[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Table. Baseline Characteristics of Trial Participants and Mean (SD) Change in Outcome Measures at Trial Completion From Baseline in 86 Women
Table. Baseline Characteristics of Trial Participants and Mean (SD) Change in Outcome Measures at Trial Completion From Baseline in 86 Women
1.
Ferri CP, Prince M, Brayne C,  et al; Alzheimer's Disease International.  Global prevalence of dementia: a Delphi consensus study.  Lancet. 2005;366(9503):2112-211716360788PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
Petersen RC, Smith GE, Waring SC, Ivnik RJ, Tangalos EG, Kokmen E. Mild cognitive impairment: clinical characterization and outcome.  Arch Neurol. 1999;56(3):303-30810190820PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
Liu-Ambrose T, Nagamatsu LS, Graf P, Beattie BL, Ashe MC, Handy TC. Resistance training and executive functions: a 12-month randomized controlled trial.  Arch Intern Med. 2010;170(2):170-17820101012PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Liu-Ambrose T, Nagamatsu LS, Voss MW, Khan KM, Handy TC. Resistance training and functional plasticity of the aging brain: a 12-month randomized controlled trial [published online July 6, 2011].  Neurobiol Aging21741129PubMedGoogle Scholar
5.
Colcombe SJ, Kramer AF, Erickson KI,  et al.  Cardiovascular fitness, cortical plasticity, and aging.  Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2004;101(9):3316-332114978288PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Baker LD, Frank LL, Foster-Schubert K,  et al.  Effects of aerobic exercise on mild cognitive impairment: a controlled trial.  Arch Neurol. 2010;67(1):71-7920065132PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Royall DR, Palmer R, Chiodo LK, Polk MJ. Declining executive control in normal aging predicts change in functional status: the Freedom House Study.  J Am Geriatr Soc. 2004;52(3):346-35214962147PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bédirian V,  et al.  The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment.  J Am Geriatr Soc. 2005;53(4):695-69915817019PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Spreen O, Strauss E. A Compendium of Neurological Tests2nd ed. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 1998
10.
Herholz K, Ehlen P, Kessler J, Strotmann T, Kalbe E, Markowitsch H-J. Learning face-name associations and the effect of age and performance: a PET activation study.  Neuropsychologia. 2001;39(6):643-65011257289PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
Research Letter
April 23, 2012

Resistance Training Promotes Cognitive and Functional Brain Plasticity in Seniors With Probable Mild Cognitive Impairment

Author Affiliations

Author Affiliations: Departments of Psychology (Ms Nagamatsu and Dr Handy) and Physical Therapy (Mr Hsu and Dr Liu-Ambrose), University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada; The Brain Research Centre (Ms Nagamatsu, Drs Handy and Liu-Ambrose, and Mr Hsu) and The Centre for Hip Health and Mobility (Ms Nagamatsu, Mr Hsu, and Dr Liu-Ambrose), Vancouver Coastal Health Research Institute, Vancouver; and University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, The Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology, Urbana (Dr Voss).

Arch Intern Med. 2012;172(8):666-668. doi:10.1001/archinternmed.2012.379

Cognitive decline is a pressing health care issue. Worldwide, 1 new case of dementia is detected every 7 seconds.1 Mild cognitive impairment—a well-recognized risk factor for dementia2—represents a critical window of opportunity for intervening and altering the trajectory of cognitive decline in seniors.

Exercise is a promising strategy for combating cognitive decline. Both aerobic training (AT) and resistance training (RT) enhance cognitive performance and functional plasticity in healthy, community-dwelling seniors3-5 and those with mild cognitive impairment.6 However, to our knowledge, no intervention study has compared the efficacy of both types of exercise on cognitive function and functional brain plasticity in seniors with mild cognitive impairment. Understanding this is crucial to using exercise as a strategy for altering the trajectory of cognitive decline in seniors with mild cognitive impairment.

We conducted a proof-of-concept, single-blinded, randomized controlled trial primarily designed to provide preliminary evidence of efficacy of both RT and AT to improve executive cognitive functions—robust predictors of conversion from mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer disease7—in senior women with probable mild cognitive impairment. Secondarily, we aimed to examine the effect of both types of exercise on associative memory performance, everyday problem solving ability, regional patterns of functional brain plasticity, and physical function.

Methods

The EXCEL (EXercise for Cognition and Everyday Living) study was a 6-month randomized trial. Eighty-six community-dwelling women 70 to 80 years old were randomly allocated to twice-weekly RT (28 women), twice-weekly AT (30 women), or twice-weekly balance and tone (BAT) training (control group) (28 women). Participants were classified as having probable mild cognitive impairment if they had a score lower than 26 out of 30 on the Montreal Cognitive Assessment8 and had subjective memory complaints.

The primary outcome measure was Stroop Test9 performance, an executive cognitive test of selective attention/conflict resolution. Secondary measures of executive cognitive functions included set shifting (Trail Making Tests) and working memory (Verbal Digits Tests). Broader effects of exercise training on cognitive function were examined by assessing associative memory (memorizing face-scene pairs) and everyday problem solving ability (Everyday Problems Test). Regional patterns of functional brain plasticity were assessed using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) during the associative memory task. Finally, we assessed general balance and mobility (Short Physical Performance Battery) and general cardiovascular capacity (Six-Minute Walk Test).

The 60-minute classes were led by certified fitness instructors. For RT, both a Keiser Pressurized Air system and free weights were used.3 Participants performed 2 sets of 6 to 8 repetitions, and loading was increased when sets were completed with proper form. The AT program was an outdoor walking program. The training stimulus started at 40% of a participant's age-specific target heart rate (ie, heart rate reserve [HRR]) and progressed to 70% to 80% of the HRR. The BAT program consisted of stretching, range of motion, balance exercises, and relaxation techniques.3 This group served to control for confounding variables. Participants were questioned about the presence of any adverse effects and were monitored by instructors.

Results

Of the 86 participants, 77 completed the 26-week trial (26 in the RT group, 24 in the AT group, 27 in the BAT group). Twenty-two participants were included in our fMRI analysis (7 in the RT group, 7 in the AT group, and 8 in the BAT group).

The Table shows the baseline characteristics of our sample and change in scores from baseline to trial completion for the primary and secondary outcome measures, excluding fMRI. Compared with the BAT group, the RT group significantly improved performance on the Stroop Test (P = .04) and the associative memory task (P = .03). Compared with the BAT group, RT also led to functional changes in 3 regions of the cortex—the right lingual (P = .03) and occipital-fusiform (P = .02) gyri and the right frontal pole (P = .03)—during the encoding and recall of associations. In addition, there was a significant positive correlation between change in hemodynamic activity in the right lingual gyrus and change in behavioral associative memory performance (r = 0.51; P = .02). The AT group significantly improved general balance and mobility (P = .03) and cardiovascular capacity (P = .04) compared with the BAT group.

Adverse effects included acute episodes of shortness of breath (2 participants) and noninjurious falls (4 participants). There were no significant between-group differences (P = .54) in adverse events.

Comment

In senior women with subjective memory complaints, 6 months of twice-weekly RT improved selective attention/conflict resolution, associative memory, and regional patterns of functional brain plasticity compared with twice-weekly BAT exercises. In contrast, 6 months of twice-weekly AT improved physical function. We provide novel evidence that RT can benefit multiple domains in those at risk for dementia.

While we previously demonstrated that 12 months of twice-weekly RT significantly improved Stroop Test performance in cognitively healthy women 65 to 75 years old,3 our current study found an improvement after only 6 months in women 70 to 80 years old with probable mild cognitive impairment. Thus, the benefits of RT on selective attention/conflict resolution may be more potent among those at greater risk for dementia.

Baker et al6 previously demonstrated that 6 months of AT improved selective attention/conflict resolution and set shifting performance in older women with amnestic mild cognitive impairment. This may be attributed to differences in both the frequency and intensity of AT between the 2 studies. In addition, our study participants were older and had lower baseline Mini-Mental State Examination scores.

We also demonstrated that 6 months of RT twice-weekly significantly improved associative memory performance, co-occurring with positive functional changes in hemodynamic activity in regions involved in the memorization of associations.10 Impaired associative memory is a hallmark of early stages of Alzheimer disease.

Exercise compliance was low, suggesting that we are providing conservative estimates of the efficacy of RT on cognition and functional plasticity. While the AT group had the highest dropout rate, they demonstrated a significant increase in general cardiovascular capacity. Our findings may not generalize to men or to women of other ages.

In conclusion, our study suggests that twice-weekly RT is a promising strategy to alter the trajectory of cognitive decline in seniors with mild cognitive impairment.

Back to top
Article Information

Correspondence: Dr Liu-Ambrose, Department of Physical Therapy, University of British Columbia, 212-2177 Wesbrook Mall, Vancouver, BC V6T 1Z3, Canada (tlambrose@exchange.ubc.ca).

Author Contributions: All authors had full access to all the data in the study and take responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis. Study concept and design: Voss and Liu-Ambrose. Acquisition of data: Nagamatsu, Hsu, and Liu-Ambrose. Analysis and interpretation of data: Nagamatsu, Handy, Voss, and Liu-Ambrose. Drafting of the manuscript: Nagamatsu, Handy, and Liu-Ambrose. Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Nagamatsu, Handy, Hsu, Voss, and Liu-Ambrose. Statistical analysis: Nagamatsu, Voss, and Liu-Ambrose. Obtained funding: Liu-Ambrose. Administrative, technical, and material support: Nagamatsu, Hsu, Voss, and Liu-Ambrose. Study supervision: Handy and Liu-Ambrose.

Financial Disclosure: None reported.

Funding/Support: The Pacific Alzheimer's Research Foundation provided funding for this study (Dr Liu-Ambrose).

Previous Presentation: Data from this manuscript were presented as a podium presentation at the International Society for Neuroimaging in Psychiatry; September 9, 2011; Heidelberg, Germany.

Additional Information: Ms Nagamatsu is a Michael Smith Foundation for Health Research Senior Graduate trainee and a Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada Doctoral trainee. Dr Liu-Ambrose is a Michael Smith Foundation for Health Research Scholar, a Canadian Institutes of Health Research New Investigator, and a Heart and Stroke Foundation of Canada's Henry J. M. Barnett's Scholarship recipient.

Additional Contributions: Alison Chan, BSc, Jennifer C. Davis, PhD, B. Lynn Beattie, MD, and Peter Graf, PhD, made significant contributions to this study. We thank the instructors for their commitment to the participants' health and safety.

This article was corrected for errors on July 10, 2013.

References
1.
Ferri CP, Prince M, Brayne C,  et al; Alzheimer's Disease International.  Global prevalence of dementia: a Delphi consensus study.  Lancet. 2005;366(9503):2112-211716360788PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
Petersen RC, Smith GE, Waring SC, Ivnik RJ, Tangalos EG, Kokmen E. Mild cognitive impairment: clinical characterization and outcome.  Arch Neurol. 1999;56(3):303-30810190820PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
Liu-Ambrose T, Nagamatsu LS, Graf P, Beattie BL, Ashe MC, Handy TC. Resistance training and executive functions: a 12-month randomized controlled trial.  Arch Intern Med. 2010;170(2):170-17820101012PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Liu-Ambrose T, Nagamatsu LS, Voss MW, Khan KM, Handy TC. Resistance training and functional plasticity of the aging brain: a 12-month randomized controlled trial [published online July 6, 2011].  Neurobiol Aging21741129PubMedGoogle Scholar
5.
Colcombe SJ, Kramer AF, Erickson KI,  et al.  Cardiovascular fitness, cortical plasticity, and aging.  Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2004;101(9):3316-332114978288PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Baker LD, Frank LL, Foster-Schubert K,  et al.  Effects of aerobic exercise on mild cognitive impairment: a controlled trial.  Arch Neurol. 2010;67(1):71-7920065132PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Royall DR, Palmer R, Chiodo LK, Polk MJ. Declining executive control in normal aging predicts change in functional status: the Freedom House Study.  J Am Geriatr Soc. 2004;52(3):346-35214962147PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bédirian V,  et al.  The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment.  J Am Geriatr Soc. 2005;53(4):695-69915817019PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Spreen O, Strauss E. A Compendium of Neurological Tests2nd ed. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 1998
10.
Herholz K, Ehlen P, Kessler J, Strotmann T, Kalbe E, Markowitsch H-J. Learning face-name associations and the effect of age and performance: a PET activation study.  Neuropsychologia. 2001;39(6):643-65011257289PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
×