[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Figure 1.  Cross-sectional Association Between Thyroid Dysfunction and Cognitive Function Test Scores
Cross-sectional Association Between Thyroid Dysfunction and Cognitive Function Test Scores

Standardized mean differences were adjusted for age and sex. Error bars indicate 95% CIs.

Figure 2.  Longitudinal Association Between Thyroid Dysfunction and Cognitive Function Test Scores
Longitudinal Association Between Thyroid Dysfunction and Cognitive Function Test Scores

Standardized mean differences were adjusted for age, sex, and baseline cognitive function. Error bars indicate 95% CIs.

Figure 3.  Longitudinal Association Between Thyroid Dysfunction and Incident Dementia
Longitudinal Association Between Thyroid Dysfunction and Incident Dementia

Hazard ratios were adjusted for age and sex. Error bars indicate 95% CIs.

Table.  Baseline Characteristics of the 38 144 Participants With Cognitive Function Measurements in Included Studies
Baseline Characteristics of the 38 144 Participants With Cognitive Function Measurements in Included Studies
1.
Knopman  DS, DeKosky  ST, Cummings  JL,  et al.  Practice parameter: diagnosis of dementia (an evidence-based review). report of the Quality Standards Subcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology.   Neurology. 2001;56(9):1143-1153. doi:10.1212/WNL.56.9.1143 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
Sorbi  S, Hort  J, Erkinjuntti  T,  et al; EFNS Scientist Panel on Dementia and Cognitive Neurology.  EFNS-ENS Guidelines on the diagnosis and management of disorders associated with dementia.   Eur J Neurol. 2012;19(9):1159-1179. doi:10.1111/j.1468-1331.2012.03784.xPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
Cognitive Decline Partnership Centre. Clinical practice guidelines and principles for people with dementia. Published February 2016. Accessed August 2, 2021. https://cdpc.sydney.edu.au/research/clinical-guidelines-for-dementia/
4.
Muangpaisan  W, Petcharat  C, Srinonprasert  V.  Prevalence of potentially reversible conditions in dementia and mild cognitive impairment in a geriatric clinic.   Geriatr Gerontol Int. 2012;12(1):59-64. doi:10.1111/j.1447-0594.2011.00728.x PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Ritchie  M, Yeap  BB.  Thyroid hormone: influences on mood and cognition in adults.   Maturitas. 2015;81(2):266-275. doi:10.1016/j.maturitas.2015.03.016 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Vogel  A, Elberling  TV, Hørding  M,  et al.  Affective symptoms and cognitive functions in the acute phase of Graves’ thyrotoxicosis.   Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2007;32(1):36-43. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2006.09.012 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Wekking  EM, Appelhof  BC, Fliers  E,  et al.  Cognitive functioning and well-being in euthyroid patients on thyroxine replacement therapy for primary hypothyroidism.   Eur J Endocrinol. 2005;153(6):747-753. doi:10.1530/eje.1.02025 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Jorde  R, Waterloo  K, Storhaug  H, Nyrnes  A, Sundsfjord  J, Jenssen  TG.  Neuropsychological function and symptoms in subjects with subclinical hypothyroidism and the effect of thyroxine treatment.   J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2006;91(1):145-153. doi:10.1210/jc.2005-1775 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Parle  J, Roberts  L, Wilson  S,  et al.  A randomized controlled trial of the effect of thyroxine replacement on cognitive function in community-living elderly subjects with subclinical hypothyroidism: the Birmingham Elderly Thyroid study.   J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2010;95(8):3623-3632. doi:10.1210/jc.2009-2571 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Stott  DJ, Rodondi  N, Kearney  PM,  et al; TRUST Study Group.  Thyroid hormone therapy for older adults with subclinical hypothyroidism.   N Engl J Med. 2017;376(26):2534-2544. doi:10.1056/NEJMoa1603825 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
Mooijaart  SP, Du Puy  RS, Stott  DJ,  et al.  Association between levothyroxine treatment and thyroid-related symptoms among adults aged 80 years and older with subclinical hypothyroidism.   JAMA. 2019;322(20):1977-1986. doi:10.1001/jama.2019.17274 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Aghili  R, Khamseh  ME, Malek  M,  et al.  Changes of subtests of Wechsler Memory Scale and cognitive function in subjects with subclinical hypothyroidism following treatment with levothyroxine.   Arch Med Sci. 2012;8(6):1096-1101. doi:10.5114/aoms.2012.32423 PubMedGoogle Scholar
13.
Feller  M, Snel  M, Moutzouri  E,  et al.  Association of thyroid hormone therapy with quality of life and thyroid-related symptoms in patients with subclinical hypothyroidism: a systematic review and meta-analysis.   JAMA. 2018;320(13):1349-1359. doi:10.1001/jama.2018.13770 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
Rieben  C, Segna  D, da Costa  BR,  et al.  Subclinical thyroid dysfunction and the risk of cognitive decline: a meta-analysis of prospective cohort studies.   J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2016;101(12):4945-4954. doi:10.1210/jc.2016-2129 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Akintola  AA, Jansen  SW, van Bodegom  D,  et al.  Subclinical hypothyroidism and cognitive function in people over 60 years: a systematic review and meta-analysis.   Front Aging Neurosci. 2015;7:150. doi:10.3389/fnagi.2015.00150 PubMedGoogle Scholar
16.
Pasqualetti  G, Pagano  G, Rengo  G, Ferrara  N, Monzani  F.  Subclinical hypothyroidism and cognitive impairment: systematic review and meta-analysis.   J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2015;100(11):4240-4248. doi:10.1210/jc.2015-2046 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
Wu  Y, Pei  Y, Wang  F, Xu  D, Cui  W.  Higher FT4 or TSH below the normal range are associated with increased risk of dementia: a meta-analysis of 11 studies.   Sci Rep. 2016;6:31975. doi:10.1038/srep31975 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
Rodondi  N, den Elzen  WP, Bauer  DC,  et al; Thyroid Studies Collaboration.  Subclinical hypothyroidism and the risk of coronary heart disease and mortality.   JAMA. 2010;304(12):1365-1374. doi:10.1001/jama.2010.1361 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
19.
Vaes  B, Pasquet  A, Wallemacq  P,  et al.  The BELFRAIL (BFC80+) study: a population-based prospective cohort study of the very elderly in Belgium.   BMC Geriatr. 2010;10:39. doi:10.1186/1471-2318-10-39 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Cappola  AR, Arnold  AM, Wulczyn  K, Carlson  M, Robbins  J, Psaty  BM.  Thyroid function in the euthyroid range and adverse outcomes in older adults.   J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2015;100(3):1088-1096. doi:10.1210/jc.2014-3586 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
Aubert  CE, Bauer  DC, da Costa  BR,  et al; Health ABC Study.  The association between subclinical thyroid dysfunction and dementia: the Health, Aging and Body Composition (Health ABC) Study.   Clin Endocrinol (Oxf). 2017;87(5):617-626. doi:10.1111/cen.13458 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
Yeap  BB, Alfonso  H, Chubb  SA,  et al.  Higher free thyroxine levels predict increased incidence of dementia in older men: the Health in Men Study.   J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2012;97(12):E2230-E2237. doi:10.1210/jc.2012-2108 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
23.
Bjoro  T, Holmen  J, Krüger  O,  et al; The Health Study of Nord-Trondelag (HUNT).  Prevalence of thyroid disease, thyroid dysfunction and thyroid peroxidase antibodies in a large, unselected population.   Eur J Endocrinol. 2000;143(5):639-647. doi:10.1530/eje.0.1430639 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Ceresini  G, Lauretani  F, Maggio  M,  et al.  Thyroid function abnormalities and cognitive impairment in elderly people: results of the Invecchiare in Chianti study.   J Am Geriatr Soc. 2009;57(1):89-93. doi:10.1111/j.1532-5415.2008.02080.x PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
Han  JW, Kim  TH, Kwak  KP,  et al.  Overview of the Korean longitudinal study on cognitive aging and dementia.   Psychiatry Investig. 2018;15(8):767-774. doi:10.30773/pi.2018.06.02 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
Moon  JH, Park  YJ, Kim  TH,  et al.  Lower-but-normal serum TSH level is associated with the development or progression of cognitive impairment in elderly: Korean Longitudinal Study on Health and Aging (KLoSHA).   J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2014;99(2):424-432. doi:10.1210/jc.2013-3385 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
27.
Gussekloo  J, van Exel  E, de Craen  AJ, Meinders  AE, Frölich  M, Westendorp  RG.  Thyroid status, disability and cognitive function, and survival in old age.   JAMA. 2004;292(21):2591-2599. doi:10.1001/jama.292.21.2591 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
28.
Waring  AC, Harrison  S, Samuels  MH,  et al; Osteoporotic Fractures in Men (MrOS) Study.  Thyroid function and mortality in older men: a prospective study.   J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2012;97(3):862-870. doi:10.1210/jc.2011-2684 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
29.
van Eersel  MEA, Visser  ST, Joosten  H, Gansevoort  RT, Slaets  JPJ, Izaks  GJ.  Pharmacological treatment of increased vascular risk and cognitive performance in middle-aged and old persons: six-year observational longitudinal study.   BMC Neurol. 2020;20(1):242. doi:10.1186/s12883-020-01822-0 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
30.
Wijsman  LW, de Craen  AJ, Trompet  S,  et al.  Subclinical thyroid dysfunction and cognitive decline in old age.   PLoS One. 2013;8(3):e59199. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0059199 PubMedGoogle Scholar
31.
Yamada  M, Kasagi  F, Mimori  Y, Miyachi  T, Ohshita  T, Sasaki  H.  Incidence of dementia among atomic-bomb survivors—Radiation Effects Research Foundation Adult Health Study.   J Neurol Sci. 2009;281(1-2):11-14. doi:10.1016/j.jns.2009.03.003 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
32.
Chaker  L, Wolters  FJ, Bos  D,  et al.  Thyroid function and the risk of dementia: the Rotterdam Study.   Neurology. 2016;87(16):1688-1695. doi:10.1212/WNL.0000000000003227 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
33.
Völzke  H, Alte  D, Dörr  M,  et al.  The association between subclinical hyperthyroidism and blood pressure in a population-based study.   J Hypertens. 2006;24(10):1947-1953. doi:10.1097/01.hjh.0000244942.57417.8e PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
34.
Roberts  LM, Pattison  H, Roalfe  A,  et al.  Is subclinical thyroid dysfunction in the elderly associated with depression or cognitive dysfunction?   Ann Intern Med. 2006;145(8):573-581. doi:10.7326/0003-4819-145-8-200610170-00006 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
35.
Hogervorst  E, Huppert  F, Matthews  FE, Brayne  C.  Thyroid function and cognitive decline in the MRC Cognitive Function and Ageing Study.   Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2008;33(7):1013-1022. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.05.008 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
36.
de Jongh  RT, Lips  P, van Schoor  NM,  et al.  Endogenous subclinical thyroid disorders, physical and cognitive function, depression, and mortality in older individuals.   Eur J Endocrinol. 2011;165(4):545-554. doi:10.1530/EJE-11-0430 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
37.
Schoenmaker  M, de Craen  AJ, de Meijer  PH,  et al.  Evidence of genetic enrichment for exceptional survival using a family approach: the Leiden Longevity Study.   Eur J Hum Genet. 2006;14(1):79-84. doi:10.1038/sj.ejhg.5201508 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
38.
Cárdenas-Ibarra  L, Solano-Velázquez  JA, Salinas-Martínez  R, Aspera-Ledezma  TD, Sifuentes-Martínez  MdelR, Villarreal-Pérez  JZ.  Cross-sectional observations of thyroid function in geriatric Mexican outpatients with and without dementia.   Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2008;46(2):173-180. doi:10.1016/j.archger.2007.03.009 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
39.
Manciet  G, Dartigues  JF, Decamps  A,  et al.  The PAQUID survey and correlates of subclinical hypothyroidism in elderly community residents in the southwest of France.   Age Ageing. 1995;24(3):235-241. doi:10.1093/ageing/24.3.235 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
40.
Zipf  G, Chiappa  M, Porter  KS, Ostchega  Y, Lewis  BG, Dostal  J.  National health and nutrition examination survey: plan and operations, 1999-2010.   Vital Health Stat 1. 2013;(56):1-37.PubMedGoogle Scholar
41.
Hollowell  JG, Staehling  NW, Flanders  WD,  et al.  Serum TSH, T(4), and thyroid antibodies in the United States population (1988 to 1994): National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES III).   J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2002;87(2):489-499. doi:10.1210/jcem.87.2.8182 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
42.
Folstein  MF, Folstein  SE, McHugh  PR.  “Mini-mental state”. a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician.   J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12(3):189-198. doi:10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
43.
Teng  EL, Chui  HC.  The Modified Mini-Mental State (3MS) examination.   J Clin Psychiatry. 1987;48(8):314-318.PubMedGoogle Scholar
44.
Choe  JY, Youn  JC, Park  JH,  et al.  The Severe Cognitive Impairment Rating Scale—an instrument for the assessment of cognition in moderate to severe dementia patients.   Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2008;25(4):321-328. doi:10.1159/000119124 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
45.
Andrews  JS, Desai  U, Kirson  NY, Zichlin  ML, Ball  DE, Matthews  BR.  Disease severity and minimal clinically important differences in clinical outcome assessments for Alzheimer’s disease clinical trials.   Alzheimers Dement (N Y). 2019;5:354-363. doi:10.1016/j.trci.2019.06.005PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
46.
Jaeger  J.  Digit Symbol Substitution Test: the case for sensitivity over specificity in neuropsychological testing.   J Clin Psychopharmacol. 2018;38(5):513-519. doi:10.1097/JCP.0000000000000941 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
47.
Tombaugh  TN.  Trail Making Test A and B: normative data stratified by age and education.   Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2004;19(2):203-214. doi:10.1016/S0887-6177(03)00039-8PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
48.
van der Elst  W, van Boxtel  MPJ, van Breukelen  GJP, Jolles  J.  The Letter Digit Substitution Test: normative data for 1,858 healthy participants aged 24-81 from the Maastricht Aging Study (MAAS): influence of age, education, and sex.   J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2006;28(6):998-1009. doi:10.1080/13803390591004428 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
49.
Royall  DR, Mahurin  RK, Gray  KF.  Bedside assessment of executive cognitive impairment: the executive interview.   J Am Geriatr Soc. 1992;40(12):1221-1226. doi:10.1111/j.1532-5415.1992.tb03646.x PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
50.
Ruff  RM, Light  RH, Evans  RW.  The Ruff Figural Fluency Test: a normative study with adults.   Dev Neuropsychol. 1987;3(1):37-51. doi:10.1080/87565648709540362 Google ScholarCrossref
51.
Benedict  RH, DeLuca  J, Phillips  G, LaRocca  N, Hudson  LD, Rudick  R; Multiple Sclerosis Outcome Assessments Consortium.  Validity of the Symbol Digit Modalities Test as a cognition performance outcome measure for multiple sclerosis.   Mult Scler. 2017;23(5):721-733. doi:10.1177/1352458517690821PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
52.
Rey  A.  L'examen psychologique dans les cas d'encéphalopathie traumatique. (les problems).  [The psychological examination in cases of traumatic encepholopathy. problems].  Archives de Psychologie. 1941;28:215-285.Google Scholar
53.
Brand  N, Jolles  J.  Learning and retrieval rate of words presented auditorily and visually.   J Gen Psychol. 1985;112(2):201-210. doi:10.1080/00221309.1985.9711004 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
54.
Elwood  RW.  The Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised: psychometric characteristics and clinical application.   Neuropsychol Rev. 1991;2(2):179-201. doi:10.1007/BF01109053PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
55.
Spaan  P, Bouma  JM. Visuele Associatie Test (VAT). In: Bouma  JM, Mulder  J, Lindeboom  J, Schmand  B, eds.  Handboek Neuropsychologische Diagnostiek. Pearson Assessment and Information B.V.;2012: 283-296.
56.
Creavin  ST, Wisniewski  S, Noel-Storr  AH,  et al.  Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) for the detection of dementia in clinically unevaluated people aged 65 and over in community and primary care populations.   Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2016;(1):CD011145. doi:10.1002/14651858.CD011145.pub2 PubMedGoogle Scholar
57.
Riley  RD, Lambert  PC, Abo-Zaid  G.  Meta-analysis of individual participant data: rationale, conduct, and reporting.   BMJ. 2010;340:c221. doi:10.1136/bmj.c221 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
58.
Viechtbauer  W.  Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package.   J Stat Softw. 2010;36(3):48. doi:10.18637/jss.v036.i03 Google ScholarCrossref
59.
George  KM, Lutsey  PL, Selvin  E, Palta  P, Windham  BG, Folsom  AR.  Association between thyroid dysfunction and incident dementia in the Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities Neurocognitive Study.   J Endocrinol Metab. 2019;9(4):82-89. doi:10.14740/jem588 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
60.
Folkestad  L, Brandt  F, Lillevang-Johansen  M, Brix  TH, Hegedüs  L.  Graves’ disease and toxic nodular goiter, aggravated by duration of hyperthyroidism, are associated with Alzheimer’s and vascular dementia: a registry-based long-term follow-up of two large cohorts.   Thyroid. 2020;30(5):672-680. doi:10.1089/thy.2019.0672 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
61.
Agarwal  R, Kushwaha  S, Chhillar  N, Kumar  A, Dubey  DK, Tripathi  CB.  A cross-sectional study on thyroid status in North Indian elderly outpatients with dementia.   Ann Indian Acad Neurol. 2013;16(3):333-337. doi:10.4103/0972-2327.116916 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
62.
Winkler  A, Weimar  C, Jöckel  KH,  et al.  Thyroid-stimulating hormone and mild cognitive impairment: results of the Heinz Nixdorf Recall Study.   J Alzheimers Dis. 2016;49(3):797-807. doi:10.3233/JAD-150561 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
63.
Chachamovitz  DS, Vigário  PdosS, Silva  SO,  et al.  Does low-normal serum TSH level adversely impact cognition in elderly adults and might methimazole therapy improve outcomes?   Endocr J. 2016;63(5):495-505. doi:10.1507/endocrj.EJ15-0458 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
64.
Wouters  HJCM, Slagter  SN, Muller Kobold  AC, van der Klauw  MM, Wolffenbuttel  BHR.  Epidemiology of thyroid disorders in the Lifelines Cohort Study (the Netherlands).   PLoS One. 2020;15(11):e0242795. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0242795 PubMedGoogle Scholar
65.
Chaker  L, Baumgartner  C, den Elzen  WP,  et al; Thyroid Studies Collaboration.  Thyroid function within the reference range and the risk of stroke: an individual participant data analysis.   J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2016;101(11):4270-4282. doi:10.1210/jc.2016-2255 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
66.
Bano  A, Chaker  L, Mattace-Raso  FUS,  et al.  Thyroid function and the risk of atherosclerotic cardiovascular morbidity and mortality: the Rotterdam Study.   Circ Res. 2017;121(12):1392-1400. doi:10.1161/CIRCRESAHA.117.311603 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
67.
Mooijaart  SP, Broekhuizen  K, Trompet  S,  et al.  Evidence-based medicine in older patients: how can we do better?   Neth J Med. 2015;73(5):211-218.PubMedGoogle Scholar
68.
Calamia  M, Markon  K, Tranel  D.  Scoring higher the second time around: meta-analyses of practice effects in neuropsychological assessment.   Clin Neuropsychol. 2012;26(4):543-570. doi:10.1080/13854046.2012.680913 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2 Comments for this article
EXPAND ALL
,Dementia Causes or Risk Factors
Ghanshyam Dwivedi, MD | Private Practice, Fayettteville, NC
Good study report. After doing this intense study of association between thyroid disorder and dementia, did you identify any other illness or disease that affected dementia that can be corrected, or any other modifiable cause?
CONFLICT OF INTEREST: None Reported
RE: Association of Thyroid Dysfunction With Cognitive Function
Tomoyuki Kawada, MD | Nippon Medical School
van Vliet et al. evaluated the associations of baseline thyroid dysfunction with cognitive function and dementia by using individual patient data meta-analysis (1). They categorized subjects into five groups; overt hyperthyroidism, subclinical hyperthyroidism, euthyroidism, subclinical hypothyroidism, and overt hypothyroidism, and thyroid dysfunction was not associated with global cognitive function, executive function, memory, or risk of dementia. This report concluded that screening for subclinical and overt thyroid dysfunction in the context of cognitive decline in older adults was not recommended. I have a comment about their study.

As the authors mentioned, inconsistent results have been reported on the association between
thyroid dysfunction and dementia. George et al. reported that the adjusted hazard ratios (95% confidence intervals [CIs]) of subclinical hypothyroidism and overt hyperthyroidism vs euthyroidism for dementia were 0.74 (0.60 - 0.92) and 1.40 (1.02 - 1.92), respectively (2). Rieben et all. conducted a meta-analysis and the pooled adjusted risk ratios (95% CI) of subclinical hyperthyroidism and subclinical hypothyroidism vs euthyroidism for dementia was 1.67 (1.04 - 2.69) and 1.14 (0.84 - 1.55), respectively (3). Although there is a need of further study to conclude that thyroid dysfunction can lead to an onset of dementia, hyperthyroidism may be a risk of dementia. Treatment status for thyroid dysfunction should be specified in patients with overt hyper- and hypothyroidism.

References
1. van Vliet NA, van Heemst D, Almeida OP, et al. Association of Thyroid Dysfunction With Cognitive Function: An Individual Participant Data Analysis. JAMA Intern Med. 2021 Sep 7. doi: 10.1001/jamainternmed.2021.5078

2. George KM, Lutsey PL, Selvin E, et al. Association Between Thyroid Dysfunction and Incident Dementia in the Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities Neurocognitive Study. J Endocrinol Metab 2019;9(4):82-89.

3. Rieben C, Segna D, da Costa BR, et al. Subclinical Thyroid Dysfunction and the Risk of Cognitive Decline: a Meta-Analysis of Prospective Cohort Studies. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 2016;101(12):4945-4954.

CONFLICT OF INTEREST: None Reported
READ MORE
Original Investigation
September 7, 2021

Association of Thyroid Dysfunction With Cognitive Function: An Individual Participant Data Analysis

Nicolien A. van Vliet, MD1; Diana van Heemst, PhD1; Osvaldo P. Almeida, MD, PhD2,3; et al Bjørn O. Åsvold, MD, PhD4,5,6; Carole E. Aubert, MD7,8,9,10; Jong Bin Bae, MD11; Linda E. Barnes, RGN12; Douglas C. Bauer, MD13; Gerard J. Blauw, MD, PhD1; Carol Brayne, MBBS, MD12; Anne R. Cappola, MD, ScM14; Graziano Ceresini, MD, PhD15; Hannie C. Comijs, PhD16,17; Jean-Francois Dartigues, MD, PhD18; Jean-Marie Degryse, MD, PhD19,20; Robin P. F. Dullaart, MD, PhD21; Marlise E. A. van Eersel, MD, PhD22; Wendy P. J. den Elzen, PhD23,24,25; Luigi Ferrucci, MD, PhD26,27; Howard A. Fink, MD, MPH28,29; Leon Flicker, MB, BS, PhD2,3; Hans J. Grabe, MD30,31; Ji Won Han, MD, PhD11; Catherine Helmer, MD, PhD18; Martijn Huisman, PhD32,33; M. Arfan Ikram, MD, PhD34; Misa Imaizumi, MD, PhD35; Renate T. de Jongh, MD, PhD36; J. Wouter Jukema, MD, PhD37,38; Ki Woong Kim, MD, PhD11,39,40; Lewis H. Kuller, MD, DrPH41; Oscar L. Lopez, MD42; Simon P. Mooijaart, MD, PhD1; Jae Hoon Moon, MD, PhD43; Elisavet Moutzouri, MD, PhD7,8; Matthias Nauck, MD44,45; Jim Parle, MBChB, MD46; Robin P. Peeters, MD, PhD47,48; Mary H. Samuels, MD49; Carsten O. Schmidt, MD, PhD50; Ulf Schminke, MD51; P. Eline Slagboom, PhD52,53; Eystein Stordal, MD, PhD54,55; Bert Vaes, MD, PhD19; Henry Völzke, MD56; Rudi G. J. Westendorp, MD, PhD1,57; Michiko Yamada, MD, PhD35; Bu B. Yeap, MBBS, PhD2,58; Nicolas Rodondi, MD, MAS7,8; Jacobijn Gussekloo, MD, PhD1,59; Stella Trompet, PhD1; for the Thyroid Studies Collaboration
Author Affiliations
  • 1Department of Internal Medicine, Section of Gerontology and Geriatrics, Leiden University Medical Center, Leiden, the Netherlands
  • 2Medical School, University of Western Australia, Perth, Western Australia, Australia
  • 3Western Australian Centre for Health and Ageing, University of Western Australia, Perth, Western Australia, Australia
  • 4K.G. Jebsen Center for Genetic Epidemiology, Department of Public Health and Nursing, NTNU, Norwegian University of Science and Technology, Trondheim, Norway
  • 5Department of Endocrinology, Clinic of Medicine, St Olav’s Hospital, Trondheim University Hospital, Trondheim, Norway
  • 6HUNT Research Center, Department of Public Health and Nursing, NTNU, Norwegian University of Science and Technology, Levanger, Norway
  • 7Department of General Internal Medicine, Inselspital, Bern University Hospital, University of Bern, Bern, Switzerland
  • 8Institute of Primary Health Care (BIHAM), University of Bern, Bern, Switzerland
  • 9Center for Clinical Management Research, Veterans Affairs Ann Arbor Healthcare System, Ann Arbor, Michigan
  • 10Institute for Healthcare Policy and Innovation, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor
  • 11Department of Neuropsychiatry, Seoul National University Bundang Hospital, Seongnam, South Korea
  • 12Department of Public Health and Primary Care, Cambridge Institute of Public Health, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, United Kingdom
  • 13Division of General Internal Medicine, School of Medicine, University of California, San Francisco
  • 14Division of Endocrinology, Diabetes, and Metabolism, Department of Medicine, Perelman School of Medicine at the University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia
  • 15Department of Medicine and Surgery, University of Parma, Unit of Internal Medicine and Oncological Endocrinology, University Hospital of Parma, Parma, Italy
  • 16Department of Psychiatry, Amsterdam Public Health research institute, Amsterdam UMC, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
  • 17GGZ inGeest Specialized Mental Health Care, Research and Innovation, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
  • 18UMR 1219, Bordeaux Population Health Research Center, Inserm, University of Bordeaux, Bordeaux, France
  • 19Department of Public Health and Primary Care, Katholieke Universiteit Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
  • 20Institute of Health and Society, Université catholique de Louvain, Brussels, Belgium
  • 21Division of Endocrinology, Department of Internal Medicine, University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Groningen, the Netherlands
  • 22University Center for Geriatric Medicine, University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Groningen, the Netherlands
  • 23Department of Clinical Chemistry and Laboratory Medicine, Leiden University Medical Center, Leiden, the Netherlands
  • 24Atalmedial Diagnostics Centre, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
  • 25Department of Clinical Chemistry, Amsterdam UMC, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
  • 26Longitudinal Studies Section, Translational Gerontology Branch, Harbor Hospital, Baltimore, Maryland
  • 27National Institute on Aging NIA-ASTRA Unit, Baltimore, Maryland
  • 28Geriatric Research Education and Clinical Center, VA Healthcare System, Minneapolis, Minnesota
  • 29Department of Medicine, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis
  • 30Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University Medicine Greifswald, Greifswald, Germany
  • 31German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (DZNE), Site Rostock/Greifswald, Germany
  • 32Department of Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Amsterdam Public Health Research Institute, Amsterdam UMC, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
  • 33Department of Sociology, VU University Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
  • 34Department of Epidemiology, Erasmus MC, Rotterdam, the Netherlands
  • 35Department of Clinical Studies, Radiation Effects Research Foundation, Hiroshima and Nagasaki, Japan
  • 36Department of Internal Medicine and Endocrinology, Amsterdam UMC, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
  • 37Department of Cardiology, Leiden University Medical Center, Leiden, the Netherlands
  • 38Netherlands Heart Institute, Utrecht, the Netherlands
  • 39Department of Brain and Cognitive Science, Seoul National University College of Natural Sciences, Seoul, South Korea
  • 40Department of Psychiatry, Seoul National University, College of Medicine, Seoul, South Korea
  • 41Department of Epidemiology, Graduate School of Public Health, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania
  • 42Department of Neurology, University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania
  • 43Department of Internal Medicine, Seoul National University Bundang Hospital, Seongnam, South Korea
  • 44Institute of Clinical Chemistry and Laboratory Medicine, University Medicine Greifswald, Greifswald, Germany
  • 45DZHK (German Centre for Cardiovascular Research), partner site, Greifswald, Germany
  • 46Institute of Clinical Sciences, University of Birmingham, Birmingham, United Kingdom
  • 47Department of Internal Medicine, Erasmus MC, Rotterdam, the Netherlands
  • 48Academic Center for Thyroid Diseases, Erasmus MC, Rotterdam, the Netherlands
  • 49Division of Endocrinology, Diabetes and Clinical Nutrition, Department of Medicine, Oregon Health & Science University, Portland
  • 50Department of Radiology, University Medicine Greifswald, Greifswald, Germany
  • 51Department of Neurology, University Medicine Greifswald, Greifswald, Germany
  • 52Department of Biomedical Data Sciences, Section of Molecular Epidemiology, Leiden University Medical Center, Leiden, the Netherlands
  • 53Max Planck Institute for Biology of Ageing, Cologne, Germany
  • 54Namsos Hospital, Nord-Trøndelag Hospital Trust, Namsos, Norway
  • 55Department of Mental Health, NTNU, Norwegian University of Science and Technology, Trondheim, Norway
  • 56Institute for Community Medicine, University Medicine Greifswald, Greifswald, Germany
  • 57Department of Public Health, Section of Epidemiology, Center for Healthy Aging, University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen, Denmark
  • 58Department of Endocrinology and Diabetes, Fiona Stanley Hospital, Western Australia, Australia
  • 59Department of Public Health and Primary Care, Leiden University Medical Center, Leiden, the Netherlands
JAMA Intern Med. 2021;181(11):1440-1450. doi:10.1001/jamainternmed.2021.5078
Key Points

Question  Is thyroid dysfunction associated with cognitive decline?

Findings  In this individual participant data analysis of 23 cohorts including 74 565 participants with cognitive function and/or dementia measurements, subclinical thyroid dysfunction was not associated with global cognitive function at baseline (standardized mean difference, −0.02 for subclinical hyperthyroidism and 0.05 for subclinical hypothyroidism) or annual decline (standardized mean difference, −0.02 for subclinical hyperthyroidism and −0.00 for subclinical hypothyroidism).

Meaning  These findings do not support the need for screening for subclinical thyroid dysfunction for prevention of cognitive decline or dementia.

Abstract

Importance  In clinical guidelines, overt and subclinical thyroid dysfunction are mentioned as causal and treatable factors for cognitive decline. However, the scientific literature on these associations shows inconsistent findings.

Objective  To assess cross-sectional and longitudinal associations of baseline thyroid dysfunction with cognitive function and dementia.

Design, Setting, and Participants  This multicohort individual participant data analysis assessed 114 267 person-years (median, 1.7-11.3 years) of follow-up for cognitive function and 525 222 person-years (median, 3.8-15.3 years) for dementia between 1989 and 2017. Analyses on cognitive function included 21 cohorts comprising 38 144 participants. Analyses on dementia included eight cohorts with a total of 2033 cases with dementia and 44 573 controls. Data analysis was performed from December 2016 to January 2021.

Exposures  Thyroid function was classified as overt hyperthyroidism, subclinical hyperthyroidism, euthyroidism, subclinical hypothyroidism, and overt hypothyroidism based on uniform thyrotropin cutoff values and study-specific free thyroxine values.

Main Outcomes and Measures  The primary outcome was global cognitive function, mostly measured using the Mini-Mental State Examination. Executive function, memory, and dementia were secondary outcomes. Analyses were first performed at study level using multivariable linear regression and multivariable Cox regression, respectively. The studies were combined with restricted maximum likelihood meta-analysis. To overcome the use of different scales, results were transformed to standardized mean differences. For incident dementia, hazard ratios were calculated.

Results  Among 74 565 total participants, 66 567 (89.3%) participants had normal thyroid function, 577 (0.8%) had overt hyperthyroidism, 2557 (3.4%) had subclinical hyperthyroidism, 4167 (5.6%) had subclinical hypothyroidism, and 697 (0.9%) had overt hypothyroidism. The study-specific median age at baseline varied from 57 to 93 years; 42 847 (57.5%) participants were women. Thyroid dysfunction was not associated with global cognitive function; the largest differences were observed between overt hypothyroidism and euthyroidism—cross-sectionally (−0.06 standardized mean difference in score; 95% CI, –0.20 to 0.08; P = .40) and longitudinally (0.11 standardized mean difference higher decline per year; 95% CI, –0.01 to 0.23; P = .09). No consistent associations were observed between thyroid dysfunction and executive function, memory, or risk of dementia.

Conclusions and Relevance  In this individual participant data analysis of more than 74 000 adults, subclinical hypothyroidism and hyperthyroidism were not associated with cognitive function, cognitive decline, or incident dementia. No rigorous conclusions can be drawn regarding the role of overt thyroid dysfunction in risk of dementia. These findings do not support the practice of screening for subclinical thyroid dysfunction in the context of cognitive decline in older adults as recommended in current guidelines.

Introduction

Thyroid dysfunction is considered a potentially reversible cause of cognitive decline; hence, thyroid function screening tests are described in guidelines as an essential component of the workup for the diagnosis of dementia.1-3 Thyroid dysfunction is frequently observed in individuals with suspected dementia.4 However, the outcomes of treatment of overt hypothyroidism and hyperthyroidism and subclinical hyperthyroidism on cognitive function are not fully clarified.5-7 For subclinical hypothyroidism, 4 of 5 recent randomized clinical trials and a meta-analysis on levothyroxine treatment did not find evidence for an improvement in cognitive function.8-13 Moreover, meta-analyses of observational studies have yielded inconsistent results on associations of subclinical and overt thyroid dysfunction with cognitive impairment and risk of dementia.14-17 An individual participant data analysis of cohort studies might help clarify the conflicting results of previous studies, as it allows for uniform definitions of thyroid dysfunction and it can assess the differential associations by age groups, sex, and thyroid medication in subgroup analyses.18 In the present study, we investigated cross-sectional and longitudinal associations of thyroid dysfunction with cognitive function and dementia in an individual participant data analysis of multiple cohorts.

Methods
Study Population

We first approached the coordinating center of the Thyroid Studies Collaboration, a collaborative project of 25 existing longitudinal studies with information on thyroid status.18 The Medical Ethics Committee of the Leiden University Medical Center waived the need for review owing to the retrospective nature of the study using only previously collected data; no individuals underwent interventions for the present study. Each participant gave informed consent to the original study they participated in, which was oral or written depending on the original study design and legislation at the time of data collection. All 15 Thyroid Studies Collaboration cohorts that had collected data on cognitive function or dementia joined the project. The study designs for all cohorts participating in the current study have been described previously in more detail.19-33 We approached 14 additional cohorts that were extracted from 4 recent meta-analyses on subclinical thyroid dysfunction and cognitive function or dementia.14-17 Six of these cohorts consented to collaborating and sharing data.34-39 Lastly, we included publicly available data of the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey waves of 1999 to 2002 and 2011 to 2012, which simultaneously collected thyroid and cognitive function among many other parameters.40

Thyroid Function

Thyroid dysfunction was determined biochemically by measurements of thyrotropin and free thyroxine (FT4) concentrations in all cohorts. Cohort-specific cutoff values were used for FT4 levels (eTable 1 in the Supplement). In accordance with previous projects in the Thyroid Studies Collaboration, participants were classified as euthyroid if thyrotropin level was 0.45 to 4.49 mIU/L.18 Overt hyperthyroidism was defined as a thyrotropin level less than 0.45 mIU/L and FT4 level above the reference range. Subclinical hyperthyroidism was defined as a thyrotropin level less than 0.45 mIU/L and FT4 levels within the reference range, or only as thyrotropin level less than 0.45 mIU/L in absence of an FT4 measurement (n = 896 among 10 cohorts) because overt hyperthyroidism is rare.41 A combination of thyrotropin level of 4.50 to 20 mIU/L and FT4 levels within the reference range was defined as subclinical hypothyroidism. Individuals who had missing FT4 measurements with mildly elevated thyrotropin levels (4.50-20 mIU/L) were considered subclinically hypothyroid (n = 523 among 8 cohorts) because chances of overt hypothyroidism in this patient category are low.41 A thyrotropin level of 20 mIU/L or greater or thyrotropin level of 4.50 mIU/L or greater combined with FT4 levels below the reference range was defined as overt hypothyroidism.

Cognitive Function

The primary outcome was global cognitive function measured by Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), Modified Mini-Mental State (3MS), or Severe Cognitive Impairment Rating Scale.42-44 A difference of 1 point in MMSE score is considered the minimal clinically important difference in individuals without dementia.45 Executive function and memory were secondary outcomes. For executive function, various tests were used: Digit Symbol Substitution Test, Trail Making Test B, Letter Digit Substitution Test (LDST), Executive Interview 15, and Ruff Figural Fluency Test.46-50 The minimal clinically important difference for executive function was defined as a difference of 4 points in LDST.51 Memory was measured using either Rey’s Auditory Verbal Learning Test (also referred to as Word Learning Test or Verbal Learning Test), Digit Span Test, or Visual Association Test.52-55 No minimal clinically important difference for memory tests was found in the literature.

Dementia

Depending on the study design, dementia was diagnosed either in a clinical setting or at a research center. The diagnosis was, at least in part, based on clinical presentation. Studies in which dementia diagnosis was based only on a cutoff point for the MMSE were excluded from this analysis because cognitive function tests are insufficient to diagnose dementia.56 Prevalence of dementia at baseline was available for 11 cohorts; 431 participants had a diagnosis of dementia at baseline, but only 78 of them were classified as noneuthyroid. Owing to the small number of participants with thyroid dysfunction at baseline, no cross-sectional analyses for dementia were performed.

Statistical Analyses

We used a 2-stage individual participant data analysis approach, which accommodates uniform definitions and analyses for each cohort while keeping complexity to a minimum.18,57 The first stage consisted of study-level analysis of thyroid dysfunction and cognitive function or dementia conducted on the original data sets with participant-level data. In the second stage, the effect estimates from the first stage were pooled using a random-effects model based on restricted maximum likelihood. Heterogeneity across studies was quantified using the I2 statistic: less than 40% was considered low heterogeneity; 40% to 75%, moderate heterogeneity; and greater than 75%, high heterogeneity.

For both the cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses between thyroid dysfunction and cognitive function, we used multivariable linear regression models. To facilitate combination of different scales, the results were transformed to standardized mean differences. In the prospective analysis of cognitive decline, we calculated the difference between the last available measurement of cognitive function and baseline cognitive function. The difference was divided by the follow-up time in years to obtain an annual decline, irrespective of duration of follow-up. The annual decline was subsequently standardized, resulting in a standardized mean difference in annual change in cognitive function allowing comparison of changes over time.

The risk of developing dementia during follow-up was assessed using Cox regression models. In these analyses, participants with dementia at baseline were excluded. For studies without precise registration of the date of dementia diagnosis, it was assumed that dementia developed halfway between the registration date and the last date that absence of dementia was ascertained.

Thyroid dysfunction (overt hyperthyroidism, subclinical hyperthyroidism, subclinical hypothyroidism, and overt hypothyroidism) was included as a categorical variable with the euthyroid group serving as reference. All analyses were adjusted for age and sex. The longitudinal analyses of cognitive decline were adjusted for baseline cognitive function. Prespecified subgroup analyses were performed by stratification and interaction analysis for sex and for age younger or older than 75 years. Additional analyses were performed with adjustment for educational attainment, though this variable was not available in all cohorts. In sensitivity analyses, participants with missing FT4 measurements in the subclinical hyperthyroid and subclinical hypothyroid groups were excluded, as were those who used antithyroid medication or thyroid hormone replacement therapy at baseline. Furthermore, we assessed robustness of the associations by pooling the estimates using fixed-effect models and by excluding studies with strata of fewer than 10 participants. To assess whether effects were dependent on degree of disruption of thyrotropin, analyses were repeated with thyrotropin categories of less than 0.10 mIU/L, 0.10 to 0.44 mIU/L, 4.5 to 6.9 mIU/L, 7.0 to 10 mIU/L, and greater than 10 mIU/L, in which participants with thyrotropin between 0.45 and 4.49 mIU/L served as reference. Lastly, instead of using biochemical cutoff points, thyrotropin and FT4 were analyzed continuously across the full range with cognitive function. Thyrotropin was transformed using the natural logarithm; for both natural log-transformed thyrotropin and FT4, models were constructed per standard deviation. Continuous models were performed minimally adjusted by age and sex and with additional adjustment for educational attainment. For sensitivity purposes, the analyses were also conducted excluding the participants who used antithyroid medication or thyroid hormone replacement therapy at baseline. Cohorts with greater than 10% missing measurements for FT4 were excluded for the continuous analyses on FT4. All P values were 2-tailed; statistical significance was set at P < .05.

Study-level analyses were performed using SPSS Statistics, version 25 (IBM). Effect estimates were pooled and summarized in forest plots using R, version 3.6.1 and metafor package (R Foundation for Statistical Computing).58 This study followed the Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) reporting guideline.

Results
Population Characteristics

Individual participant data on thyroid function and cognitive function and/or dementia were provided by 23 cohorts comprising 74 565 participants. At baseline, 66 567 (89.3%) participants were biochemically classified as euthyroid, 577 (0.8%) as overtly hyperthyroid, 2557 (3.4%) as subclinically hyperthyroid, 4167 (5.6%) as subclinically hypothyroid, and 697 (0.9%) as overtly hypothyroid (eTable 1 in the Supplement). The study-specific median age at baseline varied from 57 to 93 years; 42 847 (57.5%) participants were women.

A total of 38 144 participants from 21 cohorts provided data on a measure of cognitive function (Table). The median age varied from 58 to 93 years, and 18 089 (47.4%) participants were women. Follow-up for cognitive decline was available for 14 cohorts, with a median follow-up duration varying from 1.7 to 11.3 years, accumulating 114 267 person-years.

Eight cohorts provided follow-up for dementia incidence on 46 606 participants (eTable 2 in the Supplement). Among these participants, 28 820 (61.8%) were women, and the median age at baseline was between 57 and 85 years. During follow-up, 2033 (4.4%) cases of incident dementia were identified. Median follow-up duration ranged from 3.8 to 15.3 years, accumulating 525 222 person-years.

Thyroid Dysfunction and Global Cognitive Function

Cross-sectionally, thyroid dysfunction was not associated with global cognitive function among 18 cohorts (Figure 1; eFigure 1 in the Supplement). The largest observed difference was −0.06 standardized mean difference (95% CI, −0.20 to 0.08; P = .40) global cognitive function for overt hypothyroidism compared with euthyroidism, which could be interpreted as an approximately 0.1-point lower MMSE score based on the SD for the 2 largest cohorts included. No statistically significant association was observed between thyroid dysfunction at baseline and annual change in global cognitive function during follow-up among 13 cohorts (Figure 2). Participants with overt hypothyroidism had 0.11 standardized mean difference (95% CI, −0.01 to 0.23; P = .09) higher decline per year in global cognitive function than participants who were euthyroid, which translates to approximately 0.1 point on the MMSE scale faster decline per year based on the SD in the largest cohort for this analysis. Additional adjustment for educational attainment did not materially change the results (eFigure 2 in the Supplement). Stratification by age and sex did not show any differential effects for global cognitive function (eTable 3 in the Supplement). No statistically significant associations were found when individuals were categorized by severity of thyrotropin abnormality (eFigure 3 in the Supplement). Reanalyzing the data with a fixed-effects model or without strata with fewer than 10 participants did not yield different results (eTable 4 in the Supplement). Leaving out participants with missing FT4 measurements or those using antithyroid medication or thyroid hormone replacement therapy at baseline also did not change the results. A positive association was found between continuous thyrotropin and global cognition only when thyroid supplementation and antithyroid medication users were excluded (0.028 higher standardized mean difference per SD; 95% CI, 0.003 to 0.053; P = .03; eTable 5 in the Supplement). No association between continuous FT4 levels and global cognitive function was found. Heterogeneity across studies was low for the cross-sectional main analyses (I2 = 0%-40%), while heterogeneity was low to moderate for the longitudinal and sensitivity analyses (I2 = 0%-70%).

Thyroid Dysfunction and Executive Function and Memory

No negative association was observed cross-sectionally between thyroid dysfunction and executive function or memory among 11 and 8 cohorts, respectively (Figure 1; eFigures 4 and 5 in the Supplement). Participants with overt hyperthyroidism had 0.20 standardized mean difference (95% CI, 0.07 to 0.33; P = .002) higher executive function score compared with participants who were euthyroid; transformed, this would account for 1.6 more correct substitutions within 60 seconds for the LDST based on the largest cohort in this analysis. In both executive function and memory, participants with subclinical hypothyroidism performed better than participants who were euthyroid (executive function: 0.07 standardized mean difference; 95% CI, 0.01 to 0.13; P = .03; memory: 0.08 standardized mean difference; 95% CI, 0.01 to 0.15; P = .03). Longitudinally, no association was found between thyroid dysfunction at baseline and decline in executive function among 7 cohorts or memory among 6 cohorts; all differences were smaller than 0.1 standardized mean difference (Figure 2). Additional adjustment for educational attainment did not materially change the results (eFigure 2 in the Supplement). No statistically significant interaction with sex or age was present (P > .05 for all; supporting data in eTable 3 in the Supplement). Using a fixed-effects model or excluding strata with fewer than 10 participants did not change the results for executive function or memory (eTable 4 in the Supplement). The association of subclinical hypothyroidism and better executive function was attenuated when participants with missing FT4 measurements were left out, while the association with memory was unchanged. The positive association between overt hyperthyroidism and executive function disappeared when participants using thyroid medication were removed. No association was found when individuals were categorized by severity of thyrotropin abnormality or when thyrotropin level was analyzed continuously (eFigure 3 and eTable 5 in the Supplement). Continuous analysis of FT4 levels showed a positive association with executive function (0.019 higher standardized mean difference per SD; 95% CI, 0.002 to 0.036; P = .03), which was attenuated when participants using thyroid medication were left out. Heterogeneity across studies was low for the cross-sectional main analyses (I2 = 0%-40%), while heterogeneity was low to moderate for the longitudinal analyses (I2 = 0%-70%) and up to high heterogeneity in the sensitivity analyses (I2 ≤ 73%).

Thyroid Dysfunction and Dementia

Cross-sectional analysis of thyroid dysfunction and dementia were unfeasible owing to few participants who were not euthyroid with dementia at baseline (78 participants among 11 cohorts). In longitudinal analyses among 12 cohorts, no association was found between thyroid dysfunction and incident dementia (Figure 3; eFigure 6 in the Supplement). The hazard ratio of dementia ranged from 1.54 (95% CI, 0.76 to 3.10) for overt hyperthyroidism to 0.79 (95% CI, 0.48 to 1.28) for overt hypothyroidism. Continuous analysis of thyrotropin and FT4 levels also did not provide evidence for an association; hazard ratio, 0.96 per SD increase of natural log-transformed thyrotropin (95% CI, 0.91 to 1.02; P = .16); hazard ratio, 1.05 per SD increase of FT4 (95% CI, 0.98 to 1.13; P = .16). Heterogeneity between studies was low (I2 = 0%-40%).

Discussion

In this individual participant data analysis of 74 565 participants from 23 cohorts, there was no association between subclinical thyroid dysfunction and cognitive function, cognitive decline, or the onset of dementia. Owing to uncertainty of the results for overt hypothyroidism and hyperthyroidism, no rigorous conclusions can be drawn regarding the association between overt thyroid dysfunction and cognitive decline and dementia.

While prior study-level meta-analyses also reported no association between subclinical hypothyroidism and cognitive function, cognitive decline, or dementia, they were limited by heterogeneity in definitions of thyroid dysfunction and choices of covariates in the statistical models.14-17 Because we performed an individual participant data analysis, we could standardize definitions of thyroid function categories and of cognitive function and cognitive decline and standardize the statistical models. By addressing these limitations and reaching the same results, the present study provides the strongest observational evidence to date suggesting that subclinical hypothyroidism is not associated with cognitive function or cognitive decline.

Various studies and 2 meta-analyses did show an association between subclinical or overt hyperthyroidism or low thyrotropin level within the reference range and a higher risk of dementia.14,17,20,26,59-61 Although our findings for subclinical and overt hyperthyroidism and dementia did not reach statistical significance, they are directionally consistent with the literature. Despite combining 8 cohorts comprising more than 45 000 participants, the number of individuals with subclinical and overt hyperthyroidism and the number of individuals who developed dementia during follow-up are limited. Therefore, we cannot exclude a higher risk of dementia in individuals with hyperthyroidism. In addition, individuals with overt hyperthyroidism had a slightly higher rate of cognitive decline, though not statistically significant. Considering the existing literature and the other results in the present study, the observed cross-sectional association between overt hyperthyroidism and better executive function was most likely a chance finding. Moreover, the observed difference in executive function was less than half the minimal clinically important difference, making it a clinically insignificant finding regardless of the P value.

Higher vulnerability among subgroups has been proposed; younger adults and women might be more susceptible to cognitive dysregulation associated with thyroid dysfunction.16,62 Moreover, cognitive decline might only be present in individuals with more extreme values of thyrotropin,21,63 or variation in FT4 instead of thyrotropin levels could be associated with dementia risk.22 In the present multicohort study, we did not observe differential associations for participants younger and older than 75 years or for men and women, nor any association with variation in FT4 level or more extreme values of thyrotropin. Therefore, subgroup associations reported in prior studies might not be generalizable outside the original cohorts.

As mentioned before, all but 1 randomized clinical trial on levothyroxine treatment for subclinical hypothyroidism also did not provide evidence for improvement of cognitive function.8-12 Moreover, both undertreatment and overtreatment with levothyroxine are common, estimated at 27% and 14%, respectively.64 Overtreatment is associated with increased risk of atrial fibrillation and atherosclerosis65,66 and, via cerebrovascular damage, might be associated with increased risk of cognitive decline. Therefore, screening for subclinical thyroid dysfunction in older adults to prevent cognitive impairment and dementia does not appear to be effective.

The current individual participant data analysis has several strengths. The use of individual participant data from cohorts from all over the globe enhances generalization while allowing standardized definitions and relevant subgroup analyses. All but 5 of the included studies had a median age of 70 years or older, which is essential but often not the case in research concerning outcomes that are most relevant for older adults.67 The present study approached cognition comprehensively; we assessed multiple domains of cognitive function, cross-sectionally and longitudinally, and incidence of dementia.

Limitations

Some limitations need to be acknowledged. Thyroid function categorization was based on biochemical characteristics. For 20% to 30% of the participants who were categorized as subclinical hypothyroid or hyperthyroid, we could not confirm subclinical thyroid dysfunction owing to the absence of FT4 measurement. This may have led to some misclassification, yet sensitivity analyses excluding those participants with missing FT4 data yielded similar results. We could not include educational attainment in our main analysis because 5 out of 18 cohorts did not collect these data. Even though the sensitivity analyses with adjustment for educational attainment yielded similar results as the main analysis, education is a possible confounder that could not be accounted for. For most cohorts, only 1 measurement of thyroid function was available, which is why only baseline thyroid function was used in the present individual participant data analysis. This study could therefore not capture any changes in cognitive function that might occur at the transition of one thyroid status to another. Moreover, for the vast majority of study participants, a maximum of 2 measurements of cognitive function was available, which precluded advanced modeling of change over time including nonlinear trajectories. In addition, the interpretation of longitudinal studies of cognitive function can be complicated by practice effects.68 Standardization of change over time might not fully alleviate this; hence, residual practice effects may still be present. Furthermore, because dementia is clinically difficult to diagnose, some misclassification could have occurred, which may have led to an underestimation of the association. In addition, the number of incident dementia cases in the included cohort studies was low; we therefore cannot rule out a clinically relevant association between thyroid dysfunction and risk of dementia. The heterogeneity between studies may have been increased by the use of different cognitive function tests, different durations of follow-up, differences in age and sex distribution, different lifestyles across continents, and different inclusion criteria. As heterogeneity was expected a priori, we performed all meta-analyses with random effects. Nonetheless, results for fixed-effects meta-analyses were not materially different. The observed heterogeneity was larger in the longitudinal analyses heterogeneity (I2 = 0%-70%) than in the cross-sectional analyses (I2 = 0%-40%), likely owing to the additional variation of follow-up duration. We hypothesize that the minor differences in I2 estimates between different cross-sectional analyses are attributable to differences in sample size per exposure. Because individuals with thyroid disease generally receive medical treatment, we cannot address the question of whether long-term untreated hyperthyroidism or hypothyroidism is associated with cognitive function and dementia risk. Moreover, these results only apply to objectifiable cognitive decline, which is not synonymous with the more subjective cognitive complaints.

Conclusions

In this individual participant data analysis combining the individual participant data of 74 565 participants from 23 cohorts, subclinical thyroid dysfunction was not associated with cognitive function, cognitive decline, or risk of dementia. Hence, it is unlikely that treatment for otherwise undetected subclinical thyroid dysfunction would improve cognitive function. Moreover, the chance of overtreatment is considerable, which increases the risk of atrial fibrillation, atherosclerosis, and cerebral infarction and thereby might increase the risk of cognitive decline. Whether treatment of overt hypothyroidism or hyperthyroidism is associated with cognitive decline and risk of dementia remains uncertain. Existing clinical guidelines that prescribe screening of subclinical thyroid dysfunction for prevention of cognitive decline or dementia should therefore be revisited.

Back to top
Article Information

Accepted for Publication: July 15, 2021.

Published Online: September 7, 2021. doi:10.1001/jamainternmed.2021.5078

Open Access: This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the CC-BY License. © 2021 van Vliet NA et al. JAMA Internal Medicine.

Corresponding Author: Nicolien A. van Vliet, MD, Department of Internal Medicine, Section of Gerontology and Geriatrics, Leiden University Medical Center, PO Box 9600, 2300 RC Leiden, the Netherlands (n.a.van_vliet@lumc.nl).

Author Contributions: Drs van Vliet and Trompet had full access to all of the data in the study and take responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.

Concept and design: van Vliet, Blauw, Comijs, Dartigues, den Elzen, Imaizumi, Mooijaart, Stordal, Westendorp, Gussekloo, Trompet.

Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: van Vliet, van Heemst, Almeida, Åsvold, Aubert, Bae, Barnes, Bauer, Brayne, Cappola, Ceresini, Dartigues, Degryse, Dullaart, van Eersel, den Elzen, Ferrucci, Fink, Flicker, Grabe, Han, Helmer, Huisman, Ikram, Imaizumi, de Jongh, Jukema, Kim, Kuller, Lopez, Mooijaart, Moon, Moutzouri, Nauck, Parle, Peeters, Samuels, Schmidt, Schminke, Slagboom, Vaes, Völzke, Westendorp, Yamada, Yeap, Rodondi, Trompet.

Drafting of the manuscript: van Vliet, Barnes, Comijs, Dullaart, Mooijaart.

Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: van Vliet, van Heemst, Almeida, Åsvold, Aubert, Bae, Bauer, Blauw, Brayne, Cappola, Ceresini, Dartigues, Degryse, van Eersel, den Elzen, Ferrucci, Fink, Flicker, Grabe, Han, Helmer, Huisman, Ikram, Imaizumi, de Jongh, Jukema, Kim, Kuller, Lopez, Mooijaart, Moon, Moutzouri, Nauck, Parle, Peeters, Samuels, Schmidt, Schminke, Slagboom, Stordal, Vaes, Völzke, Westendorp, Yamada, Yeap, Rodondi, Gussekloo, Trompet.

Statistical analysis: van Vliet, Gussekloo.

Obtained funding: van Heemst, Almeida, Barnes, Comijs, Dartigues, Degryse, Dullaart, Flicker, Grabe, Helmer, Ikram, Kim, Mooijaart, Slagboom, Völzke, Yeap, Rodondi.

Administrative, technical, or material support: Bae, Barnes, Ceresini, Comijs, Dartigues, Dullaart, den Elzen, Flicker, Han, Kim, Moon, Moutzouri, Nauck, Peeters, Slagboom, Stordal, Vaes, Völzke, Yamada, Yeap, Rodondi.

Supervision: van Heemst, Blauw, van Eersel, Grabe, Ikram, Jukema, Kim, Lopez, Mooijaart, Westendorp, Gussekloo, Trompet.

Conflict of Interest Disclosures: Dr van Vliet reported grants from European Commission Horizon 2020 program during the conduct of the study. Dr van Heemst reported grants from European Commission during the conduct of the study; grants from Velux Stiftung (grant No. 1156) outside the submitted work. Dr Almeida reported grants from National Health and Medical Research Council of Australia during the conduct of the study and outside the submitted work. Dr Brayne reported grants from Medical Research Council during the conduct of the study. Dr Degryse reported grants from Fondation Louvain during the conduct of the study. Dr Flicker reported grants to the University of Western Australia from Australian National Health and Medical Research Council during the conduct of the study. Dr Grabe has received travel grants and speakers honoraria from Fresenius Medical Care, Neuraxpharm, Servier, and Janssen Cilag as well as research funding from Fresenius Medical Care outside the submitted work. Dr Huisman reported grants from Ministry of Health, Welfare and Sport for setting up and maintaining a prospective cohort study, of which data were used during the conduct of the study. Dr Lopez reported personal fees for consulting from Biogen and Grifols outside the submitted work. Dr Mooijaart reported nonfinancial support (provision of medication free of charge) from Merck during the conduct of the study. Dr Nauck reported grants from Federal Ministry of Education and Research, Germany, European Union Interreg IVa grants No. 01ZZ9603, 01ZZ0103, 01ZZ0403, 81Z7400171, 81Z7400173, INT-10-0008; personal fees for advisory boards and presentations from Radiometer, Becton Dickinson, AstraZeneca, Technopath Clinical Diagnostics, Novartis, Sysmex, Tosoh, medpoint Medizinkommunikations GmbH, ProteinT, and MDI Limbach; and travel/accommodations expenses covered or reimbursed from German Medical Association (BÄK), German Centre for Cardiovascular Research (GCCR), National Cohort, German Society for Clinical Chemistry and Laboratory Medicine e.V. (DGKL), ISBER, DAkkS, Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG), European Association for the Study of Diabetes (EASD), and Max Rubner-Institut outside the submitted work. Dr Yeap reported grants from National Health and Medical Research Council of Australia during the conduct of the study. Dr Rodondi reported grants from Swiss National Science Foundation during the conduct of the study. No other disclosures were reported.

Funding/Support: This work was supported by the European Commission project THYRAGE (Horizon 2020 research and innovation program, 666869). The Thyroid Studies Collaboration is funded by a grant from the Swiss National Science Foundation (SNSF 320030-172676 to Dr Rodondi). This research was supported by contracts HHSN268201200036C, HHSN268200800007C, HHSN268201800001C, N01HC55222, N01HC85079, N01HC85080, N01HC85081, N01HC85082, N01HC85083, N01HC85086, 75N92021D00006, and grants U01HL080295 and U01HL130114 from the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute, with additional contribution from the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke. Additional support was provided by R01AG023629 from the National Institute on Aging. Dr Cappola received additional support from R01AG027058, Dr Kuller from R01AG15928, and Dr Lopez from R01AG20098. A full list of principal Cardiovascular Health Study investigators and institutions can be found at https://chs-nhlbi.org/. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health (NIH). This research was supported by National Institute on Aging contracts N01-AG-6-2101; N01-AG-6-2103; N01-AG-6-2106; National Institute on Aging grant R01-AG028050, and National Institute of Nursing Research grant R01-NR012459. This research was funded in part by the Intramural Research Program of the NIH National Institute on Aging. The Trøndelag Health Study (The HUNT Study) is a collaboration between HUNT Research Centre (Faculty of Medicine and Health Sciences, NTNU, Norwegian University of Science and Technology), Trøndelag County Council, Central Norway Regional Health Authority, and the Norwegian Institute of Public Health. Information on incident dementia diagnoses for the HUNT study population was provided by Nord-Trøndelag Hospital Trust. The InCHIANTI study was supported as a “targeted project” (ICS110.1/RF97.71) by the Italian Ministry of Health and in part by the US National Institute on Aging (contracts: 263 MD 9164 and 263 MD 821336), supported in part by the Intramural Research Program of the NIH National Institute on Aging, Baltimore, Maryland. This study was supported by a grant from the Korean Health Technology R&D Project, Ministry of Health and Welfare, Republic of Korea (grant no. HI09C1379 [A092077]). The Longitudinal Aging Study Amsterdam is supported by a grant from the Netherlands Ministry of Health Welfare and Sports, Directorate of Long-Term Care. The Osteoporotic Fractures in Men Study is supported by NIH funding. The following institutes provide support: the National Institute on Aging, the National Institute of Arthritis and Musculoskeletal and Skin Diseases, the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences, and NIH Roadmap for Medical Research under the following grant numbers: U01 AG027810, U01 AG042124, U01 AG042139, U01 AG042140, U01 AG042143, U01 AG042145, U01 AG042168, U01 AR066160, and UL1 TR000128. MHS is supported by NIH NCATS UL1TR002369, The Oregon Clinical and Translational Institute. The PREVEND study was funded by the Dutch Kidney Foundation (https://nierstichting.nl/; grant E.033). This study was partly funded by the Dutch Alzheimer Foundation (https://www.alzheimer-nederland.nl/). The Radiation Effects Research Foundation (RERF), Hiroshima and Nagasaki, Japan is a public interest foundation funded by the Japanese Ministry of Health, Labour and Welfare and the US Department of Energy. This publication was supported by RERF Research Protocol RP-A2-16. The views of the authors do not necessarily reflect those of the 2 governments. SHIP (Study of Health in Pomerania) is part of the Community Medicine Research Network of the University Medicine Greifswald, which is supported by the German Federal State of Mecklenburg-West Pomerania.

Role of the Funder/Sponsor: The funders had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

Participating Studies of the Thyroid Studies Collaboration: United States: Cardiovascular Health Study; Health, Aging, and Body Composition Study; and the Osteoporotic Fractures in Men (MrOS) Study. Norway: Nord-Trøndelag Health Study (HUNT Study). The Netherlands: Leiden 85-Plus Study; Prevention of Renal and Vascular End-stage Disease Study (PREVEND Study); and the Rotterdam Study. The Netherlands/Ireland/Scotland: Prospective Study of Pravastatin in the Elderly at Risk (PROSPER Study). Italy: Invecchiare in Chianti (InCHIANTI) Study. Belgium: BELFRAIL Study. Germany: Study of Health in Pomerania (SHIP). Australia: Health in Men Study. Japan: Nagasaki Adult Health Study (Radiation Effects Research Foundation, RERF). Republic of Korea: Korean Longitudinal Study on Cognitive Aging and Dementia KLoSCAD); and Korean Longitudinal Study on Health and Aging (KLoSHA).

Additional Contributions: We thank all study participants for making this study possible. We thank Lilia Cárdenas-Ibarra, MD, for providing data from the Mexican Memory Clinic; she was compensated for this contribution.

References
1.
Knopman  DS, DeKosky  ST, Cummings  JL,  et al.  Practice parameter: diagnosis of dementia (an evidence-based review). report of the Quality Standards Subcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology.   Neurology. 2001;56(9):1143-1153. doi:10.1212/WNL.56.9.1143 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
Sorbi  S, Hort  J, Erkinjuntti  T,  et al; EFNS Scientist Panel on Dementia and Cognitive Neurology.  EFNS-ENS Guidelines on the diagnosis and management of disorders associated with dementia.   Eur J Neurol. 2012;19(9):1159-1179. doi:10.1111/j.1468-1331.2012.03784.xPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
Cognitive Decline Partnership Centre. Clinical practice guidelines and principles for people with dementia. Published February 2016. Accessed August 2, 2021. https://cdpc.sydney.edu.au/research/clinical-guidelines-for-dementia/
4.
Muangpaisan  W, Petcharat  C, Srinonprasert  V.  Prevalence of potentially reversible conditions in dementia and mild cognitive impairment in a geriatric clinic.   Geriatr Gerontol Int. 2012;12(1):59-64. doi:10.1111/j.1447-0594.2011.00728.x PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Ritchie  M, Yeap  BB.  Thyroid hormone: influences on mood and cognition in adults.   Maturitas. 2015;81(2):266-275. doi:10.1016/j.maturitas.2015.03.016 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Vogel  A, Elberling  TV, Hørding  M,  et al.  Affective symptoms and cognitive functions in the acute phase of Graves’ thyrotoxicosis.   Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2007;32(1):36-43. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2006.09.012 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Wekking  EM, Appelhof  BC, Fliers  E,  et al.  Cognitive functioning and well-being in euthyroid patients on thyroxine replacement therapy for primary hypothyroidism.   Eur J Endocrinol. 2005;153(6):747-753. doi:10.1530/eje.1.02025 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Jorde  R, Waterloo  K, Storhaug  H, Nyrnes  A, Sundsfjord  J, Jenssen  TG.  Neuropsychological function and symptoms in subjects with subclinical hypothyroidism and the effect of thyroxine treatment.   J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2006;91(1):145-153. doi:10.1210/jc.2005-1775 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Parle  J, Roberts  L, Wilson  S,  et al.  A randomized controlled trial of the effect of thyroxine replacement on cognitive function in community-living elderly subjects with subclinical hypothyroidism: the Birmingham Elderly Thyroid study.   J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2010;95(8):3623-3632. doi:10.1210/jc.2009-2571 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Stott  DJ, Rodondi  N, Kearney  PM,  et al; TRUST Study Group.  Thyroid hormone therapy for older adults with subclinical hypothyroidism.   N Engl J Med. 2017;376(26):2534-2544. doi:10.1056/NEJMoa1603825 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
Mooijaart  SP, Du Puy  RS, Stott  DJ,  et al.  Association between levothyroxine treatment and thyroid-related symptoms among adults aged 80 years and older with subclinical hypothyroidism.   JAMA. 2019;322(20):1977-1986. doi:10.1001/jama.2019.17274 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Aghili  R, Khamseh  ME, Malek  M,  et al.  Changes of subtests of Wechsler Memory Scale and cognitive function in subjects with subclinical hypothyroidism following treatment with levothyroxine.   Arch Med Sci. 2012;8(6):1096-1101. doi:10.5114/aoms.2012.32423 PubMedGoogle Scholar
13.
Feller  M, Snel  M, Moutzouri  E,  et al.  Association of thyroid hormone therapy with quality of life and thyroid-related symptoms in patients with subclinical hypothyroidism: a systematic review and meta-analysis.   JAMA. 2018;320(13):1349-1359. doi:10.1001/jama.2018.13770 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
Rieben  C, Segna  D, da Costa  BR,  et al.  Subclinical thyroid dysfunction and the risk of cognitive decline: a meta-analysis of prospective cohort studies.   J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2016;101(12):4945-4954. doi:10.1210/jc.2016-2129 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Akintola  AA, Jansen  SW, van Bodegom  D,  et al.  Subclinical hypothyroidism and cognitive function in people over 60 years: a systematic review and meta-analysis.   Front Aging Neurosci. 2015;7:150. doi:10.3389/fnagi.2015.00150 PubMedGoogle Scholar
16.
Pasqualetti  G, Pagano  G, Rengo  G, Ferrara  N, Monzani  F.  Subclinical hypothyroidism and cognitive impairment: systematic review and meta-analysis.   J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2015;100(11):4240-4248. doi:10.1210/jc.2015-2046 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
Wu  Y, Pei  Y, Wang  F, Xu  D, Cui  W.  Higher FT4 or TSH below the normal range are associated with increased risk of dementia: a meta-analysis of 11 studies.   Sci Rep. 2016;6:31975. doi:10.1038/srep31975 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
Rodondi  N, den Elzen  WP, Bauer  DC,  et al; Thyroid Studies Collaboration.  Subclinical hypothyroidism and the risk of coronary heart disease and mortality.   JAMA. 2010;304(12):1365-1374. doi:10.1001/jama.2010.1361 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
19.
Vaes  B, Pasquet  A, Wallemacq  P,  et al.  The BELFRAIL (BFC80+) study: a population-based prospective cohort study of the very elderly in Belgium.   BMC Geriatr. 2010;10:39. doi:10.1186/1471-2318-10-39 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Cappola  AR, Arnold  AM, Wulczyn  K, Carlson  M, Robbins  J, Psaty  BM.  Thyroid function in the euthyroid range and adverse outcomes in older adults.   J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2015;100(3):1088-1096. doi:10.1210/jc.2014-3586 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
Aubert  CE, Bauer  DC, da Costa  BR,  et al; Health ABC Study.  The association between subclinical thyroid dysfunction and dementia: the Health, Aging and Body Composition (Health ABC) Study.   Clin Endocrinol (Oxf). 2017;87(5):617-626. doi:10.1111/cen.13458 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
Yeap  BB, Alfonso  H, Chubb  SA,  et al.  Higher free thyroxine levels predict increased incidence of dementia in older men: the Health in Men Study.   J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2012;97(12):E2230-E2237. doi:10.1210/jc.2012-2108 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
23.
Bjoro  T, Holmen  J, Krüger  O,  et al; The Health Study of Nord-Trondelag (HUNT).  Prevalence of thyroid disease, thyroid dysfunction and thyroid peroxidase antibodies in a large, unselected population.   Eur J Endocrinol. 2000;143(5):639-647. doi:10.1530/eje.0.1430639 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Ceresini  G, Lauretani  F, Maggio  M,  et al.  Thyroid function abnormalities and cognitive impairment in elderly people: results of the Invecchiare in Chianti study.   J Am Geriatr Soc. 2009;57(1):89-93. doi:10.1111/j.1532-5415.2008.02080.x PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
Han  JW, Kim  TH, Kwak  KP,  et al.  Overview of the Korean longitudinal study on cognitive aging and dementia.   Psychiatry Investig. 2018;15(8):767-774. doi:10.30773/pi.2018.06.02 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
Moon  JH, Park  YJ, Kim  TH,  et al.  Lower-but-normal serum TSH level is associated with the development or progression of cognitive impairment in elderly: Korean Longitudinal Study on Health and Aging (KLoSHA).   J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2014;99(2):424-432. doi:10.1210/jc.2013-3385 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
27.
Gussekloo  J, van Exel  E, de Craen  AJ, Meinders  AE, Frölich  M, Westendorp  RG.  Thyroid status, disability and cognitive function, and survival in old age.   JAMA. 2004;292(21):2591-2599. doi:10.1001/jama.292.21.2591 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
28.
Waring  AC, Harrison  S, Samuels  MH,  et al; Osteoporotic Fractures in Men (MrOS) Study.  Thyroid function and mortality in older men: a prospective study.   J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2012;97(3):862-870. doi:10.1210/jc.2011-2684 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
29.
van Eersel  MEA, Visser  ST, Joosten  H, Gansevoort  RT, Slaets  JPJ, Izaks  GJ.  Pharmacological treatment of increased vascular risk and cognitive performance in middle-aged and old persons: six-year observational longitudinal study.   BMC Neurol. 2020;20(1):242. doi:10.1186/s12883-020-01822-0 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
30.
Wijsman  LW, de Craen  AJ, Trompet  S,  et al.  Subclinical thyroid dysfunction and cognitive decline in old age.   PLoS One. 2013;8(3):e59199. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0059199 PubMedGoogle Scholar
31.
Yamada  M, Kasagi  F, Mimori  Y, Miyachi  T, Ohshita  T, Sasaki  H.  Incidence of dementia among atomic-bomb survivors—Radiation Effects Research Foundation Adult Health Study.   J Neurol Sci. 2009;281(1-2):11-14. doi:10.1016/j.jns.2009.03.003 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
32.
Chaker  L, Wolters  FJ, Bos  D,  et al.  Thyroid function and the risk of dementia: the Rotterdam Study.   Neurology. 2016;87(16):1688-1695. doi:10.1212/WNL.0000000000003227 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
33.
Völzke  H, Alte  D, Dörr  M,  et al.  The association between subclinical hyperthyroidism and blood pressure in a population-based study.   J Hypertens. 2006;24(10):1947-1953. doi:10.1097/01.hjh.0000244942.57417.8e PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
34.
Roberts  LM, Pattison  H, Roalfe  A,  et al.  Is subclinical thyroid dysfunction in the elderly associated with depression or cognitive dysfunction?   Ann Intern Med. 2006;145(8):573-581. doi:10.7326/0003-4819-145-8-200610170-00006 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
35.
Hogervorst  E, Huppert  F, Matthews  FE, Brayne  C.  Thyroid function and cognitive decline in the MRC Cognitive Function and Ageing Study.   Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2008;33(7):1013-1022. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.05.008 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
36.
de Jongh  RT, Lips  P, van Schoor  NM,  et al.  Endogenous subclinical thyroid disorders, physical and cognitive function, depression, and mortality in older individuals.   Eur J Endocrinol. 2011;165(4):545-554. doi:10.1530/EJE-11-0430 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
37.
Schoenmaker  M, de Craen  AJ, de Meijer  PH,  et al.  Evidence of genetic enrichment for exceptional survival using a family approach: the Leiden Longevity Study.   Eur J Hum Genet. 2006;14(1):79-84. doi:10.1038/sj.ejhg.5201508 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
38.
Cárdenas-Ibarra  L, Solano-Velázquez  JA, Salinas-Martínez  R, Aspera-Ledezma  TD, Sifuentes-Martínez  MdelR, Villarreal-Pérez  JZ.  Cross-sectional observations of thyroid function in geriatric Mexican outpatients with and without dementia.   Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2008;46(2):173-180. doi:10.1016/j.archger.2007.03.009 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
39.
Manciet  G, Dartigues  JF, Decamps  A,  et al.  The PAQUID survey and correlates of subclinical hypothyroidism in elderly community residents in the southwest of France.   Age Ageing. 1995;24(3):235-241. doi:10.1093/ageing/24.3.235 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
40.
Zipf  G, Chiappa  M, Porter  KS, Ostchega  Y, Lewis  BG, Dostal  J.  National health and nutrition examination survey: plan and operations, 1999-2010.   Vital Health Stat 1. 2013;(56):1-37.PubMedGoogle Scholar
41.
Hollowell  JG, Staehling  NW, Flanders  WD,  et al.  Serum TSH, T(4), and thyroid antibodies in the United States population (1988 to 1994): National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES III).   J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2002;87(2):489-499. doi:10.1210/jcem.87.2.8182 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
42.
Folstein  MF, Folstein  SE, McHugh  PR.  “Mini-mental state”. a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician.   J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12(3):189-198. doi:10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
43.
Teng  EL, Chui  HC.  The Modified Mini-Mental State (3MS) examination.   J Clin Psychiatry. 1987;48(8):314-318.PubMedGoogle Scholar
44.
Choe  JY, Youn  JC, Park  JH,  et al.  The Severe Cognitive Impairment Rating Scale—an instrument for the assessment of cognition in moderate to severe dementia patients.   Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2008;25(4):321-328. doi:10.1159/000119124 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
45.
Andrews  JS, Desai  U, Kirson  NY, Zichlin  ML, Ball  DE, Matthews  BR.  Disease severity and minimal clinically important differences in clinical outcome assessments for Alzheimer’s disease clinical trials.   Alzheimers Dement (N Y). 2019;5:354-363. doi:10.1016/j.trci.2019.06.005PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
46.
Jaeger  J.  Digit Symbol Substitution Test: the case for sensitivity over specificity in neuropsychological testing.   J Clin Psychopharmacol. 2018;38(5):513-519. doi:10.1097/JCP.0000000000000941 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
47.
Tombaugh  TN.  Trail Making Test A and B: normative data stratified by age and education.   Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2004;19(2):203-214. doi:10.1016/S0887-6177(03)00039-8PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
48.
van der Elst  W, van Boxtel  MPJ, van Breukelen  GJP, Jolles  J.  The Letter Digit Substitution Test: normative data for 1,858 healthy participants aged 24-81 from the Maastricht Aging Study (MAAS): influence of age, education, and sex.   J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2006;28(6):998-1009. doi:10.1080/13803390591004428 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
49.
Royall  DR, Mahurin  RK, Gray  KF.  Bedside assessment of executive cognitive impairment: the executive interview.   J Am Geriatr Soc. 1992;40(12):1221-1226. doi:10.1111/j.1532-5415.1992.tb03646.x PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
50.
Ruff  RM, Light  RH, Evans  RW.  The Ruff Figural Fluency Test: a normative study with adults.   Dev Neuropsychol. 1987;3(1):37-51. doi:10.1080/87565648709540362 Google ScholarCrossref
51.
Benedict  RH, DeLuca  J, Phillips  G, LaRocca  N, Hudson  LD, Rudick  R; Multiple Sclerosis Outcome Assessments Consortium.  Validity of the Symbol Digit Modalities Test as a cognition performance outcome measure for multiple sclerosis.   Mult Scler. 2017;23(5):721-733. doi:10.1177/1352458517690821PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
52.
Rey  A.  L'examen psychologique dans les cas d'encéphalopathie traumatique. (les problems).  [The psychological examination in cases of traumatic encepholopathy. problems].  Archives de Psychologie. 1941;28:215-285.Google Scholar
53.
Brand  N, Jolles  J.  Learning and retrieval rate of words presented auditorily and visually.   J Gen Psychol. 1985;112(2):201-210. doi:10.1080/00221309.1985.9711004 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
54.
Elwood  RW.  The Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised: psychometric characteristics and clinical application.   Neuropsychol Rev. 1991;2(2):179-201. doi:10.1007/BF01109053PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
55.
Spaan  P, Bouma  JM. Visuele Associatie Test (VAT). In: Bouma  JM, Mulder  J, Lindeboom  J, Schmand  B, eds.  Handboek Neuropsychologische Diagnostiek. Pearson Assessment and Information B.V.;2012: 283-296.
56.
Creavin  ST, Wisniewski  S, Noel-Storr  AH,  et al.  Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) for the detection of dementia in clinically unevaluated people aged 65 and over in community and primary care populations.   Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2016;(1):CD011145. doi:10.1002/14651858.CD011145.pub2 PubMedGoogle Scholar
57.
Riley  RD, Lambert  PC, Abo-Zaid  G.  Meta-analysis of individual participant data: rationale, conduct, and reporting.   BMJ. 2010;340:c221. doi:10.1136/bmj.c221 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
58.
Viechtbauer  W.  Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package.   J Stat Softw. 2010;36(3):48. doi:10.18637/jss.v036.i03 Google ScholarCrossref
59.
George  KM, Lutsey  PL, Selvin  E, Palta  P, Windham  BG, Folsom  AR.  Association between thyroid dysfunction and incident dementia in the Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities Neurocognitive Study.   J Endocrinol Metab. 2019;9(4):82-89. doi:10.14740/jem588 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
60.
Folkestad  L, Brandt  F, Lillevang-Johansen  M, Brix  TH, Hegedüs  L.  Graves’ disease and toxic nodular goiter, aggravated by duration of hyperthyroidism, are associated with Alzheimer’s and vascular dementia: a registry-based long-term follow-up of two large cohorts.   Thyroid. 2020;30(5):672-680. doi:10.1089/thy.2019.0672 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
61.
Agarwal  R, Kushwaha  S, Chhillar  N, Kumar  A, Dubey  DK, Tripathi  CB.  A cross-sectional study on thyroid status in North Indian elderly outpatients with dementia.   Ann Indian Acad Neurol. 2013;16(3):333-337. doi:10.4103/0972-2327.116916 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
62.
Winkler  A, Weimar  C, Jöckel  KH,  et al.  Thyroid-stimulating hormone and mild cognitive impairment: results of the Heinz Nixdorf Recall Study.   J Alzheimers Dis. 2016;49(3):797-807. doi:10.3233/JAD-150561 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
63.
Chachamovitz  DS, Vigário  PdosS, Silva  SO,  et al.  Does low-normal serum TSH level adversely impact cognition in elderly adults and might methimazole therapy improve outcomes?   Endocr J. 2016;63(5):495-505. doi:10.1507/endocrj.EJ15-0458 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
64.
Wouters  HJCM, Slagter  SN, Muller Kobold  AC, van der Klauw  MM, Wolffenbuttel  BHR.  Epidemiology of thyroid disorders in the Lifelines Cohort Study (the Netherlands).   PLoS One. 2020;15(11):e0242795. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0242795 PubMedGoogle Scholar
65.
Chaker  L, Baumgartner  C, den Elzen  WP,  et al; Thyroid Studies Collaboration.  Thyroid function within the reference range and the risk of stroke: an individual participant data analysis.   J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2016;101(11):4270-4282. doi:10.1210/jc.2016-2255 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
66.
Bano  A, Chaker  L, Mattace-Raso  FUS,  et al.  Thyroid function and the risk of atherosclerotic cardiovascular morbidity and mortality: the Rotterdam Study.   Circ Res. 2017;121(12):1392-1400. doi:10.1161/CIRCRESAHA.117.311603 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
67.
Mooijaart  SP, Broekhuizen  K, Trompet  S,  et al.  Evidence-based medicine in older patients: how can we do better?   Neth J Med. 2015;73(5):211-218.PubMedGoogle Scholar
68.
Calamia  M, Markon  K, Tranel  D.  Scoring higher the second time around: meta-analyses of practice effects in neuropsychological assessment.   Clin Neuropsychol. 2012;26(4):543-570. doi:10.1080/13854046.2012.680913 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
×