Background
Collecting data on medical errors is essential for improving patient safety, but factors affecting error reporting by physicians are poorly understood.
Methods
Survey of faculty and resident physicians in the midwest, mid-Atlantic, and northeast regions of the United States to investigate reporting of actual errors, likelihood of reporting hypothetical errors, attitudes toward reporting errors, and demographic factors.
Results
Responses were received from 338 participants (response rate, 74.0%). Most respondents agreed that reporting errors improves the quality of care for future patients (84.3%) and would likely report a hypothetical error resulting in minor (73%) or major (92%) harm to a patient. However, only 17.8% of respondents had reported an actual minor error (resulting in prolonged treatment or discomfort), and only 3.8% had reported an actual major error (resulting in disability or death). Moreover, 16.9% acknowledged not reporting an actual minor error, and 3.8% acknowledged not reporting an actual major error. Only 54.8% of respondents knew how to report errors, and only 39.5% knew what kind of errors to report. Multivariate analyses of answers to hypothetical vignettes showed that willingness to report was positively associated with believing that reporting improves the quality of care, knowing how to report errors, believing in forgiveness, and being a faculty physician (vs a resident).
Conclusion
Most faculty and resident physicians are inclined to report harm-causing hypothetical errors, but only a minority have actually reported an error.
To improve patient safety, it is necessary to understand the frequency, seriousness, and causes of medical errors.1-8 Such knowledge is acquired by the analysis of data collected through error-reporting systems. These systems may be internal or external to health care institutions and may be voluntary or mandatory.9 Whatever their particular design, at the ground level of clinical practice, such reporting relies on a professional culture in which physicians and other health care providers view error disclosure as an integral part of learning and quality improvement.4,10-12 It is also important to consider how reporting errors to institutions to enhance patient safety may differ from disclosing errors to patients as part of direct patient care.13
Despite recent professional and legislative efforts to encourage error reporting,9,14 including the passage of the Patient Safety and Quality Improvement Act of 2005,15 it is not clear whether error reporting has become more common since the Institute of Medicine published its seminal report, To Err Is Human: Building a Safer Health System, in 2000.16-18 Obstacles to reporting are diverse and daunting,19,20 and underreporting of errors is believed to be pervasive.9,21 Because error surveillance systems generally rely on self-reporting,22 it is important to understand the factors that continue to impede reporting, as well as those that may facilitate it. To improve our knowledge about error reporting, we conducted a survey of physicians in teaching hospitals. We chose this setting because of the influence teaching hospitals may have on trainees' attitudes toward error reporting and patient safety.
Faculty and resident physicians were drawn from 3 medical centers located in the midwest, mid-Atlantic, and northeast regions of the United States. Surveys were completed from June 1 through September 30, 2004, with the exception of 32 resident physicians and 51 faculty physicians at 1 study site who completed surveys from January 1 through March 31, 2005. Two of the 3 states in which the study was conducted enacted mandatory error reporting legislation in 2002; the third state had no such legislation.23 All participating hospitals had error reporting systems in place for clinicians during the study.
After approval by the institutional review boards at each of the participating institutions, potential participants were invited to complete a self-administered, paper-based survey as previously described.13 No personal identifying information was collected, and participants were assured that they and their institutions would remain anonymous.
Resident physicians were from the disciplines of family medicine, internal medicine, and pediatrics. Faculty physicians were from the disciplines of family medicine, general internal medicine, and general pediatrics, with the addition of 36 pediatric specialists from 1 study site. The pediatric specialists were surveyed as a comparison group to contrast with the generalist faculty physicians. When all variables were analyzed for differences, responses from pediatric specialists were not significantly different from generalist faculty responses, so their responses were included for analysis.
Actual experiences with errors and disclosure were queried by asking if the respondents had ever made a mistake that (1) prolonged treatment or caused discomfort or (2) caused disability or death. In each case, they were asked whether they had reported or not reported the error to their institution.
To assess respondents' willingness to disclose errors, we used a hypothetical error vignette similar to that used by Blendon et al,24 followed by 3 different outcomes of varying severity (no harm, minor harm, and major harm) (Figure). The question at the end of each outcome was followed by response options of very likely, likely, not sure, unlikely, and very unlikely. A modified version of the hypothetical vignette and responses was given to the pediatric faculty and residents by changing the patient to a 7-year-old boy and adjusting the third clinical outcome by making no mention of a myocardial infarction and concluding that “the patient's condition stabilizes and he is transferred out of the intensive care unit after 24 hours.”
Questions concerning knowledge and attitudes toward error reporting were based on an empirically derived taxonomy of factors that facilitate and impede disclosure and were designed to probe the domains of responsibility to community, attitudinal barriers, uncertainties, helplessness, and fears and anxieties.19 Attitudinal questions used 5-point Likert scale responses ranging from strongly agree to strongly disagree.
Demographic variables included training level, specialty, sex, belief in forgiveness, experience giving medicolegal testimony, and being named as a defendant in a malpractice case. To ensure the anonymity of responses, we did not query age, year of graduation, or race/ethnicity.
Based on previous focus group data, we did not make a distinction between medical error and medical mistake and printed the following statement in the questionnaire:
“We use ‘medical error’ and ‘medical mistake’ interchangeably to describe a preventable adverse event that affects a patient by prolonging treatment or causing discomfort, disability, or death.”
The questionnaire was pilot tested for face validity, clarity, and stability over time with 16 participants. Two rounds of identical surveys were distributed to these participants, separated by 2 weeks. Based on the stability of each item (calculated using the Spearman correlation of each individual's response at times 1 and 2), 1 question was clarified and 17 questions were removed. Items that had a correlation coefficient of less than 0.50 were not included in the final questionnaire. All of the final questions had good to excellent reliability, with a Spearman ρ ≥ 0.6.25 (The questionnaire is available on request from the first author.)
Answers from the questionnaires were entered into an Access data file and uploaded into SAS statistical software (PC SAS, version 8.1; SAS Institute Inc, Cary, North Carolina). Likert scale responses were dichotomized as follows: (1) likely/very likely vs not sure/unlikely/very unlikely, and (2) agree/strongly agree vs neutral/disagree/strongly disagree. By grouping undecided and negative responses together, this dichotomization placed primary analytic focus on positive responses. To simplify reporting in the “Results” section, likely signifies the combination of “likely” and “very likely” responses, and agree signifies the combination of “agree” and “strongly agree” responses.
We calculated frequency distributions of responses and used the 2-tailed Fisher exact test or the χ2 statistic to test differences between proportions. For multivariate analysis, 7 knowledge and attitudinal variables and 6 demographic variables (training level, specialty, sex, belief in forgiveness, experience giving medicolegal testimony, and being named as a defendant in a malpractice case) served as independent variables, and answers to the hypothetical vignettes served as dependent variables. Independent variables that were significant (P < .10) in bivariate analyses were entered into a backward stepwise logistic regression model, with a stay criterion of P < .10, for each of the hypothetical vignettes. Once a final set of predictor variables was established, models were adjusted for any differences between sites.
Response rates and demographic characteristics
Surveys were completed by 138 faculty physicians and 200 resident physicians. The overall response rate was 74.0%, with subgroup response rates of 81.7% (faculty) and 69.4% (residents). Table 1 describes respondents' demographic characteristics.
Among the 338 respondents, 17.8% acknowledged reporting a minor error (prolonging treatment or causing discomfort), and 3.8% acknowledged reporting a major error (causing disability or death), as shown in Table 2. Conversely, 16.9% of respondents acknowledged not reporting a minor error, and 3.8% acknowledged not reporting a major error. There was minimal overlap between respondents who did and did not acknowledge reporting errors; 16 respondents acknowledged that they both reported and did not report minor errors, and 2 respondents acknowledged that they both reported and did not report major errors. Taking all acknowledged errors together (reported and not reported), 35.6% of respondents (50.9% of faculty and 25.8% of residents) acknowledged having made at least 1 minor or 1 major error, with 34.7% acknowledging a minor error and 7.5% acknowledging a major error.
Reporting hypothetical errors
The likelihood of reporting a hypothetical error depended on the outcome of the error. Of the respondents, 92% indicated that they would report a hypothetical error if it resulted in major harm, 73% if it resulted in minor harm, and 43% if it resulted in no harm (Table 3).
Knowledge and attitudes regarding error reporting
As shown in Table 4, most physicians believed that reporting errors improves the quality of care for future patients, but only 54.8% knew how to report errors to their institution and only 39.5% knew what kind of errors should be reported. Many respondents believed that it is hard to be certain about the true causes of adverse events, but few believed that reporting errors was not worth their time. The anticipation of feedback would make 47.9% of the respondents more likely to report errors, and 57.7% acknowledged concerns about professional discipline when thinking about disclosing errors in general. Resident physicians were less knowledgeable than faculty about how to report errors and what errors to report, and they were generally more concerned about professional discipline when disclosing errors.
Variables associated with reporting hypothetical errors
Respondents who had actually reported a minor error were more likely to indicate they would report a hypothetical error resulting in no harm (58.3% vs 38.7%; P = .006), minor harm (88.3% vs 68.3%; P = .002), and major harm (100% vs 90.9%; P = .02). Similar differences were found for respondents who had actually reported an error that resulted in major harm, although these differences did not reach statistical significance (P < .05).
Multivariate analyses were performed to create models for each of the 3 outcomes of the error (no harm, minor harm, and major harm). Five variables were found to be significant (P < .10) in 1 or more of the 3 models, as shown in Table 5. Respondents were more likely to report errors if they knew how to report them or believed that error reporting improves the quality of care, and they were less likely to report errors if they believed that reporting them was not worth their time. Faculty physicians were more likely than residents to report errors, and respondents for whom forgiveness was important were also more likely to report errors. Neither sex nor exposure to malpractice was associated with differences in the likelihood of reporting any of the hypothetical errors, and only 1 variable dropped out of 1 model after adjusting for site differences (an association between specialty and reporting an error resulting in no harm).
The results of this study make important contributions to a limited empirical literature on physician reporting of medical errors to improve patient safety. Our data provide new information from teaching hospitals about attitudes toward reporting, actual practices, and the potential influence of demographic factors such as level of training and beliefs about forgiveness. Particularly notable was the finding that although most of our respondents indicated they would likely report to their institutions a hypothetical error resulting in minor or major harm to a patient, only 17.8% acknowledged ever reporting an actual minor error, and only 3.8% acknowledged ever reporting an actual major error. Furthermore, 16.9% of respondents acknowledged not reporting an actual minor error and 3.8% acknowledged not reporting an actual major error. Taken together, these results suggest there may be a gap between attitude and practice among physicians regarding the reporting of medical errors.
If true, such a gap between attitude and practice in reporting errors has significant implications for efforts to improve patient safety and the quality of care. Although research has not yet demonstrated a clear link between reporting, intervention, and improved outcomes,26 the patient safety movement reasonably assumes that a better understanding of errors and their causes will lead to a reduction in their frequency. Our study was premised on the belief that error reports by physicians represent an important source of information from the front lines of care. If a substantial number of physicians are not reporting errors, efforts to improve patient safety may be handicapped.
One reason for underreporting errors may be a lack of knowledge: only 62.3% of faculty and 49.5% of residents knew how to report errors. The implications of this knowledge deficit are suggested by our multivariate analyses: respondents who knew how to report errors were 2 or 3 times more likely to report hypothetical errors. Knowledge about how to report errors is essential, especially in a training environment in which trainees need to observe a connection between institutional messages about the importance of reporting and clinical practices that make such messages credible. Another reason for underreporting may be lack of knowledge about what should be reported: only 53.6% of faculty and 30.0% of residents believed they knew what kinds of errors to report. Institutions should provide guidelines for reporting27 to help clinicians identify errors that are most likely to have significance for patient safety, and the ideal of comprehensive reporting should be balanced against opportunity costs to clinicians and data analysts.
Another source of underreporting may arise from reliance on error outcomes to decide whether an error should be reported. By using hypothetical vignettes with outcomes of variable seriousness, we found that many clinicians associate the need for error reporting with the severity of error outcome. This approach to error reporting fails to appreciate that many significant errors may not result in harm. Such errors—known as near misses—represent important opportunities to learn from mistakes that have not affected patients.17,28-30 Our hypothetical results are consistent with actual reporting patterns observed among physicians in intensive care units who report near misses less frequently than they report errors resulting in harm.31 Near misses may represent an underused resource for learning and improvement.
The need for a tangible connection between error reporting and improved patient care is suggested by our finding that more than half of respondents stated they would be more likely to report errors if they knew they would receive feedback afterward. Institutions can send important signals to those who report errors by acknowledging reports soon after they are submitted and, once assessed, informing the reporter how the report contributed (or is expected to contribute) to patient safety.21,29 Such information can help physicians see error reporting as a clinically relevant, institutionally valued, and effective activity.
Questions about legal liability regularly arise in discussions about error reporting because of concerns that reported information may be discoverable in a malpractice proceeding.9,17,18,32 In response to such concerns, leaders recommend that error reporting systems be confidential.9 Our study did not find that respondents who had been exposed to malpractice litigation were any less likely to report hypothetical errors. This negative finding in a relatively small study sample should be interpreted cautiously, and it does not address the general inhibitory effect on error reporting that may be exerted by fears of legal liability. There remain compelling reasons for legislation to protect physicians under the umbrella of peer review by making the reporting and discussion of errors privileged.33 Such legislation can reassure wary physicians that their conscientious efforts to improve the quality of health care will not be used against them. The Patient Safety and Quality Improvement Act15 represents important progress in addressing such concerns.
Advocates of patient safety have rightly called for the removal of blame and shame from the reporting of medical errors.1,10 In the effort to avoid casting blame, however, institutions need to recognize that feelings of guilt after an error may nevertheless be very real for physicians.13 Guilt feelings after errors may be related to a compulsive mindset that automatically views bad outcomes as failures,34 and their presence emphasizes the need for empathy.35 Related to this, our results suggest there may be connections between physicians' beliefs about forgiveness and their willingness to report errors. Respondents who agreed that forgiveness is an important part of their belief system were more than twice as likely to report a hypothetical error resulting in no harm or in minor harm. Although forgiveness is typically discussed in the context of disclosing errors to patients,36,37 these data suggest that forgiveness may also be relevant to reporting errors to institutions, at least among some physicians.
Our study had limitations. First, although the survey was anonymous, social desirability bias may have led some respondents to give answers that were perceived to be more socially acceptable. Second, we used a hypothetical error vignette with outcomes of variable severity that served as the dependent variables for multivariate analyses; however, answers to hypothetical scenarios may not predict actual behavior. Third, our respondents were based in teaching hospitals and represented internal medicine, family medicine, and pediatrics. Although adjustment for site differences had little effect on our multivariate analyses, our results may not be generalizable to physicians in other specialties or in other practice settings. For example, a recent study38 of pediatricians (439 faculty and 118 residents) found that 92% of participants had used at least 1 formal mechanism to report an error and that 82% believed they should report near misses to their institutions. Such results contrast with our findings and support the need for further investigation in other populations. Finally, our data were collected in 2004 and 2005 and may not reflect more current attitudes or practices.
The results of this study suggest that physicians' attitudes about the value of error reporting may not be matched by actual behaviors. If correct, the potential causes of this discrepancy ought to be addressed. Health care institutions should continue efforts to create learning environments in which error discussions are valued and those who discuss their own errors are respected. Institutions should also ensure that error-reporting systems are confidential, simple, and worthwhile.9,39 To convince physicians that reporting errors is not a fruitless exercise, institutions should advertise examples that display the connection between error analysis and system improvement. Institutions should also teach physicians how to report errors and what errors to report. Without such efforts to facilitate a shift toward a culture of reporting among physicians, the effect of federally protected patient safety reporting systems is likely to be reduced and the reporting bias inherent in these systems will be unlikely to diminish.40 Finally, institutions should consider ways to promote patient-centered ethical values that may motivate physicians to report errors in the midst of countervailing pressures,19,41 especially in teaching hospitals where role models play a vital part in the formation of trainees' attitudes and practices. Such values are rightly seen as part of medical professionalism42-44 and reflect a commitment not merely to good systems but to the good of our future patients.
Correspondence: Lauris C. Kaldjian, MD, PhD, Department of Internal Medicine, 1-106 MEB, University of Iowa Roy J. and Lucille A. Carver College of Medicine, 500 Newton Rd, Iowa City, IA 52242 (lauris-kaldjian@uiowa.edu).
Accepted for Publication: August 12, 2007.
Author Contributions: Dr Kaldjian had full access to all the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis. Study concept and design: Kaldjian, Jones, Wu, and Rosenthal. Acquisition of data: Jones, Wu, and Levi. Analysis and interpretation of data: Kaldjian, Forman-Hoffman, and Rosenthal. Drafting of the manuscript: Kaldjian. Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Kaldjian, Jones, Forman-Hoffman, Levi, and Rosenthal. Statistical analysis: Forman-Hoffman. Obtained funding: Kaldjian. Administrative, technical, and material support: Jones, Wu, Levi, and Rosenthal. Study supervision: Rosenthal.
Financial Disclosure: None reported.
Funding/Support: This study was supported by grant 45446 from the Robert Wood Johnson Foundation's Generalist Physician Faculty Scholars Program (Dr Kaldjian). Drs Kaldjian, Forman-Hoffman, and Rosenthal are investigators in the Center for Research in the Implementation of Innovative Strategies in Practice at the Iowa City Veterans Affairs Health Care System, which is funded through the Department of Veterans Affairs, Veterans Health Administration, Health Services Research and Development Service.
Role of the Sponsor: The funding organization had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; or preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript.
Disclaimer: The views expressed in this article are those of the authors and do not necessarily represent the views of the Department of Veterans Affairs.
Previous Presentations: Partial data from this study were presented at the 30th Annual Meeting of the Society of General Internal Medicine; April 26, 2007; Toronto, Ontario, Canada, and have been published as an abstract (J Gen Intern Med. 2007;22[suppl 1]:136).
1.Kohn
LTCorrigan
JMDonaldson
MS To Err Is Human: Building a Safer Health System. Washington, DC National Academy Press2000;
2.Weingart
SNWilson
RMBibberd
RWHarrison
B Epidemiology of medical error.
BMJ 2000;320
(7237)
774- 777
PubMedGoogle Scholar 3.Vincent
C Understanding and responding to adverse events.
N Engl J Med 2003;348
(11)
1051- 1056
PubMedGoogle Scholar 5.Brennan
TA The Institute of Medicine report on medical errors: could it do harm?
N Engl J Med 2000;342
(15)
1123- 1125
PubMedGoogle Scholar 6.Berwick
DM Not again! preventing errors lies in redesign—not exhortation.
BMJ 2001;322
(7281)
247- 248
PubMedGoogle Scholar 7.Studdert
DMBrennan
TA No fault compensation for medical injuries: the prospect of error prevention.
JAMA 2001;286
(2)
217- 223
PubMedGoogle Scholar 11.Leape
LLWoods
DDHatlie
MJKizer
KWSchroeder
SALundberg
GD Promoting patient safety by preventing medical error.
JAMA 1998;280
(16)
1444- 1447
PubMedGoogle Scholar 12.Wachter
RMShojania
KGMarkowitz
AJSmith
MSaint
S Quality grand rounds: the case for patient safety.
Ann Intern Med 2006;145
(8)
629- 630
PubMedGoogle Scholar 13.Kaldjian
LCJones
EWWu
BJForman-Hoffman
VLLevi
BHRosenthal
GE Disclosing medical errors to patients: attitudes and practices of physicians and trainees.
J Gen Intern Med 2007;22
(7)
988- 996
PubMedGoogle Scholar 14.Finkelstein
J Patient safety laboratories.
Am Med News 2005;48
(1)
5- 6
Google Scholar 16.Robinson
ARHohmann
KBRifkin
JI
et al. Physician and public opinions on quality of health care and the problem of medical errors.
Arch Intern Med 2002;162
(19)
2186- 2190
PubMedGoogle Scholar 17.Altman
DEClancy
CBlendon
RJ Improving patient safety: five years after the IOM report.
N Engl J Med 2004;351
(20)
2041- 2043
PubMedGoogle Scholar 18.Leape
LLBerwick
DM Five years after
To Err Is Human: what have we learned?
JAMA 2005;293
(19)
2384- 2390
PubMedGoogle Scholar 19.Kaldjian
LCJones
EWRosenthal
GETripp-Reimer
THillis
SL An empirically derived taxonomy of factors affecting physicians' willingness to disclose medical errors.
J Gen Intern Med 2006;21
(9)
942- 948
PubMedGoogle Scholar 20.Kaldjian
LCJones
EWRosenthal
GE Facilitating and impeding factors for physicians' error disclosure: a structured literature review.
Jt Comm J Qual Patient Saf 2006;32
(4)
188- 198
PubMedGoogle Scholar 21.Wachter
RM The end of the beginning: patient safety five years after “To Err is Human.”
Health Aff (Millwood) July-December2004;
((suppl Web exclusives))
W4-534- W4-54510.1377hlthaff.W4.534
PubMedGoogle Scholar 22.Pronovost
PJMiller
MRWachter
RM Tracking progress in patient safety: an elusive target.
JAMA 2006;296
(6)
696- 699
PubMedGoogle Scholar 24.Blendon
RJDesRoches
CMBrodie
M
et al. Views of practicing physicians and the public on medical errors.
N Engl J Med 2002;347
(24)
1933- 1940
PubMedGoogle Scholar 25.Nunnally
JCBernstein
IH Psychometric Theory. 3rd ed. New York, NY McGraw-Hill Co1994;
26.Pronovost
PJThompson
DAHolzmueller
CG
et al. Toward learning from patient safety reporting systems.
J Crit Care 2006;21
(4)
305- 315
PubMedGoogle Scholar 27.Winters
BDBerenholtz
SMPronovost
P Improving patient safety reporting systems.
Crit Care Med 2007;35
(4)
1206- 1207
PubMedGoogle Scholar 28.Barach
PSmall
SD Reporting and preventing medical mishaps: lessons from non-medical near miss reporting systems.
BMJ 2000;320
(7237)
759- 763
PubMedGoogle Scholar 29.Uribe
CLSchweikhart
SBPathak
DSDow
MMarsh
GB Perceived barriers to medical-error reporting: an exploratory investigation.
J Healthc Manag 2002;47
(4)
263- 280
PubMedGoogle Scholar 30.Chaudhry
SIOlofinboba
KAKrumholz
HM Detection of errors by attending physicians on a general medicine service.
J Gen Intern Med 2003;18
(8)
595- 600
PubMedGoogle Scholar 31.Harris
CBKrauss
MJCoopersmith
CM
et al. Patient safety event reporting in critical care: a study of three intensive care units.
Crit Care Med 2007;35
(4)
1068- 1076
PubMedGoogle Scholar 32.Mikkelsen
THSokolowski
IOlesen
F General practitioners' attitudes toward reporting and learning from adverse events: results from a survey.
Scand J Prim Health Care 2006;24
(1)
27- 32
PubMedGoogle Scholar 34.Gabbard
GO The role of compulsiveness in the normal physician.
JAMA 1985;254
(20)
2926- 2929
PubMedGoogle Scholar 35.Dubovsky
SLSchrier
RW The mystique of medical training: is teaching perfection in medical house-staff training a reasonable goal or a precursor of low self-esteem?
JAMA 1983;250
(22)
3057- 3058
PubMedGoogle Scholar 36.Mazor
KMSimon
SRYood
RA
et al. Health plan members' views on forgiving medical errors.
Am J Manag Care 2005;11
(1)
49- 52
PubMedGoogle Scholar 37.Leape
LL Full disclosure and apology: an idea whose time has come.
Physician Exec 2006;32
(2)
16- 18
PubMedGoogle Scholar 38.Garbutt
JBrownstein
DRKlein
EJ
et al. Reporting and disclosing medical errors: pediatricians' attitudes and behaviors.
Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 2007;161
(2)
179- 185
PubMedGoogle Scholar 39.Classen
DCKilbridge
PM The roles and responsibility of physicians to improve patient safety within health care delivery systems.
Acad Med 2002;77
(10)
963- 972
PubMedGoogle Scholar 40.Pronovost
PHolzmueller
CGNeedham
DM
et al. How will we know patients are safer? an organization-wide approach to measuring and improving safety.
Crit Care Med 2006;34
(7)
1988- 1995
PubMedGoogle Scholar 41.Andrus
CHVillasenor
EGKettelle
JBRoth
RSweeney
AMMatolo
NM “To Err Is Human:” uniformly reporting medical errors and near misses, a naïve, costly, and misdirected goal.
J Am Coll Surg 2003;196
(6)
911- 918
PubMedGoogle Scholar 42.ABIM Foundation, American Board of Internal Medicine; ACP-ASIM Foundation, American College of Physicians/American Society of Internal Medicine; European Federation of Internal Medicine, Medical professionalism in the new millennium: a physician charter.
Ann Intern Med 2002;136
(3)
243- 246
PubMedGoogle Scholar 43.Brennan
TA Physicians' professional responsibility to improve the quality of care.
Acad Med 2002;77
(10)
973- 980
PubMedGoogle Scholar 44.Katz
JNKessler
CLO’Connell
ALevine
SA Professionalism and evolving concepts of quality.
J Gen Intern Med 2007;22
(1)
137- 139
PubMedGoogle Scholar