and n = 7344 test scores (n = 1257 children with diabetes and n = 2966 test scores). All models are adjusted for grade-, topic-, and year-specific effects (bottom panel is only adjusted for year-specific effects). In models adjusted for socioeconomic status (SES), adjustment was made for sex of child, immigration status, birth order, number of siblings, living with both parents, maternal/paternal educational attainment, maternal/paternal income, maternal/paternal diabetes status, and zip code.

Figure 2.  Mean Test Score Differences Between Subgroups of Children With vs Without Type 1 Diabetes—Linear Model
Mean Test Score Differences Between Subgroups of Children With vs Without Type 1 Diabetes—Linear Model

For no diabetes, n = 606 777 children and n = 1 557 536 test scores; for diabetes onset at younger than 6 years, n = 564 children and n = 1391 test scores; diabetes onset at 6 years or older, n = 1314 children and n = 2843 test scores; diabetes duration greater than 4 years, n = 799 children and n = 1659 test scores; diabetes duration 4 years or less, n = 1324 children and n = 2575 test scores; hemoglobin A1c (HbA1c) level greater than 7.5%, n = 1155 children and n = 2354 test scores; HbA1c level 7.5% or less, n = 728 children and n = 1330 test scores; HbA1c level greater than 8.6%, n = 472 children and n = 867 test scores; HbA1c level 8.6% or less, n = 1388 children and n = 2817 test scores; diabetic ketoacidosis (DKA) at onset, n = 336 children and n = 755 test scores; no DKA at onset, n = 1542 children and n = 3479 test scores. All models are adjusted for grade-, topic-, and year-specific effects. In models adjusted for socioeconomic status (SES), adjustment was made for sex of child, immigration status, birth order, number of siblings, living with both parents, maternal/paternal educational attainment, maternal/paternal income, maternal/paternal diabetes status, and zip code.

Figure 3.  Local Kernel–Weighted Mean of Test Score on HbA1c Level Among Children With Type 1 Diabetes (n = 1729 Children [3684 Test Scores])
Local Kernel–Weighted Mean of Test Score on HbA1c Level Among Children With Type 1 Diabetes (n = 1729 Children [3684 Test Scores])

Local kernel–weighted mean of test score on hemoglobin A1c (HbA1c) measure. Shaded areas indicate 95% CIs; dashed lines indicate population mean test scores. The lowest and highest 1% of the HbA1c distribution is not shown. Panel B is adjusted for sex of child, immigration status, birth order, number of siblings, living with both parents, maternal/paternal educational attainment, maternal/paternal income, maternal/paternal diabetes status, zip code residence, and diabetes duration. Kernel-weighted mean adjustment was performed by obtaining the residuals from a linear projection of test scores on the socioeconomic covariates (y-axis) and the residuals from a linear projection of HbA1c values on the socioeconomic covariates (x-axis).23 The linear slope coefficient stems from a linear regression model for test scores with and without adjusting for covariates. SES indicates socioeconomic status.

Table 1.  Background Characteristics of Children With and Without Type 1 Diabetesa
Background Characteristics of Children With and Without Type 1 Diabetesa
Table 2.  Linear Regression Models—Test Score Outcomes for Children With and Without Diabetesa
Linear Regression Models—Test Score Outcomes for Children With and Without Diabetesa
1.
Nathan  DM, Genuth  S, Lachin  J,  et al; Diabetes Control and Complications Trial Research Group.  The effect of intensive treatment of diabetes on the development and progression of long-term complications in insulin-dependent diabetes mellitus.  N Engl J Med. 1993;329(14):977-986. doi:10.1056/NEJM199309303291401PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
McCrimmon  RJ, Ryan  CM, Frier  BM.  Diabetes and cognitive dysfunction.  Lancet. 2012;379(9833):2291-2299. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(12)60360-2PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
Ferguson  SC, Blane  A, Wardlaw  J,  et al.  Influence of an early-onset age of type 1 diabetes on cerebral structure and cognitive function.  Diabetes Care. 2005;28(6):1431-1437. doi:10.2337/diacare.28.6.1431PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Hannonen  R, Komulainen  J, Riikonen  R,  et al; Study Group.  Academic skills in children with early-onset type 1 diabetes: the effects of diabetes-related risk factors.  Dev Med Child Neurol. 2012;54(5):457-463. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8749.2012.04248.xPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Semenkovich  K, Patel  PP, Pollock  AB,  et al.  Academic abilities and glycaemic control in children and young people with type 1 diabetes mellitus.  Diabet Med. 2016;33(5):668-673. doi:10.1111/dme.12854PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Persson  S, Dahlquist  G, Gerdtham  UG, Steen Carlsson  K.  Impact of childhood-onset type 1 diabetes on schooling: a population-based register study.  Diabetologia. 2013;56(6):1254-1262. doi:10.1007/s00125-013-2870-8PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Hannonen  R, Komulainen  J, Eklund  K, Tolvanen  A, Riikonen  R, Ahonen  T.  Verbal and academic skills in children with early-onset type 1 diabetes.  Dev Med Child Neurol. 2010;52(7):e143-e147. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8749.2010.03648.xPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
McCarthy  AM, Lindgren  S, Mengeling  MA, Tsalikian  E, Engvall  JC.  Effects of diabetes on learning in children.  Pediatrics. 2002;109(1):E9. doi:10.1542/peds.109.1.e9PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Patiño-Fernández  AM, Delamater  AM, Applegate  EB,  et al.  Neurocognitive functioning in preschool-age children with type 1 diabetes mellitus.  Pediatr Diabetes. 2010;11(6):424-430. doi:10.1111/j.1399-5448.2009.00618.xPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Ly  TT, Anderson  M, McNamara  KA, Davis  EA, Jones  TW.  Neurocognitive outcomes in young adults with early-onset type 1 diabetes: a prospective follow-up study.  Diabetes Care. 2011;34(10):2192-2197. doi:10.2337/dc11-0697PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
Cooper  MN, McNamara  KA, de Klerk  NH, Davis  EA, Jones  TW.  School performance in children with type 1 diabetes: a contemporary population-based study.  Pediatr Diabetes. 2016;17(2):101-111. doi:10.1111/pedi.12243PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Gejl  M, Gjedde  A, Brock  B,  et al.  Effects of hypoglycaemia on working memory and regional cerebral blood flow in type 1 diabetes: a randomised, crossover trial.  Diabetologia. 2018;61(3):551-561. doi:10.1007/s00125-017-4502-1PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Allen  KV, Pickering  MJ, Zammitt  NN,  et al.  Effects of acute hypoglycemia on working memory and language processing in adults with and without type 1 diabetes.  Diabetes Care. 2015;38(6):1108-1115. doi:10.2337/dc14-1657PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
McCarthy  AM, Lindgren  S, Mengeling  MA, Tsalikian  E, Engvall  J.  Factors associated with academic achievement in children with type 1 diabetes.  Diabetes Care. 2003;26(1):112-117. doi:10.2337/diacare.26.1.112PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Svensson  J, Cerqueira  C, Kjærsgaard  P,  et al.  Danish Registry of Childhood and Adolescent Diabetes.  Clin Epidemiol. 2016;8:679-683. doi:10.2147/CLEP.S99469PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Wandall  J.  National tests in Denmark—CAT as a pedagogic tool.  J Appl Testing Technol. 2011;12(special issue):1-21.Google Scholar
17.
Rasch  G.  Probabilistic Models for Some Intelligence and Attainment Tests. Copenhagen, Denmark: Dansk Paedagogisk Institut; 1960.
18.
Beuchert  L, Nandrup  A. The Danish National Tests at a glance. Danish Economic Journal website. https://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=2996431. July 6, 2017. Accessed December 26, 2018.
19.
Andersen  SC, Humlum  MK, Nandrup  AB.  Increasing instruction time in school does increase learning.  Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2016;113(27):7481-7484. doi:10.1073/pnas.1516686113PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Elkjær  LS, Bech  BH, Sun  Y, Laursen  TM, Christensen  J.  Association between prenatal valproate exposure and performance on standardized language and mathematics tests in school-aged children.  JAMA Neurol. 2018;75(6):663-671. doi:10.1001/jamaneurol.2017.5035PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
Sievertsen  HH, Gino  F, Piovesan  M.  Cognitive fatigue influences students’ performance on standardized tests.  Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2016;113(10):2621-2624. doi:10.1073/pnas.1516947113PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
International Society for Pediatric and Adolescent Diabetes (ISPAD). ISPAD clinical practice guidelines 2018. ISPAD website. https://www.ispad.org/page/ISPADGuidelines2018. 2018. Accessed December 14, 2018.
23.
Lovell  MC.  Seasonal adjustment of economic time series and multiple regression analysis.  J Am Stat Assoc. 1963;58:993-1010. doi:10.1080/01621459.1963.10480682Google ScholarCrossref
24.
Steineck  I, Cederholm  J, Eliasson  B,  et al; Swedish National Diabetes Register.  Insulin pump therapy, multiple daily injections, and cardiovascular mortality in 18,168 people with type 1 diabetes: observational study.  BMJ. 2015;350:h3234. doi:10.1136/bmj.h3234PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
Original Investigation
February 5, 2019

Association of Type 1 Diabetes With Standardized Test Scores of Danish Schoolchildren

Author Affiliations
  • 1Department of Economics and Business Economics, Aarhus University, Aarhus, Denmark
  • 2Department of Economics, Kansas State University, Manhattan
  • 3Copenhagen University Hospital, Copenhagen, Denmark
  • 4VIVE—The Danish Center for Social Science Research, Aarhus, Denmark
  • 5Department of Economics, University of Copenhagen, Denmark
  • 6Center for Economic Behavior and Inequality (CEBI), University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen, Denmark
JAMA. 2019;321(5):484-492. doi:10.1001/jama.2018.21819
Key Points

Question  What is the association between type 1 diabetes and standardized test scores in schoolchildren?

Findings  In this population-based retrospective cohort study including 631 620 Danish public schoolchildren, there was no significant difference in standardized reading and mathematics test scores of children with type 1 diabetes compared with test scores of children without diabetes (adjusted test score difference, 0.45 points; test score range, 1-100 points).

Meaning  Among Danish schoolchildren, type 1 diabetes was not significantly associated with differences in standardized test scores.

Abstract

Importance  Type 1 diabetes has been associated with cardiovascular disease and late complications such as retinopathy and nephropathy. However, it is unclear whether there is an association between type 1 diabetes and school performance in children.

Objective  To compare standardized reading and mathematics test scores of schoolchildren with type 1 diabetes vs those without diabetes.

Design, Setting, and Participants  Population-based retrospective cohort study from January 1, 2011, to December 31, 2015 (end date of follow-up), including Danish public schoolchildren attending grades 2, 3, 4, 6, and 8. Test scores were obtained in math (n = 524 764) and reading (n = 1 037 006). Linear regression models compared outcomes with and without adjustment for socioeconomic characteristics.

Exposures  Type 1 diabetes.

Main Outcomes and Measures  Primary outcomes were pooled test scores in math and reading (range, 1-100).

Results  Among 631 620 included public schoolchildren, the mean (SD) age was 10.31 (SD, 2.42) years, and 51% were male; 2031 had a confirmed diagnosis of type 1 diabetes. Overall, the mean combined score in math and reading was 56.11 (SD, 24.93). There were no significant differences in test scores found between children with type 1 diabetes (mean, 56.56) and children without diabetes (mean, 56.11; difference, 0.45 [95% CI, −0.31 to 1.22]). The estimated difference in test scores between children with and without type 1 diabetes from a linear regression model with adjustment for grade, test topic, and year was 0.24 (95% CI, −0.90 to 1.39) and 0.45 (95% CI, −0.58 to 1.49) with additional adjustment for socioeconomic status.

Conclusions and Relevance  Among Danish public schoolchildren, there was no significant difference in standardized reading and mathematics test scores of children with type 1 diabetes compared with test scores of children without diabetes.

Introduction

Type 1 diabetes leads to microvascular and macrovascular complications1 and increased risk of cardiovascular mortality. The elevated risk of neuroglycopenia and hyperglycemia associated with diabetes has been suggested to affect cognitive performance,2 but results are mixed. Some studies have linked diabetes in children to lower academic performance,3-7 while other studies have found no differences in performance when comparing children with and without diabetes.8-11 Other studies have focused on the association between adverse diabetes outcomes such as hypoglycemia, diabetic ketoacidosis, and poor metabolic control and cognitive skills. A recent study found that hypoglycemia adversely affects the working memory of the brain.12 Furthermore, research has shown that mild hypoglycemia has a negative effect on language processing,13 and poor metabolic control has been associated with lower academic achievement.4,8,9,14 However, most of these studies were conducted on smaller, nonrandom samples of patients with diabetes.

In Denmark, all children in public schools are tested in reading and math using standardized tests. The primary objective of this study was to compare test scores for children with diabetes with scores for children without diabetes, with and without adjustment for socioeconomic status.

Methods

The study was approved by the Danish Data Protection Agency. The Danish Registry of Childhood and Adolescent Diabetes (DanDiabKids) was approved by the Danish Health Data Authority (file No. 14/915976). These approvals constitute the necessary legal requirements, and informed consent is not required.

This study was a population-based retrospective registry study of all children attending grades 2, 3, 4, 6, and 8 in Danish public schools from January 1, 2011, to December 31, 2015. Because tests were only mandatory for students in public schools, the sample was restricted to students who attended public schools. Students were tested in reading in grades 2, 4, 6, and 8 and in math in grades 3 and 6.

Students were initially identified through Statistics Denmark and subsequently linked with background information from different administrative registries, based on unique personal identity numbers. These data were then augmented with test scores from the Danish National Tests. The DanDiabKids database15 was used to identify the population of children diagnosed with diabetes (including yearly measures of hemoglobin A1c [HbA1c]). Children with diabetes who had at least 1 test score and children with diabetes who had at least 1 HbA1c measurement before a test were included. DanDiabKids is a national database containing information based on patient journals, clinical examination, and blood samples. Data are uploaded to DanDiabKids by clinicians from all pediatric clinics in Denmark, in connection to yearly visits scheduled in close proximity to each patient’s birthday.

Outcome Measures

The pooled reading and math test score was the primary outcome. Each test evaluates a student’s abilities within 3 profile areas. In reading, these are language comprehension, decoding, and reading comprehension. In math, they are numbers and algebra, geometry, and mathematics in use.16 The tests are computer-based, adaptive (students are presented with questions of varying difficulty drawn from a national question bank based on a computerized continuous assessment of the student’s proficiency level), and objective (teachers cannot influence scoring or questions asked). Tests are initially scored on a computer according to a Rasch model17 within each profile area18 and then mapped (by the Danish Ministry of Education) into a single score in math and a single score in reading, with scores ranging between 1 and 100.

The tests have been shown to be highly predictive of exit examination grades and have been used for other research purposes.18-21 The tests are considered low stakes compared with exit examinations because they are only used to provide feedback on learning progress to students and not used for sanctioning of teachers or schools.

Because socioeconomic status can influence academic outcomes, the association between diabetes and test scores was adjusted for this potential confounder. In addition, subgroup analyses of the primary outcome were performed to assess factors that might affect the association, including early onset of diabetes, duration of diabetes, and diabetes control measured by HbA1c level and presence of diabetic ketoacidosis.

Secondary outcomes were investigated only in children with diabetes to assess whether diabetes control might influence test scores, including the association between HbA1c level, diabetic ketoacidosis, and hypoglycemia and the pooled test score.

All outcomes and analyses were repeated for math and reading test scores separately.

Clinical Measures

All clinical measures were obtained from DanDiabKids, including diabetes type, HbA1c level, self-care (including self-monitoring of blood glucose levels), treatment (including pen or pump use; insulin dose), episodes of severe hypoglycemia or diabetic ketoacidosis, and other clinical measures (eg, blood pressure, body mass index).15 HbA1c levels were measured centrally using the standardized method described by the International Federation of Clinical Chemistry and determined using high-pressure liquid chromatography (Tosoh Bioscience), with a reference range of 4.3% to 5.8%. Severe hypoglycemia and diabetic ketoacidosis were defined according to the International Society for Pediatric and Adolescent Diabetes guidelines.22 Severe hypoglycemia was defined as episodes with loss of consciousness or seizure with blood glucose level less than 63 mg/dL (3.5 mmol/L) or regain of consciousness after treatment with glucagon or glucose. Episodes of severe hypoglycemia were the number of self-reported episodes treated at home and hospitalizations for hypoglycemia the last year before data upload. Episodes of diabetic ketoacidosis were the registered number of hospital admissions for diabetic ketoacidosis the last year before data upload.

The mean value of the 3 latest HbA1c measurements (recorded yearly) before the school test was used as the measure of HbA1c level (if fewer measurements were available, the mean of those was used). The treatment target for Danish children with diabetes was HbA1c level less than 7.5%. Poor control was defined as HbA1c level greater than 8.6%, which is the cutoff used by several pediatric clinics in Denmark as an indicator for the need for extra visits or psychosocial interventions.

Diabetic ketoacidosis and hypoglycemia were recorded if the child had any episode of either diabetic ketoacidosis (dichotomous) or severe hypoglycemia (dichotomous) in the same time window as the HbA1c measures. Dates of birth, diabetes onset, and school test were used to calculate age at onset and diabetes duration at school test.

Socioeconomic Status

Data on socioeconomic covariates were obtained from administrative registries with Statistics Denmark, including information on sex and birth order for all children. For parents, information included age, income, highest completed education, number of children in the household, marital status, residential zip code, whether the parent had insulin-dependent diabetes measured by insulin prescription claims, and whether the parent was an immigrant or descendent. Immigrants are Danish residents not born in Denmark, with neither of their parents born in Denmark. Descendants are Danish residents born in Denmark, with neither of their parents born in Denmark. All socioeconomic covariates were measured when the child was 5 years old.

Statistical Analysis

Means of selected variables were compared between children with and without diabetes.

Linear regression models were estimated to investigate the relationship between diabetes and test scores. Diabetes was coded with the value 1 for children with a confirmed diagnosis of diabetes and 0 for all other observations. Children who developed diabetes between tests were coded as 0 before onset and 1 after onset. The regression-based comparison between children with and without diabetes was performed considering test scores in math and reading as distinct observations, which were then pooled and analyzed jointly in the regression analyses. Cluster-robust standard errors were used to adjust for within-individual correlation attributable to multiple observations per child (a child could possibly be observed with several test scores over time and test topic). For test scores to be comparable across grades and topics, all regression models were adjusted for grade-, topic-, and year-specific effects. Additionally, models were adjusted for socioeconomic status.

To assess the robustness of these results, a case-sibling analysis was performed. Children with diabetes were compared with their siblings without diabetes using linear regression models and adding maternal fixed effects. Additionally, a grade- and topic-specific comparison between children with and without diabetes was performed by repeating the linear regression analysis separately by grade and topic.

Linear regression was also used to compare subgroups of children with diabetes to children without diabetes. To analyze aspects of associations with early disease onset and test scores, children diagnosed before school start (before age 6 years) were considered in 1 subgroup. Associations of diabetes duration and test scores were investigated in the subgroup of children with disease duration longer than 4 years (cutoff identified post hoc from mean value in data). To identify associations of diabetes control and test scores, subgroups of children with mean HbA1c levels greater than 7.5% and greater than 8.6% were considered. The last subgroup consisted of children with diabetic ketoacidosis at onset. Only test score observations for which the subgroup criteria were met at the point of the test were included in each subgroup. These models were estimated with and without adjusting for socioeconomic status. Differences between subgroups were tested by estimating the linear regressions jointly (Seemingly Unrelated Regression Equations) and then testing for equality of coefficients of interest across regressions.

For secondary outcomes among children with diabetes, we assessed the relationship between HbA1c levels and test scores by estimating kernel-weighted local means (“lpoly” command in Stata), with and without adjustment for socioeconomic status. Furthermore, we estimated linear regression models to assess the linear relationship between test scores and HbA1c levels, diabetic ketoacidosis, and hypoglycemia within the group of children with diabetes (with and without adjustment for socioeconomic status).

To assess the importance of missing data on test scores, all children with missing test scores were assigned the lowest possible score, and the main analysis re-estimated to determine if conclusions changed. Among children with diabetes, HbA1c level was compared by missing or nonmissing test score. Among children with diabetes, test scores were compared by missing or nonmissing values of HbA1c.

The primary analysis was replicated, discarding observations in which diabetes onset was within 6 months from the day of the test. All main analyses and the kernel-weighted local mean analysis of test scores and HbA1c level among children with diabetes were prespecified. The subgroup analyses were specified post hoc.

All statistical analyses were performed using Stata 15 (StataCorp). All statistical testing was 2-sided, and P < .05 was considered statistically significant. Because of the potential for type I error due to multiple comparisons, findings for analyses of secondary end points should be interpreted as exploratory.

Results

Of 744 516 children attending grades 2 through 8 overall, 2473 had diabetes. Of these children, 631 620 (85%) attended public schools, and of these, 2031 had diabetes. Children with diabetes were not more likely to attend public schools than children without diabetes (odds ratio [OR], 0.97 [95% CI, 0.88 to 1.07]). A total of 608 655 students took at least 1 test, and 1 561 770 test scores were identified (n = 1 037 006 in reading and n = 524 764 in math). Of the children with diabetes, 1878 had at least 1 test score (n = 4234 test scores) and 1729 had HbA1c levels measured before a test (n = 3684 test scores). Children with diabetes were more likely to miss a test than children without diabetes (6.3% vs 3.8%; difference, 2.5 percentage points [95% CI, 1.8 to 3.2]).

Mean age of the study population was 10.31 (SD, 2.42) years (at first test), and 51% were male. The comparison of background characteristics between children with and without diabetes (Table 1) showed that fathers of children with diabetes were less likely to have a master’s degree (8.5% vs 10.6%; difference, −2.1 percentage points [95% CI, −3.3 to −0.8]) or be immigrants or descendants (12.5% vs 14.9%; difference, −2.5 percentage points [95% CI, −4.1 to −0.9]) and were more likely to have insulin-dependent diabetes (6.5% vs 0.8%; difference, 5.8 percentage points [95% CI, 4.7 to 6.8]). Mothers of children with diabetes were less likely to be immigrants or descendants (11.4% vs 14.7%; difference, −3.2 percentage points [95% CI, −4.6 to −1.8]) and more likely to have insulin-dependent diabetes (2.5% vs 0.5%; difference, 2.1 percentage points [95% CI, 1.4 to 2.7]). Among children with diabetes, mean duration of diabetes was 4.5 (SD, 3.3) years (first percentile, 0.1 year; 99th percentile, 13 years), 18.3% had diabetic ketoacidosis at onset, and 64.0% used insulin pumps.

Primary Outcome

The overall mean pooled test score was 56.11 (SD, 24.93). There was no statistically significant difference between test scores from children with vs without diabetes (mean, 56.56 vs 56.11; difference, 0.45 [95% CI, −0.31 to 1.22]). The estimated difference from the linear model adjusted for grade-, topic-, and year-specific effects was 0.24 (95% CI, −0.90 to 1.39]). Adjusting for socioeconomic status, the difference was 0.45 (95% CI, −0.58 to 1.49) (Table 2 and Figure 1).

Sensitivity and Subgroup Analyses of the Primary Outcome

In the case-sibling analysis (Figure 1), 7344 test scores were identified, of which 2966 were from children with diabetes (n = 1257 children) and 4378 were from siblings without diabetes (n = 1606 children). Children with diabetes had slightly higher raw mean test scores than their siblings (56.43 vs 55.03; difference, 1.40 [95% CI, 0.20 to 2.60]), but this difference was insignificant in both linear models (adjusted for grade-, topic-, and year-specific effects, 0.32 [95% CI, −0.92 to 1.57]; additionally adjusted for socioeconomic status, −0.03 [95% CI, −1.30 to 1.23]).

In the analysis of differences in test scores between children with and without diabetes separately by topic and grade (Figure 1), only reading in fourth grade with adjustment for socioeconomic status showed a significant difference (1.89 [95% CI, 0.09 to 3.68]; higher scores for children with diabetes).

When comparing test scores in subgroups of children with diabetes with test scores of children without diabetes (Figure 2), having an HbA1c level greater than 7.5% (n = 1155 children, n = 2354 test scores) was associated with lower test scores (mean, 53.84 vs 56.11; difference, −2.26 [95% CI, −3.27 to −1.26]), but the association was not statistically significant in the linear model adjusted for socioeconomic status (difference, −1.06 [95% CI, −2.37 to 0.25]). Having an HbA1c level greater than 8.6% (n = 472 children, n = 867 test scores) was associated with lower scores (mean, 49.95 vs 56.11; difference, −6.15 [95% CI, −7.80 to −4.50]), and the difference was statistically significant in the linear model with adjustment for socioeconomic status (difference, −2.47 [95% CI, −4.50 to −0.45]). In the statistical tests for interaction between subgroups of children with diabetes (separately compared with children without diabetes), there was a statistically significant difference between subgroups with HbA1c levels greater than 7.5% vs 7.5% or lower and HbA1c levels greater than 8.6% vs 8.6% or lower (P < .001 for both subgroups, both with and without adjustment).

Diabetic ketoacidosis at onset (n = 336 children, n = 755 test scores), diabetes onset at age younger than 6 years (n = 564 children, n = 1391 test scores), and diabetes duration longer than 4 years (n = 799 children, n = 1659 test scores) were not associated with significantly different test scores compared with the children without diabetes (Figure 2). In the statistical tests for interaction between subgroups with diabetic ketoacidosis at onset vs no diabetic ketoacidosis at onset (separately compared with children without diabetes), there was a statistically significant difference without adjustment (P = .01) but not with adjustment (P = .13). The tests for interaction between children with diabetes onset at age younger than 6 years vs 6 years or older (P = .77 without adjustment; P = .81 with adjustment) and children with diabetes duration longer than 4 years vs 4 years or less (P = .89 without adjustment; P = .72 with adjustment) did not reach statistical significance.

Secondary Outcomes

Among children with diabetes, levels of HbA1c and indicators for severe hypoglycemia and hospitalizations for diabetic ketoacidosis could be assigned to 87% of the test score observations (n = 3684 from n = 1729 children with diabetes). Mean HbA1c level was 7.9% (SD, 1.1%). HbA1c level was significantly higher when the test score was missing (8.3% vs 7.9%; difference, 0.4% [95% CI, 0.3 to 0.6]).

The unadjusted kernel-weighted test scores for children with diabetes and HbA1c levels less than 8% were higher than the population mean (Figure 3A). Conversely, test scores for children with HbA1c levels in the range 8% to 11% were lower than the population mean. Adjusting for socioeconomic status attenuated the association between HbA1c level and test scores. Test scores from children with mean HbA1c levels less than 7.5% were slightly above the population mean. In contrast, test scores were lower than the population mean among children with HbA1c levels ranging from 8.5% to 9.5% (Figure 3B).

The unadjusted linear model (Figure 3A) showed that each 1% increase in HbA1c level was associated with a 3.48-point lower test score (95% CI, −4.32 to −2.64). The corresponding coefficient from the adjusted model (Figure 3B) was −1.59 (95% CI, −2.53 to −0.66).

Experiencing at least 1 episode of diabetic ketoacidosis within the past 3 years was associated with lower test scores (50.80 vs 56.91; difference, −6.11 [95% CI, −10.15 to −2.07]). This association was not statistically significant when controlling for HbA1c level, socioeconomic status, or both (eTable 1 in the Supplement). No association was found between having experienced at least 1 episode of severe hypoglycemia and test scores (eTable 1 in the Supplement).

All outcomes and analyses were repeated for math and reading test scores separately (eTables 1 and 2 and eFigures 1-5 in the Supplement). The results were similar to those for the pooled test score.

Missing Values

To address the potential of the increased propensity to miss a test among children with diabetes, all missing test scores were replaced with 1 (assuming that children who missed a test performed worst of all). Children with diabetes scored slightly lower (53.08 vs 54.02; difference, −0.94 [95% CI, −1.76 to −0.13]). The difference was not statistically significant in adjusted models (linear model, −1.05 [95% CI, −2.21 to 0.12]; with adjustment for socioeconomic status, −0.88 [95% CI, −1.94 to 0.17]).

To investigate the importance of missing HbA1c values among children with diabetes, pooled test score means were compared. Mean test score was 55.38 when HbA1c values were missing and 56.74 when values were available (difference, −1.35 [95% CI, −3.71 to 1.00]).

In models in which test scores from children with diabetes onset near the date of the test (within 6 months) were discarded, results were unchanged (n = 558 test scores; difference adjusted for grade-, topic-, and year-specific effects, 0.43 [95% CI, −0.75 to 1.61]; with additional adjustment for socioeconomic status, 0.62 [95% CI, −0.44 to 1.69]).

Discussion

In this population-based cohort study, there was no statistically significant difference in pooled reading and math test scores between children with and without diabetes. The estimated difference was small and statistically insignificant. Among children with diabetes, a statistically significant (linear) association between test scores and diabetes control (assessed by HbA1c level) was found.

A key question is whether the tests measure school performance. Previous research18 has demonstrated that test scores predict 48% to 51% of the variation in ninth-grade math and Danish examination grades.

The main finding of no statisticially significant difference in test scores contrasts with the findings of some previous studies.3-7 However, most existing studies are based on small, nonrandom samples of individuals with diabetes matched to random controls. Closest to this study is the study by Persson et al,6 which found small negative effects of diabetes on school grades for the population of Swedish children with diabetes born in the 1970s. Besides school grades (potentially) measuring more dimensions than test performance, most of the children in the current study were born after the turn of the millennium. It is possible that advances in treatment modalities over recent decades (64% of children with diabetes in this study used an insulin pump) have improved not only gaps in mortality and morbidity1,24 between individuals with diabetes and the overall population but also have improved gaps in school performance. This study also documented that it is important to adjust for confounders such as socioeconomic status when assessing the association between diabetes control and test scores or school performance.

While demonstrating a negative association between poor glycemic control and test scores, the results showed that children reaching the treatment target (HbA1c level <7.5%) scored higher than the population mean. While no conclusive evidence can be provided from an observational study, one interpretation could be that the parents of children with diabetes direct more of their resources toward the affected child, giving rise to both better school performance and better metabolic control. This might happen to a lesser extent among children with poor diabetes control. A second interpretation could be that being able to maintain good glycemic control and school performance is confounded by ability (cognitive and noncognitive skills), which is correlated with socioeconomic status.

Limitations

The study has several limitations. First, there was a higher proportion of missing test scores for children with diabetes. However, the sensitivity analysis showed that setting all missing test scores to 1 did not change the results. Second, Denmark has considerable diabetes awareness and good medical and social care by international standards, and hence the findings may not apply to other countries. It is possible that the gaps in test performance are larger in countries where access to health care is associated with socioeconomic status. Third, in this study tests were performed after short disease duration (mean, 4.5 years), and diabetic complications usually manifest after a longer duration. However, no association was found after limiting to children with longer disease duration. Further, even though diabetic ketoacidosis at onset (or later) may directly affect cognitive skills, no association with test scores was found.

Conclusions

Among Danish public schoolchildren, there was no significant difference in standardized reading and mathematics test scores of children with type 1 diabetes compared with test scores of children without diabetes.

Back to top
Article Information

Corresponding Author: Niels Skipper, PhD, Department of Economics and Business Economics, Aarhus University, Fuglesangs Allé 4, 8210 Aarhus V, Denmark (nskipper@econ.au.dk).

Accepted for Publication: December 19, 2018.

Author Contributions: Dr Skipper had full access to all of the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.

Concept and design: Skipper, Gaulke, Eriksen, Svensson.

Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: All authors.

Drafting of the manuscript: Skipper, Gaulke, Eriksen, Fabrin Nielsen.

Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: All authors.

Statistical analysis: Skipper, Fabrin Nielsen, Svensson.

Obtained funding: Skipper, Svensson.

Administrative, technical, or material support: Skipper, Svensson.

Supervision: Skipper, Svensson.

Conflict of Interest Disclosures: Dr Møller Sildorf reported receiving speaking fees from Novo Nordisk. Dr Svensson reported serving as an adviser to Medtronic, Janssen, and Novo Nordisk; owning shares in Novo Nordisk; and receiving speaking fees from Medtronic, Sanofi, Novo Nordisk, and Bayer AG. No other disclosures were reported.

Funding/Support: This study has been funded by Independent Research Fund Denmark grant 8019-00055B.

Role of the Funder/Sponsor: The funding organization had no role in the design and the conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

References
1.
Nathan  DM, Genuth  S, Lachin  J,  et al; Diabetes Control and Complications Trial Research Group.  The effect of intensive treatment of diabetes on the development and progression of long-term complications in insulin-dependent diabetes mellitus.  N Engl J Med. 1993;329(14):977-986. doi:10.1056/NEJM199309303291401PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
McCrimmon  RJ, Ryan  CM, Frier  BM.  Diabetes and cognitive dysfunction.  Lancet. 2012;379(9833):2291-2299. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(12)60360-2PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
Ferguson  SC, Blane  A, Wardlaw  J,  et al.  Influence of an early-onset age of type 1 diabetes on cerebral structure and cognitive function.  Diabetes Care. 2005;28(6):1431-1437. doi:10.2337/diacare.28.6.1431PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Hannonen  R, Komulainen  J, Riikonen  R,  et al; Study Group.  Academic skills in children with early-onset type 1 diabetes: the effects of diabetes-related risk factors.  Dev Med Child Neurol. 2012;54(5):457-463. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8749.2012.04248.xPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Semenkovich  K, Patel  PP, Pollock  AB,  et al.  Academic abilities and glycaemic control in children and young people with type 1 diabetes mellitus.  Diabet Med. 2016;33(5):668-673. doi:10.1111/dme.12854PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Persson  S, Dahlquist  G, Gerdtham  UG, Steen Carlsson  K.  Impact of childhood-onset type 1 diabetes on schooling: a population-based register study.  Diabetologia. 2013;56(6):1254-1262. doi:10.1007/s00125-013-2870-8PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Hannonen  R, Komulainen  J, Eklund  K, Tolvanen  A, Riikonen  R, Ahonen  T.  Verbal and academic skills in children with early-onset type 1 diabetes.  Dev Med Child Neurol. 2010;52(7):e143-e147. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8749.2010.03648.xPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
McCarthy  AM, Lindgren  S, Mengeling  MA, Tsalikian  E, Engvall  JC.  Effects of diabetes on learning in children.  Pediatrics. 2002;109(1):E9. doi:10.1542/peds.109.1.e9PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Patiño-Fernández  AM, Delamater  AM, Applegate  EB,  et al.  Neurocognitive functioning in preschool-age children with type 1 diabetes mellitus.  Pediatr Diabetes. 2010;11(6):424-430. doi:10.1111/j.1399-5448.2009.00618.xPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Ly  TT, Anderson  M, McNamara  KA, Davis  EA, Jones  TW.  Neurocognitive outcomes in young adults with early-onset type 1 diabetes: a prospective follow-up study.  Diabetes Care. 2011;34(10):2192-2197. doi:10.2337/dc11-0697PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
Cooper  MN, McNamara  KA, de Klerk  NH, Davis  EA, Jones  TW.  School performance in children with type 1 diabetes: a contemporary population-based study.  Pediatr Diabetes. 2016;17(2):101-111. doi:10.1111/pedi.12243PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Gejl  M, Gjedde  A, Brock  B,  et al.  Effects of hypoglycaemia on working memory and regional cerebral blood flow in type 1 diabetes: a randomised, crossover trial.  Diabetologia. 2018;61(3):551-561. doi:10.1007/s00125-017-4502-1PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Allen  KV, Pickering  MJ, Zammitt  NN,  et al.  Effects of acute hypoglycemia on working memory and language processing in adults with and without type 1 diabetes.  Diabetes Care. 2015;38(6):1108-1115. doi:10.2337/dc14-1657PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
McCarthy  AM, Lindgren  S, Mengeling  MA, Tsalikian  E, Engvall  J.  Factors associated with academic achievement in children with type 1 diabetes.  Diabetes Care. 2003;26(1):112-117. doi:10.2337/diacare.26.1.112PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Svensson  J, Cerqueira  C, Kjærsgaard  P,  et al.  Danish Registry of Childhood and Adolescent Diabetes.  Clin Epidemiol. 2016;8:679-683. doi:10.2147/CLEP.S99469PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Wandall  J.  National tests in Denmark—CAT as a pedagogic tool.  J Appl Testing Technol. 2011;12(special issue):1-21.Google Scholar
17.
Rasch  G.  Probabilistic Models for Some Intelligence and Attainment Tests. Copenhagen, Denmark: Dansk Paedagogisk Institut; 1960.
18.
Beuchert  L, Nandrup  A. The Danish National Tests at a glance. Danish Economic Journal website. https://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=2996431. July 6, 2017. Accessed December 26, 2018.
19.
Andersen  SC, Humlum  MK, Nandrup  AB.  Increasing instruction time in school does increase learning.  Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2016;113(27):7481-7484. doi:10.1073/pnas.1516686113PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Elkjær  LS, Bech  BH, Sun  Y, Laursen  TM, Christensen  J.  Association between prenatal valproate exposure and performance on standardized language and mathematics tests in school-aged children.  JAMA Neurol. 2018;75(6):663-671. doi:10.1001/jamaneurol.2017.5035PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
Sievertsen  HH, Gino  F, Piovesan  M.  Cognitive fatigue influences students’ performance on standardized tests.  Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2016;113(10):2621-2624. doi:10.1073/pnas.1516947113PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
International Society for Pediatric and Adolescent Diabetes (ISPAD). ISPAD clinical practice guidelines 2018. ISPAD website. https://www.ispad.org/page/ISPADGuidelines2018. 2018. Accessed December 14, 2018.
23.
Lovell  MC.  Seasonal adjustment of economic time series and multiple regression analysis.  J Am Stat Assoc. 1963;58:993-1010. doi:10.1080/01621459.1963.10480682Google ScholarCrossref
24.
Steineck  I, Cederholm  J, Eliasson  B,  et al; Swedish National Diabetes Register.  Insulin pump therapy, multiple daily injections, and cardiovascular mortality in 18,168 people with type 1 diabetes: observational study.  BMJ. 2015;350:h3234. doi:10.1136/bmj.h3234PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
×