[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Figure.  Cumulative Incidence of All-Cause Mortality and Attendance at Religious Services in the Nurses’ Health Study, 1996-2012
Cumulative Incidence of All-Cause Mortality and Attendance at Religious Services in the Nurses’ Health Study, 1996-2012

For the predictors the multivariable model adjusted for, see the Covariates subsection of the Methods section. Hazard ratio, 0.67 (95% CI, 0.62-0.71; P < .001 for trend for Cox model).

Table 1.  Age-Adjusted Baseline Characteristics and Subsequent Mediators of Study Participants by Frequency of Attendance at Religious Services in 1996
Age-Adjusted Baseline Characteristics and Subsequent Mediators of Study Participants by Frequency of Attendance at Religious Services in 1996
Table 2.  Joint Effects of Attendance at Religious Services in 1996 and 2000 With All-Cause Mortality in the Nurses’ Health Study, 2000-2012
Joint Effects of Attendance at Religious Services in 1996 and 2000 With All-Cause Mortality in the Nurses’ Health Study, 2000-2012
Table 3.  Multivariable Adjusted Hazard Ratios Between Attendance at Religious Services and Cardiovascular Disease and Cancer Mortality in the Nurses’ Health Study, 1996-2012a
Multivariable Adjusted Hazard Ratios Between Attendance at Religious Services and Cardiovascular Disease and Cancer Mortality in the Nurses’ Health Study, 1996-2012a
Table 4.  Mediation Analysis Between Attendance at Religious Services in 1996 and All-Cause Mortality in 2012
Mediation Analysis Between Attendance at Religious Services in 1996 and All-Cause Mortality in 2012
Supplement.

eTable 1. Frequency of Attendance at Religious Service Across Follow-up Years in the Overall Nurse’s Health Study

eTable 2. Subgroup Analyses of the Association Between Religious Services Attendance With All-Cause Mortality in the Nurses’ Health Study, 1996-2012

eTable 3. Subgroup Analyses of the Joint Effects of Religious Services Attendance in 1996 and 2000 With All-Cause Mortality in the Nurses’ Health Study, 2000-2012

eTable 4. Effect Size Estimates of Attendance at Religious Service and Other Covariates

eTable 5. Time Trends for the Associations Between Attendance at Religious Services and Mortality

eTable 6. Association Between Attendance at Religious Service and Specific Cardiovascular Disease (>100 Death) Mortality in the Nurses’ Health Study

eTable 7. Association Between Attendance at Religious Service and Specific Cancer Mortality in the Nurses’ Health Study

eTable 8. Multivariate Adjusted Hazard Ratio and its Related 95% CI for Breast Cancer Incidence (1996-2012) by Frequency of Attendance at Religious Service in 1996

eTable 9. Multivariate Adjusted Hazard Ratio and its Related 95% CI for Cardiovascular Incidence (1996-2012) by Frequency of Attendance at Religious Service in 1996

eTable 10. Joint Association of Attendance at Religious Service in 1996 and Race/Ethnicity on All-Cause Mortality

eTable 11. Joint Association of Attendance at Religious Service in 1996 and Religious Affiliation on All-Cause Mortality

eTable 12. Effect Size Estimate of Attendance at Religious Service and Social Integration Score With Mutual Contemporaneous Adjustment for Social Integration

eAppendix 1. Marginal Structural Model and Inverse Probability Weighting

eAppendix 2. Description of Calculation of Years of Life Saved

1.
World Health Organization. WHO definition of health. http://www.who.int/about/definition/en/print.html. Accessed April 7, 2016.
2.
Balboni  MJ, Puchalski  CM, Peteet  JR.  The relationship between medicine, spirituality and religion: three models for integration.  J Relig Health. 2014;53(5):1586-1598.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
Koenig  HG.  Faith & Mental Health. Philadelphia, PA: Templeton Foundation Press; 2005.
4.
Koenig  H, King  DE, Benner  V.  Handbook of Religion and Health. 2nd ed. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2012.
5.
Puchalski  CM, Blatt  B, Kogan  M, Butler  A.  Spirituality and health: the development of a field.  Acad Med. 2014;89(1):10-16.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Puchalski  CM, Vitillo  R, Hull  SK, Reller  N.  Improving the spiritual dimension of whole person care: reaching national and international consensus.  J Palliat Med. 2014;17(6):642-656.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Crabtree  S, Pelham  B. What Alabamians and Iranians have in common; a global perspective on Americans’ religiosity offers a few surprises. Gallup Poll News Service. https://www.highbeam.com/doc/1G1-193998914.html. Published February 9, 2009. Accessed April 8, 2016. Gallup Poll News Service; 2009.
8.
Franck  R, Iannaccone  L.  Religious decline in the 20th century West: testing alternative explanations.  Public Choice. 2014;159(3-4):385-414. doi:10.1007/s11127-013-0103-9.Google ScholarCrossref
9.
Lucchetti  G, Lucchetti  AL, Koenig  HG.  Impact of spirituality/religiosity on mortality: comparison with other health interventions.  Explore (NY). 2011;7(4):234-238.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
McCullough  ME, Hoyt  WT, Larson  DB, Koenig  HG, Thoresen  C.  Religious involvement and mortality: a meta-analytic review.  Health Psychol. 2000;19(3):211-222.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
Hall  DE.  Religious attendance: more cost-effective than Lipitor?  J Am Board Fam Med. 2006;19(2):103-109.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Larson  DB, Larson  SS, Koenig  HG.  Mortality and religion/spirituality: a brief review of the research.  Ann Pharmacother. 2002;36(6):1090-1098.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Sloan  RP, Bagiella  E, VandeCreek  L,  et al.  Should physicians prescribe religious activities?  N Engl J Med. 2000;342(25):1913-1916.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
Sloan  RP, Bagiella  E, Powell  T.  Religion, spirituality, and medicine.  Lancet. 1999;353(9153):664-667.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Denberg  T.  Religious attendance: more cost-effective than Lipitor?  J Am Board Fam Med. 2006;19(4):430.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Koenig  HG, Idler  E, Kasl  S,  et al.  Religion, spirituality, and medicine: a rebuttal to skeptics.  Int J Psychiatry Med. 1999;29(2):123-131.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
VanderWeele  TJ.  Unmeasured confounding and hazard scales: sensitivity analysis for total, direct, and indirect effects.  Eur J Epidemiol. 2013;28(2):113-117.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
Dupre  ME, Franzese  AT, Parrado  EA.  Religious attendance and mortality: implications for the black–white mortality crossover.  Demography. 2006;43(1):141-164.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
19.
Strawbridge  WJ, Cohen  RD, Shema  SJ, Kaplan  GA.  Frequent attendance at religious services and mortality over 28 years.  Am J Public Health. 1997;87(6):957-961.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Ellison  CG, Hummer  RA, Cormier  S, Rogers  RG.  Religious involvement and mortality risk among African American adults.  Res Aging. 2000;22(6):630-677. doi:10.1177/0164027500226003. Google ScholarCrossref
21.
Colditz  GA, Hankinson  SE.  The Nurses’ Health Study: lifestyle and health among women.  Nat Rev Cancer. 2005;5(5):388-396.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
Rich-Edwards  JW, Corsano  KA, Stampfer  MJ.  Test of the national death index and Equifax nationwide death search.  Am J Epidemiol. 1994;140(11):1016-1019.PubMedGoogle Scholar
23.
Chiuve  SE, Sampson  L, Willett  WC.  The association between a nutritional quality index and risk of chronic disease.  Am J Prev Med. 2011;40(5):505-513.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Sun  Q, Townsend  MK, Okereke  OI,  et al.  Alcohol consumption at midlife and successful ageing in women: a prospective cohort analysis in the Nurses’ Health Study.  PLoS Med. 2011;8(9):e1001090.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
Ware  JE  Jr, Sherbourne  CD.  The MOS 36-item short-form health survey (SF-36): I: conceptual framework and item selection.  Med Care. 1992;30(6):473-483.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
Berkman  LF, Syme  SL.  Social networks, host resistance, and mortality: a nine-year follow-up study of Alameda County residents.  Am J Epidemiol. 1979;109(2):186-204.PubMedGoogle Scholar
27.
Andresen  EM, Malmgren  JA, Carter  WB, Patrick  DL.  Screening for depression in well older adults: evaluation of a short form of the CES-D (Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale).  Am J Prev Med. 1994;10(2):77-84.PubMedGoogle Scholar
28.
Burgess  PM, Mazzocco  L, Campbell  IM.  Discriminant validity of the Crown-Crisp Experiential Index.  Br J Med Psychol. 1987;60(pt 1):61-69.PubMedGoogle Scholar
29.
Hernán  MA, Brumback  B, Robins  JM.  Marginal structural models to estimate the causal effect of zidovudine on the survival of HIV-positive men.  Epidemiology. 2000;11(5):561-570.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
30.
Robins  JM, Hernán  MÁ, Brumback  B.  Marginal structural models and causal inference in epidemiology.  Epidemiology. 2000;11(5):550-560.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
31.
Knol  MJ, VanderWeele  TJ, Groenwold  RH, Klungel  OH, Rovers  MM, Grobbee  DE.  Estimating measures of interaction on an additive scale for preventive exposures.  Eur J Epidemiol. 2011;26(6):433-438.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
32.
VanderWeele  TJ, Vansteelandt  S.  Invited commentary: some advantages of the relative excess risk due to interaction (RERI)—towards better estimators of additive interaction.  Am J Epidemiol. 2014;179(6):670-671.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
33.
VanderWeele  T.  Explanation in Causal Inference: Methods for Mediation and Interaction. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2015.
34.
Valeri  L, VanderWeele  TJ.  SAS macro for causal mediation analysis with survival data.  Epidemiology. 2015;26(2):e23-e24.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
35.
Zucker  DM.  Restricted mean life with covariates: modification and extension of a useful survival analysis method.  J Am Stat Assoc. 1998;93(442):702-709. doi:10.1080/01621459.1998.10473722.Google ScholarCrossref
36.
Ding  P, VanderWeele  T.  Sensitivity analysis without assumptions [published online February 1, 2016].  Epidemiology.PubMedGoogle Scholar
37.
Hill  PL, Turiano  NA.  Purpose in life as a predictor of mortality across adulthood.  Psychol Sci. 2014;25(7):1482-1486.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
38.
Levin  JS, Larson  DB, Puchalski  CM.  Religion and spirituality in medicine: research and education.  JAMA. 1997;278(9):792-793.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
39.
Matthews  DA, McCullough  ME, Larson  DB, Koenig  HG, Swyers  JP, Milano  MG.  Religious commitment and health status: a review of the research and implications for family medicine.  Arch Fam Med. 1998;7(2):118-124.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
40.
Koenig  HG.  Medicine, Religion, and Health: Where Science & Spirituality Meet. West Conshohocken, PA: Templeton Foundation Press; 2008.
41.
Levin  JS.  Religion and health: is there an association, is it valid, and is it causal?  Soc Sci Med. 1994;38(11):1475-1482.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
42.
Hummer  RA, Rogers  RG, Nam  CB, Ellison  CG.  Religious involvement and US adult mortality.  Demography. 1999;36(2):273-285.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
43.
Oman  D, Reed  D.  Religion and mortality among the community-dwelling elderly.  Am J Public Health. 1998;88(10):1469-1475.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
44.
Idler  EL, Musick  MA, Ellison  CG,  et al.  Measuring multiple dimensions of religion and spirituality for health research: conceptual background and findings from the 1998 General Social Survey.  Res Aging. 2003;25(4):327-365. doi:10.1177/0164027503025004001.Google ScholarCrossref
45.
Hill  PC, Pargament  KI.  Advances in the conceptualization and measurement of religion and spirituality: implications for physical and mental health research.  Am Psychol. 2003;58(1):64-74.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
46.
Hadaway  CK, Marler  PL, Chaves  M.  What the polls don’t show—a closer look at United States church attendance.  Am Sociol Rev. 1993;58(6):741-752. doi:10.2307/2095948.Google ScholarCrossref
47.
Presser  S, Stinson  L.  Data collection mode and social desirability bias in self-reported religious attendance.  Am Sociol Rev. 1998;63(1):137-145.Google ScholarCrossref
48.
Chaves  M.  American Religion: Contemporary Trends. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press; 2011.
49.
Rothman  KJ.  Modern Epidemiology. 3rd ed. Philadelphia, PA: Wolters Kluwer Health/Lippincott Williams & Wilkins; 2008.
50.
Musick  MA, House  JS, Williams  DR.  Attendance at religious services and mortality in a national sample.  J Health Soc Behav. 2004;45(2):198-213.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
1 Comment for this article
EXPAND ALL
My Support of the Article
Sujittra Chaturongkul | Cancer Patient
From my opinion, cancer is the killer that comes from any source and attacks in any variety of ways. It's the multiplication of cells through genetic mutation that causes death by hampering the body to function normally. The body that is healthy, that is well-controlled by the mind, (I believe mind controls the body.), should be able to handle the aberration better than others. That kind of body is conscious of the change easier, knows more about foreign invasion, and is easier to fix like a good machine is easier to fix than a bad machine. I think I owe my better recovery to the practice of regular meditation and the competent hematologist. (After chemotherapy, I have 83% tumor reduction. The cancer is non-Hodgkin's lymphoma. The meditation is 46 years.)
CONFLICT OF INTEREST: None Reported
READ MORE
Original Investigation
June 2016

Association of Religious Service Attendance With Mortality Among Women

Author Affiliations
  • 1Department of Nutrition, Harvard T. H. Chan School of Public Health, Boston, Massachusetts
  • 2Department of Epidemiology, Harvard T. H. Chan School of Public Health, Boston, Massachusetts
  • 3Channing Division of Network Medicine, Department of Medicine, Brigham and Women’s Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts
  • 4Department of Social and Behavioral Sciences, Harvard T. H. Chan School of Public Health, Boston, Massachusetts
  • 5Department of Biostatistics, Harvard T. H. Chan School of Public Health, Boston, Massachusetts
JAMA Intern Med. 2016;176(6):777-785. doi:10.1001/jamainternmed.2016.1615
Abstract

Importance  Studies on the association between attendance at religious services and mortality often have been limited by inadequate methods for reverse causation, inability to assess effects over time, and limited information on mediators and cause-specific mortality.

Objective  To evaluate associations between attendance at religious services and subsequent mortality in women.

Design, Setting, and Participants  Attendance at religious services was assessed from the first questionnaire in 1992 through June 2012, by a self-reported question asked of 74 534 women in the Nurses’ Health Study who were free of cardiovascular disease and cancer at baseline. Data analysis was conducted from return of the 1996 questionnaire through June 2012.

Main Outcomes and Measures  Cox proportional hazards regression model and marginal structural models with time-varying covariates were used to examine the association of attendance at religious services with all-cause and cause-specific mortality. We adjusted for a wide range of demographic covariates, lifestyle factors, and medical history measured repeatedly during the follow-up, and performed sensitivity analyses to examine the influence of potential unmeasured and residual confounding.

Results  Among the 74 534 women participants, there were 13 537 deaths, including 2721 owing to cardiovascular deaths and 4479 owing to cancer deaths. After multivariable adjustment for major lifestyle factors, risk factors, and attendance at religious services in 1992, attending a religious service more than once per week was associated with 33% lower all-cause mortality compared with women who had never attended religious services (hazard ratio, 0.67; 95% CI, 0.62-0.71; P < .001 for trend). Comparing women who attended religious services more than once per week with those who never attend, the hazard ratio for cardiovascular mortality was 0.73 (95% CI, 0.62-0.85; P < .001 for trend) and for cancer mortality was 0.79 (95% CI, 0.70-0.89; P < .001 for trend). Results were robust in sensitivity analysis. Depressive symptoms, smoking, social support, and optimism were potentially important mediators, although the overall proportion of the association between attendance at religious services and mortality was moderate (eg, social support explained 23% of the effect [P = .003], depressive symptoms explained 11% [P < .001], smoking explained 22% [P < .001], and optimism explained 9% [P < .001]).

Conclusions and Relevance  Frequent attendance at religious services was associated with significantly lower risk of all-cause, cardiovascular, and cancer mortality among women. Religion and spirituality may be an underappreciated resource that physicians could explore with their patients, as appropriate.

Introduction

The World Health Organization defines health as “a state of complete physical, mental and social well-being.”1 Certain religious groups and others likewise view health holistically and emphasize the unity of body, mind, and spirit.2-4 Health is often viewed as an inseparable component of spiritual well-being within some religious understandings.5,6 Religious participation and beliefs can affect individual behavior, shift cognition and emotion, promote compassion, shape communities and public life, and may otherwise promote well-being, health, and wholeness, but religion can also promote guilt, anxiety, violence, and intolerance. A priori, the effects of religious practice on health are not thus immediately clear. Religious practice is common in the United States: approximately 65% of Americans consider religion to be an important part of life, 83% report praying to God in the last week, and 43% report having attended a religious service in the past week.7,8

A meta-analysis of studies on the connection between attendance at religious services and mortality between 1994 and 2009 concluded that religious service attendance helped reduce mortality by 18% in healthy populations.9 Research on religion and health has led to some controversy.9-12 Sloan et al13,14 questioned the validity of these studies and argued that the evidence is often weak and unconvincing, with poor methods and study design. Denberg criticized this kind of research as “simply reporting an association and then calling for more future research,” arguing that it was “trivial and unworthy of publication.”15(p430) Koenig et al16 responded that the review by Sloan et al was highly selective and biased with a misunderstanding of the epidemiologic method.

Much of the debate concerned major limitations to the methods in previous studies10,17: that it was difficult to infer causality and that the observed association could be owing to reverse causation if only healthy participants were able to attend services. Some stronger longitudinal studies on this subject have been published.18-20 However, to our knowledge, no previous study has used methods for repeated measures and time-dependent confounding that handle potential reverse causation between service attendance and health and evaluate incident (ie, current, conditional on the past) rather than prevalent (ie, already present) attendance at religious services and its association with mortality. We address these issues using the Nurses’ Health Study, a large prospective cohort study among US women with repeated measurements of attendance at religious services, including detailed information on diet and lifestyle, medical history, and long-term follow-up. In addition, we assess interactions with race, time-varying effects, and cause-specific mortality.

Box Section Ref ID

Key Points

  • Question Is frequent attendance at religious services associated with lower mortality among women?

  • Findings In a large prospective long-term cohort study of US women, the Nurses’ Health Study, there was a consistent inverse association between frequent attendance at religious services and all-cause mortality, cardiovascular mortality, and cancer mortality. Compared with women who never attended religious services, women who attended more than once per week had 33% lower mortality during 16 years of follow-up, with depressive symptoms, smoking, social support, and optimism as potential mediators.

  • Meaning For those who already hold religious beliefs, religion and spirituality may be an underappreciated resource that physicians could explore with their patients, as appropriate.

Methods
Study Design

The Nurses’ Health Study began in 1976 and included 121 700 nurses aged 30 to 55 years from across the United States.21 Information on lifestyle and medical history was collected using a self-administered questionnaire at baseline and every 2 years thereafter. Attendance at religious services was first self-reported in 1992, and every 4 years subsequently, in response to the question, “How often do you go to religious meetings or services?” Response categories included more than once a week, once a week, 1 to 3 times per month, less than once per month, never (or almost never). We defined baseline for this analysis as attendance at religious services as assessed in the 1996 questionnaire; we used attendance in 1992 as an additional covariate. Follow-up for mortality continued through June 2012. Participants who did not reply to the 1996 questionnaire or who died before the baseline of 1996 (n = 27 101), who had missing data for attendance at religious services in 1996 (n = 7246), or who had a diagnosis of cardiovascular disease (n = 4362) or cancer (n = 8457, except nonmelanoma skin cancer) before 1996 were excluded. Participants were followed up from the return of the 1996 questionnaire until death, loss to follow-up, or the end of follow-up in June 2012, whichever came first. Our study included 74 534 participants with 1 104 175 person-years. There were 13 537 total deaths, including 2721 cardiovascular deaths and 4479 cancer deaths, during follow up. The study protocol was approved by the Brigham and Women’s Hospital and Harvard T. H. Chan School of Public Health Institutional Review Boards.

Outcome

All-cause and cause-specific mortality was assessed between the return of the 1996 questionnaire and the end of follow-up (June 2012). Deaths were identified through reports from next of kin and the National Death Index. We identified causes of death based on family reports, death certificates, and medical records. We searched the National Death Index for names of nonresponders; this has been shown to have good sensitivity and specificity.22 We used International Classification of Diseases, 8th revision (ICD-8) codes to define cancer-related deaths (codes 140-207) and cardiovascular disease–related deaths (codes 390-459 and 795). We used the following ICD-8 codes to identify detailed subcategories of cardiovascular disease and cancer: ischemic heart disease (codes 410-414), cerebrovascular disease (codes 430-438), and any other cardiovascular disease (codes 390-459 and 795, excluding 410-414 and 430-438), and cancers of the lung (code 162), breast (code 174), ovaries (code 183), pancreas (code 157), colon or rectum (codes 153 and 154), and non-Hodgkin lymphoma (codes 200, 202, 204) and cancer of other sites.

Covariates

We adjusted the analyses for the following known predictors of mortality in this cohort: age (as a continuous variable in years), alcohol consumption (none, 0.1-4.9, 5.0-14.9, or ≥15.0 g/d), physical exercise (metabolic equivalent hours per week; quintiles), multivitamin use (yes or no), hypertension (yes or no), hypercholesterolemia (yes or no), type 2 diabetes mellitus (yes or no), menopausal status (premenopausal or postmenopausal), postmenopausal hormone use (never, past, and current), physical examination in the past 2 years (no, yes for symptoms, and yes for screenings), Alternate Healthy Eating Index–2010 score (quintiles),23 smoking status (never, former, or current), pack-years (<10, 10-19, 20-39, or ≥40 pack-years for former smokers; <25, 25-44, 45-64,or ≥65 pack-years for current smokers), body mass index (calculated as weight in kilograms divided by height in meters squared; <21.0, 21.0-22.9, 23.0-24.9, 25.0-27.4, 27.5-29.9, 30.0-34.9, or ≥35.0), husband’s educational level (less than high school, some high school, high school graduate, college, or graduate school), good physical function (defined as absence of limitations in moderate activities or moderate limitations in demanding activities; yes or no24,25), social integration score (derived from the following 6 components: marital status, other group participation, number of close friends, number of close relatives, number of close friends seen at least once per month, number of close relatives seen at least once per month26; quartiles), living alone (yes or no), median family income (dollars per year; quintiles), geographic region (North, South, Midwest, or other), depression in 1992 (yes or no), and attendance at religious services in 1992 (never, <1 time per week, or ≥1 time per week). Indicator variables were used for any missing covariate information for categorical variables and median imputation was used for missing continuous covariates.

For mediation analysis, covariate measurements before the religious attendance exposure were taken as potential confounders and those subsequent to religious attendance exposures were taken as potential mediators. For mediators, we considered the first measure available subsequent to 1996, which included, in 2000, depressive symptoms measured using the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression 10 scale27; in 1998, smoking, alcohol consumption, and diet quality; in 2000, number of close friends and having someone close to talk to; and in 2004, optimism and phobic anxiety measured using the Crown-Crisp Experiential Index.28

Statistical Analysis

Data analysis was conducted from return of the 1996 questionnaire through June 2012. We examined the association of attendance at religious services with all-cause and cause-specific mortality using various analytic strategies including Cox proportional hazards regression models and marginal structural models with weights accounting for missing data and censoring. The marginal structural models account for reverse causation and time-varying confounding by weighting and are described at length elsewhere,29,30 and in eAppendix 1 in the Supplement. Person-time was accrued from baseline (return of the 1996 questionnaire) until the date of death, loss to follow-up, or June 2012, whichever came first. We calculated hazard ratios (HRs) and their 95% CIs, comparing frequency of attendance at religious services (more than once per week, once per week, or less than once per week) vs never attend, for all-cause mortality and cause-specific mortality. For cause-specific mortality, we also further adjusted for cause-specific risk factors. Linear trends across categories of attendance at religious services were tested by modeling attendance frequency as a continuous variable. Confounders were adjusted for in 1992 in the Cox proportional hazards regression models; confounders were updated over time in the marginal structural model. We examined the joint effect of attendance at religious services in 1996 and 2000 with all-cause mortality from 2000 to 2012. We further stratified the analysis by race/ethnicity (among white and African American participants only), and religious group (among Catholic and Protestant participants only). Likelihood ratio tests were used to assess the significance of the interaction. The relative excess risk due to interaction and its 95% CI were calculated.31,32

We applied mediation analysis methods33,34 to examine proportions of the association between attendance at religious services in 1996 and mortality in 2012 that was mediated by the following factors: current smoking, alcohol intake, and diet quality in 1998; social support and depressive symptoms in 2000; components of social integration in 2000 (including currently married, number of close friends, number of close relatives, seen close friends at least once per month, seen close relatives at least once per month, and hours of social group participation); and phobic anxiety and optimism in 2004. For the mediation analysis, we further excluded participants who had mediator information missing or who died between baseline and the mediator measurement. These mediators were selected a priori based on subject knowledge and assessed using multivariable logistic regression and linear regression models of the outcome and of the potential mediator, the results of which were then combined to estimate direct and indirect effects.33,34 Methods for mediation assume baseline covariates suffice to control for exposure-outcome, mediator-outcome, and exposure-mediator confounding. Proportion mediated on a risk difference scale was calculated as the indirect effect divided by the total effect and tests were conducted for evidence of mediation. We further examined the change in attendance at religious services over time and calculated years of life saved.35

We conducted several sensitivity analyses to test the robustness of our results. To minimize the influence of reverse causation, we additionally performed subgroup analyses among participants who were not living in a nursing home, never smokers, with no physical or functional limitations, and no major medical comorbidities (such as depression), and excluded death events in the first 4 years of follow-up. We compared effects sizes of attendance at religious services with other components of social integration and with other covariates. We also updated covariates, modeled attendance at religious services as time-varying exposure, compared HRs over different specific time frames of follow-up, and with different analytic strategies as sensitivity analyses. We further assessed how substantial residual unmeasured confounding would need to be to explain away the observed associations.17,36

Results

Among 74 534 women at 1996 baseline with reported religious service attendance, 14 158 attended more than once per week, 30 401 attended once per week, 12 103 attended less than once per week, and 17 872 never attended (Table 1). Most of our study participants were Catholic or Protestant. Women who attended religious services more frequently tended to have fewer depressive symptoms, were less likely to be current smokers, and were more likely to be married (Table 1). During follow up, most participants maintained their levels of attendance at religious services, but there was also considerable movement across all categories (eTable 1 in the Supplement).

Using a Cox proportional hazards regression model, compared with women who never attended religious services, women who attended regularly had lower mortality on follow-up (Figure), with a multivariable-adjusted HR of 0.67 (95% CI, 0.62-0.71) for those attending more than once per week in 1996, HR of 0.74 (95% CI, 0.70-0.78) for those attending weekly, and HR of 0.87 (95% CI, 0.81-0.92) for those attending less than weekly (P < .001 for trend). Those who attended religious services regularly in both 2000 and 1996 had even lower mortality rates, with a multivariable-adjusted HR of 0.55 (95% CI, 0.52-0.59 [Table 2]). When using marginal structural models to better address potential feedback and reverse causation, the effect sizes were similar (Table 2). Results were also similar in analyses among participants who were not living in a nursing home, never smokers, with no physical or functional limitations, and no major medical comorbidities (such as depression), and with exclusion of death events in the first 4 years of follow up (eTable 2 and eTable 3 in the Supplement). Effect size of attendance at religious services was comparable with those of various health behaviors (eTable 4 in the Supplement). The inverse association between attendance at religious services and mortality were consistent over time (eTable 5 in the Supplement). Attendance at religious services once per week or more was associated with 0.43 (95% CI, –0.09 to –1.54) years’ longer survival for the 16 years of the study (eAppendix 2 in the Supplement).

In the Cox model, for an unmeasured confounder to explain the HR estimate of 0.67, the unmeasured confounder would have to both increase the likelihood of attendance at religious services and decrease the likelihood of mortality by 2.35-fold above and beyond the measured confounders. For an unmeasured confounder to bring the upper confidence limit of 0.71 for this estimate above 1.0, the unmeasured confounder would still have to both increase the likelihood of attendance at religious services and decrease the likelihood of mortality by 2.16-fold. Similar substantial confounding would be needed to explain the other estimates.

For cause-specific mortality, frequent attendance at religious services was also inversely associated with cardiovascular mortality and cancer mortality, with an HR of 0.73 (95% CI, 0.62-0.85; P < .001 for trend) and an HR of 0.79 (95% CI, 0.70-0.89; P < .001 for trend), respectively (Table 3). Attendance at religious services was associated with lower mortality from cerebrovascular disease and other cardiovascular diseases, but not from ischemic heart disease (eTable 6 in the Supplement). For site-specific cancer mortality, frequent attendance at religious services is associated with significantly lower risk of breast cancer mortality and colorectal cancer mortality, but not for other sites of cancer (eTable 7 in the Supplement). Although attendance at religious services was associated with lower cardiovascular mortality and cancer mortality, attendance was not significantly associated with incidence of breast cancer (eTable 8 in the Supplement) or cardiovascular disease (eTable 9 in the Supplement).

The HR comparing those attending religious services more than once per week with those not attending was 0.88 (95% CI, 0.85-0.92) for white participants and 0.64 (95% CI, 0.46-0.90) for African American participants (eTable 10 in the Supplement; P = .08 for heterogeneity). The HRs for service attendance were comparable for Protestants and Catholics; for each level of service attendance, Catholics had slightly lower mortality than did Protestants (eTable 11 in the Supplement). We further compared the magnitude of the association of attendance at religious services with other aspects of social integration and found that the inverse association with mortality was strongest for attendance at religious services (eTable 12 in the Supplement).

We used mediation analysis to estimate the proportion of the association that was mediated through each mediator. Depressive symptoms, smoking, social support, and optimism were potentially important mediators, although the overall proportion of the association that was mediated through each mediator was moderate (eg, smoking explained 22% of the effect, social support explained 23%) (Table 4).26

Discussion

In this large prospective cohort of US nurses, we found a consistent inverse association between frequent attendance at religious services and all-cause mortality, cardiovascular mortality, and cancer mortality. Compared with women who never attended religious services, women who attended services more than once per week had a 33% lower mortality risk; results were robust across different race/ethnicity groups, different analytic strategies, and in sensitivity analyses.

In examining the potential pathways from religious service to all-cause mortality, we found that depressive symptoms, smoking, social support, and optimism were potentially important mediators. No single mediator explained more than about 25% of the effect. There may be many pathways from attendance at religious services to health. However, the proportion of effects mediated may be underestimated, as mediators were considered only at a single time point and are measured imperfectly. Moreover, some individuals died before the mediators occurred and were excluded further, which may affect our results. Future studies implementing causal mediation analysis with time-varying attendance at religious services, time-varying mediators, and confounders are warranted. Other mechanisms that have been proposed that were not assessed here include increasing psychosocial resilience, religious coping mechanisms, purpose in life, and self-discipline.37

Our findings were consistent with the results of previous studies and the effect sizes were similar or somewhat larger, especially when examining associations with a consistent pattern of attendance at religious services. The literature supports the notion that attendance at religious services is associated with better health and reduced mortality.14,38-43 In our study, we were able to account for time-dependent confounding and examined the association between repeated measures of attendance at religious services with long-term all-cause and cause-specific mortality. Although our study was not targeted to a particular religious group, the study consists mainly of white Christians. Our results might not be generalizable to the general population, other countries, or areas with limited religious freedom. Moreover, our study population consists of US nurses with similar socioeconomic status who tend to be more health conscious. Our analysis is also restricted to the specific period under consideration, and the effects of attendance at religious services may vary over time as the nature of attendance itself changes. Although frequency of attendance at religious services did not substantially change in our study, it is possible that the content of the services themselves changed. Further research could examine other religious practices, mindfulness practices, other aspects of spirituality and religiosity, other race/ethnicity and demographic groups, and could further investigate the potential underlying mechanisms of causal pathways.44,45 Our results do not imply that health care professionals should prescribe attendance at religious services, but for those who already hold religious beliefs, attendance at services could be encouraged as a form of meaningful social participation.

One limitation of our study is that we have only 1 domain measure of religiosity or spirituality, namely, attendance at religious services. This domain captures only 1 aspect of religiosity and may be subject to measurement error and overreporting,46-48 although with overreporting the relative ordering of frequency might still be preserved. There is no reason to think that individual overreporting would be related to mortality, and such nondifferential misclassification in fact tends to yield conservative effect estimates.49 Our finding of substantially lower breast cancer mortality in frequent attenders, despite no association with breast cancer incidence, lends support to an effect of social participation and enhances the plausibility of our results. Attendance at religious services may be highly correlated with other measures of social engagement, such as number of close friends and having someone close to talk to, which are significant predictors for lower mortality and thus may serve as important mechanisms. However, some studies of the health effects of religious attendance4,50 have examined the role of other measures of social engagement and find that religious attendance has robust effects even after the inclusion of these measures. In our study, this was also the case, and we moreover found that the inverse association between social support and mortality was driven substantially by attendance at religious services. Future research could assess associations with other forms of social participation.

A randomized trial of attendance at religious services is neither ethical nor feasible. Our study is an observational study. Although we adjusted for major confounders for the association between attendance at religious services and mortality, the results may still be subject to unmeasured confounders and residual confounding. Personal, social, psychological, and socioeconomic characteristics may confound attendance at religious services and explain the association; for example, data on optimism were not available at baseline. However, we performed sensitivity analysis techniques to assess how strong unmeasured confounding would need to be to explain the observed association. For an unmeasured confounder to explain the association of attendance at religious services and lower mortality, it would have to both increase the likelihood of attendance at services and decrease the likelihood of mortality by 2.35-fold above and beyond the measured covariates. Such substantial confounding by unmeasured factors seems unlikely, given adjustment for an extensive set of covariates. We also performed subgroup analyses among participants who were not living in a nursing home, never smokers, had no physical or functional limitations, and had no major medical comorbidities (such as depression), and we excluded death events in the first 4 years of follow-up, and estimates were similar.

Strengths of our study include a large sample size, long duration of follow-up, prospective cohort study design, and repeated measures of attendance at religious services, analytic methods for feedback and reverse causation, and extensive control of confounding. We have clear temporality of the exposure, covariates, and outcome, and have been able to adjust for baseline attendance at religious services and baseline confounders, and account for time-dependent confounding. Our results were robust across statistical methods of analysis, exclusions to address reverse causation, and in sensitivity analysis for unmeasured confounding.

Conclusions

In this large prospective long-term cohort study of US women, frequent attendance at religious services, particularly recent attendance, was associated with lower risk of all-cause mortality, cardiovascular mortality, and cancer mortality.

Back to top
Article Information

Corresponding Author: Tyler J. VanderWeele, PhD, Department of Epidemiology, Harvard T. H. Chan School of Public Health, 677 Huntington Ave, Kresge Bldg, Boston, MA 02115 (tvanderw@hsph.harvard.edu).

Accepted for Publication: March 17, 2016.

Published Online: May 16, 2016. doi:10.1001/jamainternmed.2016.1615.

Author Contributions: Drs Li and VanderWeele had full access to all the data in the study and take responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.

Study concept and design: Li, VanderWeele.

Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: All authors.

Drafting of the manuscript: Li, VanderWeele.

Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: All authors.

Statistical analysis: Li, VanderWeele.

Obtained funding: VanderWeele.

Administrative, technical, or material support: Stampfer, Williams, VanderWeele.

Study supervision: Stampfer, VanderWeele.

Conflict of Interest Disclosures: None reported.

Funding/Support: The Nurses’ Health Study was funded by grant UM1 CA186107 from the National Institutes of Health. The analysis and article was supported by a research grant from the Templeton Foundation.

Role of the Funder/Sponsor: The funding sources played no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

References
1.
World Health Organization. WHO definition of health. http://www.who.int/about/definition/en/print.html. Accessed April 7, 2016.
2.
Balboni  MJ, Puchalski  CM, Peteet  JR.  The relationship between medicine, spirituality and religion: three models for integration.  J Relig Health. 2014;53(5):1586-1598.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
Koenig  HG.  Faith & Mental Health. Philadelphia, PA: Templeton Foundation Press; 2005.
4.
Koenig  H, King  DE, Benner  V.  Handbook of Religion and Health. 2nd ed. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2012.
5.
Puchalski  CM, Blatt  B, Kogan  M, Butler  A.  Spirituality and health: the development of a field.  Acad Med. 2014;89(1):10-16.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Puchalski  CM, Vitillo  R, Hull  SK, Reller  N.  Improving the spiritual dimension of whole person care: reaching national and international consensus.  J Palliat Med. 2014;17(6):642-656.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Crabtree  S, Pelham  B. What Alabamians and Iranians have in common; a global perspective on Americans’ religiosity offers a few surprises. Gallup Poll News Service. https://www.highbeam.com/doc/1G1-193998914.html. Published February 9, 2009. Accessed April 8, 2016. Gallup Poll News Service; 2009.
8.
Franck  R, Iannaccone  L.  Religious decline in the 20th century West: testing alternative explanations.  Public Choice. 2014;159(3-4):385-414. doi:10.1007/s11127-013-0103-9.Google ScholarCrossref
9.
Lucchetti  G, Lucchetti  AL, Koenig  HG.  Impact of spirituality/religiosity on mortality: comparison with other health interventions.  Explore (NY). 2011;7(4):234-238.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
McCullough  ME, Hoyt  WT, Larson  DB, Koenig  HG, Thoresen  C.  Religious involvement and mortality: a meta-analytic review.  Health Psychol. 2000;19(3):211-222.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
Hall  DE.  Religious attendance: more cost-effective than Lipitor?  J Am Board Fam Med. 2006;19(2):103-109.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Larson  DB, Larson  SS, Koenig  HG.  Mortality and religion/spirituality: a brief review of the research.  Ann Pharmacother. 2002;36(6):1090-1098.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Sloan  RP, Bagiella  E, VandeCreek  L,  et al.  Should physicians prescribe religious activities?  N Engl J Med. 2000;342(25):1913-1916.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
Sloan  RP, Bagiella  E, Powell  T.  Religion, spirituality, and medicine.  Lancet. 1999;353(9153):664-667.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Denberg  T.  Religious attendance: more cost-effective than Lipitor?  J Am Board Fam Med. 2006;19(4):430.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Koenig  HG, Idler  E, Kasl  S,  et al.  Religion, spirituality, and medicine: a rebuttal to skeptics.  Int J Psychiatry Med. 1999;29(2):123-131.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
VanderWeele  TJ.  Unmeasured confounding and hazard scales: sensitivity analysis for total, direct, and indirect effects.  Eur J Epidemiol. 2013;28(2):113-117.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
Dupre  ME, Franzese  AT, Parrado  EA.  Religious attendance and mortality: implications for the black–white mortality crossover.  Demography. 2006;43(1):141-164.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
19.
Strawbridge  WJ, Cohen  RD, Shema  SJ, Kaplan  GA.  Frequent attendance at religious services and mortality over 28 years.  Am J Public Health. 1997;87(6):957-961.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Ellison  CG, Hummer  RA, Cormier  S, Rogers  RG.  Religious involvement and mortality risk among African American adults.  Res Aging. 2000;22(6):630-677. doi:10.1177/0164027500226003. Google ScholarCrossref
21.
Colditz  GA, Hankinson  SE.  The Nurses’ Health Study: lifestyle and health among women.  Nat Rev Cancer. 2005;5(5):388-396.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
Rich-Edwards  JW, Corsano  KA, Stampfer  MJ.  Test of the national death index and Equifax nationwide death search.  Am J Epidemiol. 1994;140(11):1016-1019.PubMedGoogle Scholar
23.
Chiuve  SE, Sampson  L, Willett  WC.  The association between a nutritional quality index and risk of chronic disease.  Am J Prev Med. 2011;40(5):505-513.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Sun  Q, Townsend  MK, Okereke  OI,  et al.  Alcohol consumption at midlife and successful ageing in women: a prospective cohort analysis in the Nurses’ Health Study.  PLoS Med. 2011;8(9):e1001090.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
Ware  JE  Jr, Sherbourne  CD.  The MOS 36-item short-form health survey (SF-36): I: conceptual framework and item selection.  Med Care. 1992;30(6):473-483.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
Berkman  LF, Syme  SL.  Social networks, host resistance, and mortality: a nine-year follow-up study of Alameda County residents.  Am J Epidemiol. 1979;109(2):186-204.PubMedGoogle Scholar
27.
Andresen  EM, Malmgren  JA, Carter  WB, Patrick  DL.  Screening for depression in well older adults: evaluation of a short form of the CES-D (Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale).  Am J Prev Med. 1994;10(2):77-84.PubMedGoogle Scholar
28.
Burgess  PM, Mazzocco  L, Campbell  IM.  Discriminant validity of the Crown-Crisp Experiential Index.  Br J Med Psychol. 1987;60(pt 1):61-69.PubMedGoogle Scholar
29.
Hernán  MA, Brumback  B, Robins  JM.  Marginal structural models to estimate the causal effect of zidovudine on the survival of HIV-positive men.  Epidemiology. 2000;11(5):561-570.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
30.
Robins  JM, Hernán  MÁ, Brumback  B.  Marginal structural models and causal inference in epidemiology.  Epidemiology. 2000;11(5):550-560.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
31.
Knol  MJ, VanderWeele  TJ, Groenwold  RH, Klungel  OH, Rovers  MM, Grobbee  DE.  Estimating measures of interaction on an additive scale for preventive exposures.  Eur J Epidemiol. 2011;26(6):433-438.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
32.
VanderWeele  TJ, Vansteelandt  S.  Invited commentary: some advantages of the relative excess risk due to interaction (RERI)—towards better estimators of additive interaction.  Am J Epidemiol. 2014;179(6):670-671.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
33.
VanderWeele  T.  Explanation in Causal Inference: Methods for Mediation and Interaction. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2015.
34.
Valeri  L, VanderWeele  TJ.  SAS macro for causal mediation analysis with survival data.  Epidemiology. 2015;26(2):e23-e24.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
35.
Zucker  DM.  Restricted mean life with covariates: modification and extension of a useful survival analysis method.  J Am Stat Assoc. 1998;93(442):702-709. doi:10.1080/01621459.1998.10473722.Google ScholarCrossref
36.
Ding  P, VanderWeele  T.  Sensitivity analysis without assumptions [published online February 1, 2016].  Epidemiology.PubMedGoogle Scholar
37.
Hill  PL, Turiano  NA.  Purpose in life as a predictor of mortality across adulthood.  Psychol Sci. 2014;25(7):1482-1486.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
38.
Levin  JS, Larson  DB, Puchalski  CM.  Religion and spirituality in medicine: research and education.  JAMA. 1997;278(9):792-793.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
39.
Matthews  DA, McCullough  ME, Larson  DB, Koenig  HG, Swyers  JP, Milano  MG.  Religious commitment and health status: a review of the research and implications for family medicine.  Arch Fam Med. 1998;7(2):118-124.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
40.
Koenig  HG.  Medicine, Religion, and Health: Where Science & Spirituality Meet. West Conshohocken, PA: Templeton Foundation Press; 2008.
41.
Levin  JS.  Religion and health: is there an association, is it valid, and is it causal?  Soc Sci Med. 1994;38(11):1475-1482.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
42.
Hummer  RA, Rogers  RG, Nam  CB, Ellison  CG.  Religious involvement and US adult mortality.  Demography. 1999;36(2):273-285.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
43.
Oman  D, Reed  D.  Religion and mortality among the community-dwelling elderly.  Am J Public Health. 1998;88(10):1469-1475.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
44.
Idler  EL, Musick  MA, Ellison  CG,  et al.  Measuring multiple dimensions of religion and spirituality for health research: conceptual background and findings from the 1998 General Social Survey.  Res Aging. 2003;25(4):327-365. doi:10.1177/0164027503025004001.Google ScholarCrossref
45.
Hill  PC, Pargament  KI.  Advances in the conceptualization and measurement of religion and spirituality: implications for physical and mental health research.  Am Psychol. 2003;58(1):64-74.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
46.
Hadaway  CK, Marler  PL, Chaves  M.  What the polls don’t show—a closer look at United States church attendance.  Am Sociol Rev. 1993;58(6):741-752. doi:10.2307/2095948.Google ScholarCrossref
47.
Presser  S, Stinson  L.  Data collection mode and social desirability bias in self-reported religious attendance.  Am Sociol Rev. 1998;63(1):137-145.Google ScholarCrossref
48.
Chaves  M.  American Religion: Contemporary Trends. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press; 2011.
49.
Rothman  KJ.  Modern Epidemiology. 3rd ed. Philadelphia, PA: Wolters Kluwer Health/Lippincott Williams & Wilkins; 2008.
50.
Musick  MA, House  JS, Williams  DR.  Attendance at religious services and mortality in a national sample.  J Health Soc Behav. 2004;45(2):198-213.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
×