Background
Although characteristics such as heart rate (HR) and blood pressure (BP) are commonly reported in studies of the relationship between fatigue and cardiac functioning, few reports examine how cardiac function parameters such as cardiac output (CO) and stroke volume (SV) relate to fatigue. This study examined the relationship between self-reported fatigue and hemodynamic functioning at rest and in response to a public speaking stressor in healthy individuals.
Methods
A total of 142 individuals participated in this study. Subjects were placed in low-, moderate-, or high-fatigue groups based on their Profile of Moods State fatigue scale. Heart rate, SV, and CO were determined using impedance cardiography at rest and during a speaking stressor. Stroke volume and CO values were converted to stroke index (SI) and cardiac index (CI) by adjusting for body surface area. Data were analyzed with hierarchical regression analysis and a 3 (group) × 3 (stress period) mixed model analysis of variance.
Results
At rest, fatigue was not associated with BP or HR but was significantly associated with decreased CI (P < .001; 95% confidence interval, −0.046 to −0.014) and stroke index (SI) (P = .002; 95% confidence interval −0.664 to −0.151), even after controlling for demographic variables and depressive symptoms. Heart rate and BP increased, as expected, from baseline to preparation to speaking stressor (F1,124 = 118.6 and F1,122 = 46.450, respectively) (P < .001 for both). More interestingly, there were effects on SI and CI of fatigue (P<.03 for both) and stress (P<.03 for both); high-fatigue individuals had lower SI and CI levels than moderate- and low-fatigue individuals both at rest and in response to the stressor.
Conclusion
This study demonstrates that fatigue complaints may have hemodynamic correlates even in ostensibly healthy individuals.
Fatigue is one of the most commonly encountered complaints in medical practice. It is characterized by a lessened capacity or motivation for work and is usually accompanied by feelings of weariness and sleepiness. Fatigue is a nonspecific symptom associated with many medical conditions such as hypothyroidism, celiac disease, anemia, influenza, heart failure, and sleep apnea. It is the defining symptom in chronic fatigue syndrome (CFS) and is one of the most distressing symptoms associated with chemotherapy. Fatigue is also a common complaint of many veterans of the Gulf War of the 1990s. In most instances, the precise mechanism of fatigue is not well understood.1-7
Recent reports have found disturbances in autonomic and immune activity in patients with CFS.8,9 Using tests of heart rate variability with deep respiration, blood pressure (BP) decrease on tilting, heart rate (HR) increase with postural change, and resting HR, Freeman and Komaroff10 found that patients with CFS had lower amplitude change than healthy controls in HR variability with deep respiration, HR increase with postural change, and BP decrease on tilting. Numerous other studies of stress response in patients with fatigue reveal subtle changes in autonomic nervous system activity.5,9-14
Given the symptoms associated with fatigue, it is natural to wonder if cardiac function is altered in the presence of high fatigue levels from whatever cause. Unfortunately, studies examining the relationships between cardiac function and fatigue are rare, although some studies suggest that excessive fatigue may be an early manifestation of heart failure.15,16 In a study examining hemodynamic functioning in patients with CFS, it was found that patients with severe CFS had significantly lower stroke volume (SV) and cardiac output (CO) than the controls and less ill patients.17,18 Instead of studying cardiac function directly, most studies of fatigue and cardiovascular physiology focus on more readily measurable variables such as HR, HR variability, and BP.
A recent article by Choi et al19 found that sleepiness levels were related to CO in a sample of patients with obstructive sleep apnea but with no known cardiac disease. Individuals who reported more sleepiness had lower CO and SV. These findings implied that complaints such as sleepiness may not just reflect subjective distress but may also be an early marker of cardiac impairment. Might this observation apply to healthy controls as well?
Given the work on autonomic nervous system functioning in CFS and the findings that sleepiness is associated with lower CO in patients with obstructive sleep apnea, we wondered if complaints of fatigue might be associated with altered SV and CO, even in healthy subjects. We also wondered if these noninvasive measures of cardiac hemodynamic functioning may be more sensitive to fatigue's effects than measures of HR or BP.
One hundred forty-two volunteers (47% women, 46% black, and 20% with hypertension) participated in this study. Subjects were either healthy volunteers or patients with mild to moderate hypertension who were not receiving antihypertensive medication.
We excluded patients with a prior diagnosis or treatment of diabetes, a fasting blood glucose level higher than 120 mg/dL, patients with congestive heart failure, bronchospastic pulmonary disease, symptomatic coronary or cerebral vascular disease (history of myocardial infarction, angina, stroke, or transient ischemic attack), history of life-threatening arrhythmias, cardiomyopathy, history of psychosis, current drug abuse, or current alcohol abuse (supported by history or elevated findings on liver function tests). We excluded patients who had received psychotropic medications, patients with known secondary hypertension, those with creatinine levels higher than 1.4 mg/dL, proteinuria, hematuria, or renal bruit found on physical examination. Women accepted for the study were not taking contraceptives, nor were they pregnant. Characteristics of the subjects are summarized in Table 1. The protocol was approved by the institutional review board of the University of California San Diego, and subjects entered the study after giving informed consent.
To convert fasting blood glucose to millimoles per liter, multiply by 0.0555; creatinine to micromoles per liter, multiply by 76.25.
Participants were admitted to the General Clinical Research Center of the University of California San Diego the night prior to the testing day. They completed the mood self-report using the Profile of Mood States (POMS)20 while resting comfortably the evening before testing. The POMS is a self-administered, adjective rating scale that measures 6 dimensions of mood: tension-anxiety, depression-dejection, anger-hostility, vigor-activity, confusion-bewilderment, and fatigue-inertia during the past week. The normative data indicate a mean (SD) score of 7 (6).20 The POMS has been used in a variety of chronically ill populations, including groups with obstructive sleep apnea, as well as in healthy populations.21 The present study focuses on the fatigue-inertia subscale, which measures weariness, inertia, and low energy level states.
Cardiovascular reactivity testing, as a way to perturb the heart and autonomic nervous system, was performed on the morning of the testing day. Blood pressure data were collected continuously with a Colin Pilot radial tonometric noninvasive blood pressure monitor (Colin Instruments, Houston, Texas). Heart rate was determined by electrocardiography. Stroke volume was determined with impedance cardiography, and CO was calculated.
To record the impedance cardiography measures, impedance cardiographic tape was applied in a tetrapolar configuration. The Z2 electrode was attached around the base of the neck just superior to the suprasternal notch of the thorax. The Z1 electrode was placed 3 cm above the Z2. Electrode Z3 used the xiphoid process as an anatomic landmark. The electrode tape was placed over the xiphoid process circumscribing the thorax, and the tape was kept parallel to the floor. The Z4 electrode was placed about 10 cm below and parallel to the Z3. The distance between the Z2 and Z3 was also determined while the subject was standing. After application of the impedance cardiographic electrode, the electrocardiograph electrodes were applied in a modified lead 1 or lead 2 configuration that maximized the R wave.
The signals were relayed to an analog-to-digital converter sampling at 1 kHz per channel and stored in a desktop personal computer. The dZ and Zo calibration signals were also stored in the computer for later conversion of the dZ/dt to Ω/s and SV derivation. These data were collected in 3-minute epochs. The samples were ensemble averaged by a computer program (University of Miami, Behavioral Medicine Research Center, Coral Gables, Florida) that summed the digitized beat-by-beat waveforms, time synchronized to the R wave of the electrocardiogram, and divided by the number of cardiac cycles. The ensemble average was then graphically displayed, and the waveform events were scored by computer signal processing techniques using University of Miami, Behavioral Medicine Research Center software.22 Stroke volume was calculated using the Kubicek formula.23 The resting data were collected after the participant had been seated in a recliner chair for 30 minutes after instrumentation. Stroke volume and CO were converted to SI and CI by appropriate adjustment of body surface area.
For the cardiovascular reactivity testing, subjects were attached to the instruments at 9 AM and then allowed to sit quietly for 30 minutes for habituation to the instrumentation and testing environment. A 3-minute baseline was obtained at the end of the habituation period. After a 3-minute baseline period, the subjects were given instructions for a speaking task that involved preparing and presenting a speech in response to being falsely accused of shoplifting. Instructions noted that the performance would be videotaped and rated by experts on poise and articulation. The video camera was displayed prominently during the procedure. Subjects were given 3 minutes to prepare their speech and told that the speech should cover certain points. Immediately after the preparation period, subjects spoke for 3 minutes. If a subject stopped talking before the end of the period, he or she was reminded to continue the talk by reiterating or summarizing the main points. This speech stressor has been used extensively in our laboratory and other laboratories and elicits reliable changes in the measured parameters.14,24
To test our hypothesis that fatigue would predict SI and CI but not BP or HR, we performed a hierarchical regression analysis. In separate runs we placed SI, CI, HR, and mean arterial pressure (MAP) as dependent variables against a set of demographic variables in the first block (ethnicity, sex, age, and screening BP). The POMS fatigue score was then entered on step 2. The POMS depression score was entered on step 3 to examine the possibility that depressive symptoms might account for the findings concerning fatigue.
For illustrative purposes, POMS fatigue tertiles were created, and analyses of variance were performed. Individuals with POMS fatigue scores lower than 3 were classified as having low fatigue; those with scores between 3 and 7 were considered to have moderate fatigue; and those with scores higher than 7 were considered to have high fatigue. The dependent variables were MAP, CI, SI, and HR. These data were analyzed using a 3 × 3 mixed model analysis of variance (high vs moderate vs low fatigue group by baseline, preparation, and speaking stress period) (SPSS for Windows, version 12.0; SPSS, Chicago, Illinois). All P values are reported using the orthogonal trend method for repeated measures; Bonferroni corrections were made where appropriate.
Resting baseline measures of si, ci, bp, and hr in relation to fatigue
Fatigue did not predict MAP or HR (data not shown). As summarized in Tables 2, 3, 4, and 5, fatigue was a significant predictor of SI and CI. Fatigue uniquely accounted for approximately 6% of the variance in SI and 8% of the variance in CI. The fatigue effects were independent of demographic variables such as age, ethnicity, sex, or screening BP. For explaining SI as well as CI, fatigue nearly doubled the variance explained by the demographic variables (P < .002 and P < .001, respectively). Adding depressive symptoms in a third step did not significantly increase the predictive capacity of the model, nor did it detract from the power of fatigue to explain CI and SI. Figure 1 graphically portrays the fatigue effects on hemodynamic functioning at rest.
Stress effects on bp, hr, ci, and si in relation to fatigue
As seen in Figure 2, there was a significant stress effect for MAP (F1,122 = 46.5; P < .001) and heart rate (F1,124 = 118.6; P < .001) such that HR increased significantly from baseline to speech preparation to speech delivery (P<.001 for all). Mean arterial pressure increased significantly from baseline to preparation to speaking (P<.001 for all). No significant differences were found in HR or BP response between the various fatigue groups, nor was there a significant group-by-period interaction observed for MAP or HR.
As seen in Figure 3, there were significant stress effects on SI (F1,118 = 8.80; P < .001) and on CI (F1,117 = 6.53; P < .02). There was also a significant effect of fatigue on SI (F2,118 = 4.82; P = .02) and CI (F2,117 = 2.22; P = .002). The high-fatigue group had significantly lower levels of SI than the moderate-fatigue group or the low-fatigue group (P = .03 and P = .02, respectively). No significant differences were found between moderate- and low-fatigue groups. No significant group-by-period interactions were observed.
Cardiac index increased significantly from baseline to preparation (P = .02) but showed no further change from preparation to talking. The high-fatigue group had significantly lower levels of CI than the moderate-fatigue group (P = .03) and low-fatigue group (P = .02). No significant differences were found between moderate- and low-fatigue groups.
We found that decreased SI and CI are related to higher fatigue at rest and during acute stress in healthy individuals. These findings are similar to what other researchers have found in patients with chronic fatigue and in veterans with Gulf War syndrome.17,18,25-28 The changes in hemodynamic function are subtle, certainly not in the recognized pathological range. However, these subtle changes were significantly associated with complaints of fatigue in reasonably healthy individuals.
These findings suggest that fatigue has a measurable relationship with cardiac functioning. There were no significant fatigue effects on simple measures such as HR or BP. However, fatigue had significant effects on hemodynamic measures of CI and SI. Measuring CI and SI is not typically part of the medical examination; thus, physicians may mistakenly conclude on the basis of routine office screening that there is no cardiovascular disease accounting for fatigue.
There are several limitations of this study that should be taken into consideration. One limitation deals with our use of impedance cardiography to measure cardiovascular activity. This technique tends to underestimate SI and CI but tracks relative changes well.29 Replicating these observations with other measures such as echocardiography would be helpful.
Table 1 reveals that roughly 33% of moderate-fatigue and high-fatigue subjects had hypertension, whereas only about 15% of the low-fatigue subjects did (P = .23). The link between fatigue and BP is hardly straightforward; investigators have also found that fatigue is associated with lower BP.30 Even in the absence of a significant effect of hypertension status on fatigue, one might wonder if our observations on fatigue in relation to CI and the other parameters are being driven by hypertension. We reanalyzed the data using BP as a covariate and still observed the prominent effects of fatigue on SI and CI, even after controlling for BP (Tables 2, 3, 4, and 5).
Similarly, the POMS might not be an ideal measure of fatigue. The fatigue subscale is a widely used measure of fatigue but does not assess specific dimensions of fatigue. Other measures such as the Multidimensional Fatigue Symptom Inventory31 could be used to examine specific aspects of fatigue such as physical, emotional, or mental fatigue. Since complaints of fatigue are so common and so nonspecific, use of more refined fatigue inventories may facilitate better understanding of the fatigue symptom setting that is associated with decreased cardiac functioning.
We did not evaluate sex or ethnic differences because the subgroup sample size would have been so small that the findings might have been unstable, given our statistical approaches. Future studies might include these factors. For instance, both social class and ethnicity influence complaints of fatigue.32 Because African Americans have increased rates of cardiovascular disease, it might be important to examine ethnic differences in future studies of fatigue and hemodynamic functioning.21,33,34 It would also be interesting to see if these finding are found in other patient groups for whom fatigue is a symptom (eg, patients with cancer). On the other hand, an advantage of this study was its focus on reasonably healthy individuals. With such a design, one minimizes potential confounding factors such as anemia, thyroid disease, or medications, which could affect both hemodynamic functioning and fatigue levels.
Is fatigue a prodrome of decreased hemodynamic functioning? This study suggests that there may be hidden cardiovascular manifestations of fatigue, even in healthy subjects. It remains to be seen whether the fatigue and/or stress effects on cardiovascular functioning observed herein are even stronger in patients with known cardiovascular disease. Similarly, the findings provide interesting context to studies reporting beneficial effects of exercise on both cardiovascular deconditioning and fatigue.35,36
Correspondence: Joel E. Dimsdale, MD, Department of Psychiatry, University of California San Diego, 9500 Gilman Dr (0804), La Jolla, CA 92093-0804 (jdimsdale@ucsd.edu).
Accepted for Publication: November 21, 2007.
Author Contributions: Dr Dimsdale had full access to all of the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis. Study concept and design: Nelesen and Dimsdale. Acquisition of data: Nelesen and Dar. Analysis and interpretation of data: Nelesen, Thomas, and Dimsdale. Drafting of the manuscript: Nelesen, Dar, Thomas, and Dimsdale. Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Nelesen and Dimsdale. Statistical analysis: Thomas. Obtained funding: Dimsdale. Administrative, technical, and material support: Nelesen, Dar, and Dimsdale. Study supervision: Dimsdale.
Financial Disclosure: None reported.
Funding/Support: This work was supported by National Institutes of Health grants HL36005, HL44915, CA023100, and RR 00827 (Dr Dimsdale).
Additional Contributions: Wayne Bardwell, PhD, Loki Natarajan, PhD, Michael Ziegler, MD, and Paul Mills, PhD, provided helpful comments.
1.Jason
LATaylor
RRKennedy
CL
et al. Chronic fatigue syndrome: symptom subtypes in a community based sample.
Women Health 2003;37
(1)
1- 13
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 3.Holmes
GPKaplan
JEGantz
NM
et al. Chronic fatigue syndrome: a working case definition.
Ann Intern Med 1988;108
(3)
387- 389
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 4.Fukuda
KStraus
SEHickie
I
et al. The chronic fatigue syndrome: a comprehensive approach to its definition and study.
Ann Intern Med 1994;121
(12)
953- 959
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 5.Duprez
DADe Buyzere
MLDrieghe
B
et al. Long- and short-term blood pressure and RR-interval variability and psychosomatic distress in chronic fatigue syndrome.
Clin Sci (Lond) 1998;94
(1)
57- 63
PubMedGoogle Scholar 6.Vercoulen
JHSwanink
CMFennis
JFGalama
JMvan der Meer
JWBleijenberg
G Dimensional assessment of chronic fatigue syndrome.
J Psychosom Res 1994;38
(5)
383- 392
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 7.Taylor
RRJason
LAJahn
SC Chronic fatigue and sociodemographic characteristics as predictors of psychiatric disorders in a community-based sample.
Psychosom Med 2003;65
(5)
896- 901
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 8.Pagani
MLucini
D Chronic fatigue syndrome: a hypothesis focusing on the autonomic nervous system.
Clin Sci (Lond) 1999;96
(1)
117- 125
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 9.Pagani
MLucini
DMela
GSLangewitz
WMalliani
A Sympathetic overactivity in subjects complaining of unexplained fatigue.
Clin Sci (Lond) 1994;87
(6)
655- 661
PubMedGoogle Scholar 11.Cordero
DLSisto
SATapp
WNLaManca
JJPareja
JGNatelson
BH Decreased vagal power during treadmill walking in patients with chronic fatigue syndrome.
Clin Auton Res 1996;6
(6)
329- 333
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 12.LaManca
JJPeckerman
ASisto
SADeLuca
JCook
SNatelson
BH Cardiovascular responses of women with chronic fatigue syndrome to stressful cognitive testing before and after strenuous exercise.
Psychosom Med 2001;63
(5)
756- 764
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 14.Sisto
SATapp
WDrastal
S
et al. Vagal tone is reduced during paced breathing in patients with the chronic fatigue syndrome.
Clin Auton Res 1995;5
(3)
139- 143
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 15.Dworkin
HJLawrie
CBohdiewicz
PLerner
AM Abnormal left ventricular myocardial dynamics in eleven patients with chronic fatigue syndrome.
Clin Nucl Med 1994;19
(8)
675- 677
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 16.Lerner
AMZervos
MChang
CH
et al. A small, randomized, placebo-controlled trial of the use of antiviral therapy for patients with chronic fatigue syndrome.
Clin Infect Dis 2001;32
(11)
1657- 1658
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 17.Peckerman
ALaManca
JJDahl
KAChemitiganti
RQureishi
BNatelson
BH Abnormal impedance cardiography predicts symptom severity in chronic fatigue syndrome.
Am J Med Sci 2003;326
(2)
55- 60
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 18.Peckerman
ALaManca
JJSmith
SL
et al. Cardiovascular stress responses and their relation to symptoms in Gulf War veterans with fatiguing illness.
Psychosom Med 2000;62
(4)
509- 516
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 19.Choi
JBNelesen
RLoredo
JS
et al. Sleepiness in obstructive sleep apnea: a harbinger of impaired cardiac function?
Sleep 2006;29
(12)
1531- 1536
PubMedGoogle Scholar 20.McNair
DM Profile of Mood States (POMS). San Diego, CA Educational and Industrial Testing Service1992;
21.Bardwell
WAAncoli-Israel
SDimsdale
JE Comparison of the effects of depressive symptoms and apnea severity on fatigue in patients with obstructive sleep apnea: a replication study.
J Affect Disord 2007;97
(1-3)
181- 186
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 22.Nagel
JHShyu
LYReddy
SPHurwitz
BEMcCabe
PMSchneiderman
N New signal processing techniques for improved precision of noninvasive impedance cardiography.
Ann Biomed Eng 1989;17
(5)
517- 534
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 23.Kubicek
WGKarnegis
JNPatterson
RPWitsoe
DAMattson
RH Development and evaluation of an impedance cardiac output system.
Aerosp Med 1966;37
(12)
1208- 1212
PubMedGoogle Scholar 24.Nelesen
RAShaw
RZiegler
MGDimsdale
JE Impedance cardiography-derived hemodynamic responses during baroreceptor testing with amyl nitrite and phenylephrine: a validity and reliability study.
Psychophysiology 1999;36
(1)
105- 108
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 25.Cook
DBNagelkirk
PRPeckerman
APoluri
ALamanca
JJNatelson
BH Perceived exertion in fatiguing illness: Gulf War veterans with chronic fatigue syndrome.
Med Sci Sports Exerc 2003;35
(4)
569- 574
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 26.Cook
DBNagelkirk
PRPeckerman
APoluri
ALamanca
JJNatelson
BH Perceived exertion in fatiguing illness: civilians with chronic fatigue syndrome.
Med Sci Sports Exerc 2003;35
(4)
563- 568
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 27.Nagelkirk
PRCook
DBPeckerman
A
et al. Aerobic capacity of Gulf War veterans with chronic fatigue syndrome.
Mil Med 2003;168
(9)
750- 755
PubMedGoogle Scholar 28.Nelson
JJNatelson
BHPeckerman
A
et al. Medical follow-up of Persian Gulf War Veterans with severe medically unexplained fatigue: a preliminary study.
Mil Med 2001;166
(12)
1107- 1109
PubMedGoogle Scholar 29.Sherwood
AAllen
MTFahrenberg
JKelsey
RMLovallo
WRvan Doornen
LJ Methodological guidelines for impedance cardiography.
Psychophysiology 1990;27
(1)
1- 23
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 31.Stein
KDJacobsen
PBBlanchard
CMThors
C Further validation of the multidimensional fatigue symptom inventory-short form.
J Pain Symptom Manage 2004;27
(1)
14- 23
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 32.Bardwell
WABurke
SCThomas
KSCarter
CWeingart
KDimsdale
JE Fatigue varies by social class in African Americans but not Caucasian Americans.
Int J Behav Med 2006;13
(3)
252- 258
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 33.Bardwell
WAMoore
PAncoli-Israel
SDimsdale
JE Fatigue in obstructive sleep apnea: driven by depressive symptoms instead of apnea severity?
Am J Psychiatry 2003;160
(2)
350- 355
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 34.Thomas
KSBardwell
WAAncoli-Israel
SDimsdale
JE The toll of ethnic discrimination on sleep architecture and fatigue.
Health Psychol 2006;25
(5)
635- 642
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 36.Puetz
TWO'Connor
PJDishman
RK Effects of chronic exercise on feelings of energy and fatigue: a quantitative synthesis.
Psychol Bull 2006;132
(6)
866- 876
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref