[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Table 1. Choices for Hungry vs Sated Participants
Choices for Hungry vs Sated Participants
Table 2. Purchase of Less Healthy Items in Late Evening Relative to Afternoon
Purchase of Less Healthy Items in Late Evening Relative to Afternoon
1.
Wansink  B, Tal  A, Shimizu  M.  First foods most: after 18-hour fast, people drawn to starches first and vegetables last.  Arch Intern Med. 2012;172(12):961-963. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
Frost  G, Pirani  S.  Meal frequency and nutritional intake during Ramadan: a pilot study.  Hum Nutr Appl Nutr. 1987;41(1):47-50. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
Karaağaoğlu  N, Yücecan  S.  Some behavioural changes observed among fasting subjects, their nutritional habits and energy expenditure in Ramadan.  Int J Food Sci Nutr. 2000;51(2):125-134. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Scott  RL, Sohmer  PR, MacDonald  MG.  The effect of starvation and repletion on plasma fibronectin in man.  JAMA. 1982;248(16):2025-2027. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Susser  ES, Lin  SP.  Schizophrenia after prenatal exposure to the Dutch Hunger Winter of 1944-1945.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1992;49(12):983-988. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Mela  DJ, Aaron  JI, Gatenby  SJ.  Relationships of consumer characteristics and food deprivation to food purchasing behavior.  Physiol Behav. 1996;60(5):1331-1335. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Nisbett  RE, Kanouse  DE.  Obesity, food deprivation, and supermarket shopping behavior.  J Pers Soc Psychol. 1969;12(4):289-294. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Zverev  YP.  Effect of caloric deprivation and satiety on sensitivity of the gustatory system.  BMC Neuro. 2004;5(5):1471-2202. PubMedGoogle Scholar
9.
Goldstone  AP, Prechtl de Hernandez  CG, Beaver  JD,  et al.  Fasting biases brain reward systems towards high-calorie foods.  Eur J Neurosci. 2009;30(8):1625-1635. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
Research Letter
June 24, 2013

Fattening Fasting: Hungry Grocery Shoppers Buy More Calories, Not More Food

Author Affiliations

Author Affiliations: Dyson School of Applied Economics and Management, Cornell University, Ithaca, New York.

JAMA Intern Med. 2013;173(12):1146-1148. doi:10.1001/jamainternmed.2013.650

Fasting, intended or unintended, is a common phenomenon.1 It can be self-imposed in the case of extreme dieting, religious fasts, and chaotic work schedules.2,3 It can be medically imposed in the case of fasting before blood draws or surgery.4 In extreme cases, it can be caused by external circumstances, such as food shortages, natural disasters, or poverty.5

Food deprivation has been shown to alter the quantity of food people buy or consume.6,7 However, little attention has been given to how fasting affects the composition of people's food choices. Do people just buy more when food deprived, or do they specifically increase purchases of high-calorie, relative to low-calorie, foods?

There is some reason to suspect that shifts in purchase following food deprivation would indeed focus on increases in high-calorie foods. Fasting has been shown to increase brain reactivity to particular types of food over others.8 Fasting participants showed increased activation in brain areas associated with reward, including the ventral striatum, amygdala, anterior insula, and medial and lateral orbiofrontal cortex, in response to high-calorie, vs low-calorie, foods.9

If reactivity to high-calorie foods is increased following fasting, it may well be that people would also choose more of those foods relative to low-calorie foods. Indeed, recent research has demonstrated a shift toward more high-calorie options following 18-hour fasts.1Such fasts might be a rarity in daily life, but short periods of deprivation—such as skipping a meal—are fairly common. If shifts toward a higher-calorie shopping basket occur even with short-term deprivation, this would increase the importance of such findings for a wide number of groups ranging from meal-skipping dieters to overworked medical residents. We sought to determine whether short-term food deprivation leads to an increase in high-calorie relative to low-calorie choices.

Methods

A laboratory study and a field study were conducted. In the laboratory study, 68 paid participants (age range, 18-62 years; 71% were female) were asked to avoid eating for 5 hours prior to the study. The institutional review board–approved study was conducted in the afternoon hours (12-5 PM) of 2 weekdays, involving groups of 6 to 12 participants. In a random half of the sessions, participants were given a plate of Wheat Thins (Nabisco) and instructed to eat enough to no longer feel hungry (satiety condition). In the other half of the sessions, participants were not given such foods (hunger condition). Participants were then asked to shop in a simulated online grocery store that offered a mix of lower-calorie foods (fruits, vegetables, chicken breasts) and higher-calorie (candy, salty snacks, red meat) foods with each high-calorie item paralleled by a lower-calorie alternative. Products were displayed without prices.

A follow-up field study was conducted in a grocery store where we tracked people's purchases at different times of the day (N = 82 participants) when an earlier study had indicated they were most likely to be full (1:00-4:00 PM) or hungry (4:00-7:00 PM). Purchases were categorized as low-calorie and high-calorie. We then calculated number of low-calorie options relative to high-calorie options across these hungry and full time slots.

Results

As Table 1 indicates, hungry laboratory participants chose a higher number of higher-calorie products [t66 = 2.02; P = .05], but there were no differences between conditions in the number of lower-calorie choices (P > .50) and the total number of food items they selected was similar (P = .10). This same trend was found across individual product categories (Table 2).

Field study shoppers completing the study at higher hunger hours (4:00-7:00 PM) bought less low-calorie food relative to high-calorie food options compared with those completing the study at lower-hunger, after-lunch hours (1:00-4:00 PM). The ratio of low-calorie to high-calorie foods purchased was lower after 4:00 PM (2.48) than between 1:00 PM and 4:00 PM (3.96). Differences were significant at the P = .02 level, controlling for BMI: F1,43 = 5.52. This difference emanated mostly from a decrease in healthy items for afternoon hours: from 11.2 in the early afternoon to 8.21 in the evening: F1,55 = 4.26; P = .04. In other words, people who shopped at hours when they were more likely to be hungry tended to buy less low-calorie foods proportionate to overall purchases.

Discussion

Even short-term food deprivation can lead to a shift in choices such that people choose less low-calorie, and relatively more high-calorie, food options. Given the prevalence of short-term food deprivation, this has important health implications. It suggests that people should be more careful about their choices when food-deprived and possibly avoid choice situations when hungry by making choices while in less hungry states (eg, by eating an appetizer before shopping).

In conclusion, even short-term fasts can lead people to make more unhealthy food choices, picking a lower quantity of high-calorie, relative to low-calorie, foods.

Back to top
Article Information

Correspondence: Dr Tal, Dyson School of Applied Economics and Management, 15 Warren Hall, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY 14853-7801 (425@cornell.edu).

Retraction: A notice of Retraction was published on September 19, 2018; and a notice of Expression of Concern was published on April 13, 2018.

Published Online: May 6, 2013. doi:10.1001/jamainternmed.2013.650

Author Contributions:Study concept and design: Both authors. Acquisition of data: Both Authors. Analysis and interpretation of data: Tal. Drafting of the manuscript: Tal. Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Both authors. Statistical analysis: Tal. Obtained funding: Wansink. Study supervision: Wansink.

Conflict of Interest Disclosures: None reported.

Funding/Support: This research was made possible by support from Cornell University.

References
1.
Wansink  B, Tal  A, Shimizu  M.  First foods most: after 18-hour fast, people drawn to starches first and vegetables last.  Arch Intern Med. 2012;172(12):961-963. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
Frost  G, Pirani  S.  Meal frequency and nutritional intake during Ramadan: a pilot study.  Hum Nutr Appl Nutr. 1987;41(1):47-50. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
Karaağaoğlu  N, Yücecan  S.  Some behavioural changes observed among fasting subjects, their nutritional habits and energy expenditure in Ramadan.  Int J Food Sci Nutr. 2000;51(2):125-134. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Scott  RL, Sohmer  PR, MacDonald  MG.  The effect of starvation and repletion on plasma fibronectin in man.  JAMA. 1982;248(16):2025-2027. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Susser  ES, Lin  SP.  Schizophrenia after prenatal exposure to the Dutch Hunger Winter of 1944-1945.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1992;49(12):983-988. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Mela  DJ, Aaron  JI, Gatenby  SJ.  Relationships of consumer characteristics and food deprivation to food purchasing behavior.  Physiol Behav. 1996;60(5):1331-1335. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Nisbett  RE, Kanouse  DE.  Obesity, food deprivation, and supermarket shopping behavior.  J Pers Soc Psychol. 1969;12(4):289-294. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Zverev  YP.  Effect of caloric deprivation and satiety on sensitivity of the gustatory system.  BMC Neuro. 2004;5(5):1471-2202. PubMedGoogle Scholar
9.
Goldstone  AP, Prechtl de Hernandez  CG, Beaver  JD,  et al.  Fasting biases brain reward systems towards high-calorie foods.  Eur J Neurosci. 2009;30(8):1625-1635. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
×