[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Box plots of unadjusted daily verified attack rates at 2 months and 1 year. Geometric mean attack rates are indicated near the center of each box (plus sign). Extremes of the box delineate the 25th and 75th percentile values. Error bars represent 95% of the data. B indicates temperature biofeedback; Bc, control (electromyographic) biofeedback; N, nifedipine; and Np, placebo.

Box plots of unadjusted daily verified attack rates at 2 months and 1 year. Geometric mean attack rates are indicated near the center of each box (plus sign). Extremes of the box delineate the 25th and 75th percentile values. Error bars represent 95% of the data. B indicates temperature biofeedback; Bc, control (electromyographic) biofeedback; N, nifedipine; and Np, placebo.

Table 1. 
Baseline Characteristics*
Baseline Characteristics*
Table 2. 
Numbers of Patients Screened, Randomized, Receiving Treatment, and Providing End Point Data*
Numbers of Patients Screened, Randomized, Receiving Treatment, and Providing End Point Data*
Table 3. 
Primary and Secondary Raynaud Phenomenon (RP) Attack Frequency Outcome Measures*
Primary and Secondary Raynaud Phenomenon (RP) Attack Frequency Outcome Measures*
Table 4. 
Clinical Ratings of Improvement in Raynaud Phenomenon (RP) at 1 Year*
Clinical Ratings of Improvement in Raynaud Phenomenon (RP) at 1 Year*
1.
LeRoy  ECMedsger  TA  Jr Raynaud's phenomenon: a proposal for classification.  Clin Exp Rheumatol. 1992;10485- 488Google Scholar
2.
Maricq  HRCarpentier  PHWeinrich  MC  et al.  Geographic variation in the prevalence of Raynaud's phenomenon: Charleston, SC, USA vs.Tarentaise, Savoie, France.  J Rheumatol. 1993;2070- 76Google Scholar
3.
Creager  MAHalperin  JLCoffman  JD Vasospastic diseases. Loscalzo  JCreager  MADzau  VJeds. Vascular Medicine Boston, Mass Little Brown & Co Inc1992;975- 1010Google Scholar
4.
Sarkozi  JBookman  AAMahon  WRamsay  CDetsky  ASKeystone  EC Nifedipine in the treatment of idiopathic Raynaud's syndrome.  J Rheumatol. 1986;13331- 336Google Scholar
5.
Gjorup  TKelbaek  HHartling  OJNielsen  SL Controlled double-blind trial of the clinical effect of nifedipine in the treatment of idiopathic Raynaud's phenomenon.  Am Heart J. 1986;111742- 745Google ScholarCrossref
6.
Corbin  DOWood  DAMacintyre  CCHousley  E A randomized double-blind cross-over trial of nifedipine in the treatment of primary Raynaud's phenomenon.  Eur Heart J. 1986;7165- 170Google Scholar
7.
Rademaker  MCooke  EDAlmond  NE  et al.  Comparison of intravenous infusions of iloprost and oral nifedipine in treatment of Raynaud's phenomenon in patients with systemic sclerosis: a double blind randomised study.  BMJ. 1989;298561- 564Google ScholarCrossref
8.
Kallenberg  CGWouda  AAKuitert  JJTijssen  JWesseling  H Nifedipine in Raynaud's phenomenon: relationship between immediate, short-term and long-term effects.  J Rheumatol. 1987;14284- 290Google Scholar
9.
Ettinger  WHWise  RASchaffhauser  DWigley  FM Controlled double-blind trial of dazoxiben and nifedipine in the treatment of Raynaud's phenomenon.  Am J Med. 1984;77451- 456Google ScholarCrossref
10.
Rodeheffer  RJRommer  JAWigley  FSmith  CR Controlled double-blind trial of nifedipine in the treatment of Raynaud's phenomenon.  N Engl J Med. 1983;308880- 883Google ScholarCrossref
11.
Gjorup  THartling  OJKelbaek  HNielsen  SL Controlled double blind trial of nisoldipine in the treatment of idiopathic Raynaud's phenomenon.  Eur J Clin Pharmacol. 1986;31387- 389Google ScholarCrossref
12.
Freedman  RR Physiological mechanisms of temperature biofeedback.  Biofeedback Self Regul. 1991;1695- 115Google ScholarCrossref
13.
Jobe  JBSampson  JBRoberts  DEBeetham  WP  Jr Induced vasodilation as treatment for Raynaud's disease.  Ann Intern Med. 1982;97706- 709Google ScholarCrossref
14.
Appiah  RHiller  SCaspary  LAlexander  KCreutzig  A Treatment of primary Raynaud's syndrome with traditional Chinese acupuncture.  J Intern Med. 1997;241119- 124Google ScholarCrossref
15.
Surwit  RS Biofeedback: a possible treatment for Raynaud's disease.  Semin Psychiatry. 1973;5483- 490Google Scholar
16.
Jacobson  AMHackett  TPSurman  OSSilverberg  EL Raynaud phenomenon: treatment with hypnotic and operant technique.  JAMA. 1973;225739- 740Google ScholarCrossref
17.
Blanchard  EBHaynes  M Biofeedback treatment of a case of Raynaud's disease.  J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 1975;6230- 234Google ScholarCrossref
18.
Shapiro  DSchwartz  GE Biofeedback and visceral learning: clinical applications.  Semin Psychiatry. 1972;4171- 184Google Scholar
19.
Jacobson  AMManschreck  TCSilverberg  E Behavioral treatment for Raynaud's disease: a comparative study with long-term follow-up.  Am J Psychiatry. 1979;136844- 846Google Scholar
20.
Surwit  RSPilon  RNFenton  CH Behavioral treatment of Raynaud's disease.  J Behav Med. 1978;1323- 335Google ScholarCrossref
21.
Keefe  FJSurwit  RSPilon  RN A 1-year follow-up of Raynaud's patients treated with behavioral therapy techniques.  J Behav Med. 1979;2385- 391Google ScholarCrossref
22.
Keefe  FGardner  E Learned control of skin temperature: effects of short- and long-term biofeedback training.  Behav Ther. 1979;10202- 210Google ScholarCrossref
23.
Freedman  RRIanni  P Self-control of digital temperature: physiological factors and transfer effects.  Psychophysiology. 1983;20682- 689Google ScholarCrossref
24.
Grabert  JCBregman  NJMcAllister  HA Skin temperature regulation: the effects of suggestion and feedback.  Int J Neurosci. 1980;10217- 221Google ScholarCrossref
25.
Keefe  FJ Biofeedback vs. instructional control of skin temperature.  J Behav Med. 1978;1383- 390Google ScholarCrossref
26.
Stoffer  GRJensen  JANesset  BL Effects of contingent versus yoked temperature feedback on voluntary temperature control and cold stress tolerance.  Biofeedback Self Regul. 1979;451- 61Google ScholarCrossref
27.
Freedman  RRIanni  PWenig  P Behavioral treatment of Raynaud's disease.  J Consult Clin Psychol. 1983;51539- 549Google ScholarCrossref
28.
Stambrook  MHamel  ERCarter  SA Training to vasodilate in a cooling environment: a valid treatment for Raynaud's phenomenon?  Biofeedback Self Regul. 1988;139- 23Google ScholarCrossref
29.
King  NJMontgomery  RB Biofeedback-induced control of human peripheral temperature: a critical review of the literature.  Psych Bull. 1980;88738- 752Google ScholarCrossref
30.
Rose  GDCarlson  JG The behavioral treatment of Raynaud's disease: a review.  Biofeedback Self Regul. 1987;12257- 272Google ScholarCrossref
31.
Maricq  HRCarpentier  PHWeinrich  MC  et al.  Geographic variation in the prevalence of Raynaud's phenomenon: a 5 region comparison.  J Rheumatol. 1997;24879- 889Google Scholar
32.
Maricq  HR Capillaroscopy in Raynaud's syndrome.  Vasc Med Rev. 1992;33- 20Google Scholar
33.
Ware  JESnow  KKKosinski  MGandek  B SF-36 Health Survey: Manual and Interpretation Guide.  Boston, Mass Nimrod Press1993;
34.
Cohen  SHoberman  HM Positive events and social supports as buffers of life change stress.  J Appl Soc Psychol. 1983;1399- 125Google ScholarCrossref
35.
Friou  GJ Clinical application of lupus serum-nucleoprotein reaction using fluorescent antibody technique [abstract].  J Clin Invest. 1957;36890Google Scholar
36.
Thompson  BGeller  NHunsberger  S  et al.  Designing clinical trials with behavioral and pharmacologic interventions: the Raynaud's Treatment Study.  Control Clin Trials. 1999;2052- 63Google ScholarCrossref
37.
Kotz  SJohnson  NLRead  CB Encyclopedia of Statistical Sciences.  New York, NY John Wiley & Sons Inc1983;397- 400
38.
Rubin  D Multiple imputation after 18+ years.  J Am Stat Assoc. 1996;91473- 489Google ScholarCrossref
39.
SAS Institute Inc, SAS Language: Reference, Version 6.  Cary, NC SAS Institute Inc1990;
40.
Wigley  FM Raynaud's phenomenon. Fortuin  NJed. Current Therapy in Cardiovascular Disease Philadelphia, Pa BC Decker Inc1987;319- 322Google Scholar
41.
Adee  AC Managing Raynaud's phenomenon: a practical approach.  Am Fam Physician. 1993;47823- 829Google Scholar
42.
Ferri  CCecchetti  RCini  G  et al.  Slow-releasing nicardipine in the treatment of Raynaud's phenomena without underlying diseases.  Clin Rheumatol. 1992;1176- 80Google ScholarCrossref
43.
Rhedda  AMcCans  JWillan  ARFord  PM A double blind placebo controlled crossover randomized trial of diltiazem in Raynaud's phenomenon.  J Rheumatol. 1985;12724- 727Google Scholar
44.
Leppert  JJonasson  TNilsson  HRingqvist  I The effect of isradipine, a new calcium-channel antagonist, in patients with primary Raynaud's phenomenon: a single-blind dose-response study.  Cardiovasc Drugs Ther. 1989;3397- 401Google ScholarCrossref
45.
Freedman  RRKeegan  DMigaly  PGalloway  MMayes  M Plasma catecholamines during behavioral treatments for Raynaud's disease.  Psychosom Med. 1991;53433- 439Google ScholarCrossref
46.
Freedman  RRSabharwal  SIanni  PDesai  NWenig  PMayes  M Non-neural beta-adrenergic vasodilating mechanism in temperature biofeedback.  Psychosom Med. 1988;50394- 401Google ScholarCrossref
47.
Freedman  RR Quantitative measurements of finger blood flow during behavioral treatments for Raynaud's disease.  Psychophysiology. 1989;26437- 441Google ScholarCrossref
48.
Otis  JDRasey  HWVrochopoulos  SWincze  JPAndrasik  F Temperature acquisition as a function of the computer-based biofeedback system utilized: an exploratory analysis.  Biofeedback Self Regul. 1995;20185- 190Google ScholarCrossref
49.
Freedman  RRIanni  PWenig  P Behavioral treatment of Raynaud's disease: long-term follow-up.  J Consult Clin Psychol. 1985;53136Google ScholarCrossref
Original Investigation
April 24, 2000

Comparison of Sustained-Release Nifedipine and Temperature Biofeedback for Treatment of Primary Raynaud Phenomenon: Results From a Randomized Clinical Trial With 1-Year Follow-up

Raynaud's Treatment Study Investigators
Author Affiliations

This work was supported by contracts HC-25119, HC-25120, HC-25121, HC-25122, HC-25123, and HC-25127 from the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, Md.

Arch Intern Med. 2000;160(8):1101-1108. doi:10.1001/archinte.160.8.1101
Abstract

Background  The efficacy and safety of sustained-release nifedipine for the treatment of primary Raynaud phenomenon (RP) has not previously been demonstrated by a randomized, controlled trial. Temperature biofeedback has been studied in patients with primary RP but not in a large multicenter controlled trial or compared with nifedipine therapy.

Objective  To evaluate and compare the effectiveness of sustained-release nifedipine and temperature biofeedback for the treatment of primary RP.

Participants and Methods  This is a randomized, controlled clinical trial, double-masked for drug and placebo but not masked for temperature and control biofeedback. It included 313 persons with primary RP as defined by medical history, physical examination findings, normal nailfold capillaries, and a history of 2 or more attacks per day during the previous cold season. Participants were randomized to 1 of 4 treatment groups: (1) sustained-release nifedipine, (2) pill placebo, (3) temperature biofeedback, or (4) control (electromyographic) biofeedback. The primary outcome measure was self-reported, color chart–verified RP attacks during 1 winter month approximately 1 year after initiation of treatment. Secondary outcome measures included verified attacks at 2 months, all attacks at 2 months and 1 year, and quality of life.

Results  Nifedipine-treated participants showed a 66% reduction in verified attacks compared with placebo recipients (P<.001); temperature biofeedback training did not reduce attacks significantly compared with control biofeedback (P=.37). Comparison of nifedipine and temperature biofeedback treatments favored nifedipine use (P=.08); similar results were obtained for the secondary end points. Adverse effects resulted in discontinuation of nifedipine treatment in 15% of participants.

Conclusions  Temperature biofeedback is not better than its control treatment and is inferior to sustained-release nifedipine for treating primary RP, whereas sustained-release nifedipine is a safe and effective treatment for this disease.

EPISODIC ATTACKS of Raynaud phenomenon (RP)1 occur primarily in the fingers, triggered by cold exposure or emotional stress. Their frequency can vary, and severity can be mild to temporarily disabling. The disorder affects 4% to 20% of the population, with higher prevalence in women.2 Raynaud phenomenon is customarily divided into 2 categories: primary RP (idiopathic RP) and secondary RP, which is related to connective tissue diseases, arterial occlusive disease, certain neurologic disorders, blood dyscrasias, trauma, drugs and toxins, and other miscellaneous disorders.3 Raynaud phenomenon is considered primary if underlying causal factors have been ruled out.

Avoidance of cold exposure often provides sufficient protection against attacks. In other cases, administration of vasodilating drugs is necessary. Immediate-release nifedipine therapy has been shown to reduce the frequency and severity of attacks in patients with primary RP.4-8 However, adverse effects (tachycardia, headache, flushing, dizziness, and edema) occur in 30% to 100% of patients, many of whom discontinue treatment.4-6,9-11 Although sustained-release nifedipine is being used by many physicians to treat RP, its efficacy and adverse effects have not been demonstrated in this population.

Of the nonpharmacological treatments for RP, temperature biofeedback has been one of the most studied.12-21 A degree of voluntary control of peripheral blood flow and skin temperature regulation is possible in healthy subjects through temperature biofeedback.22-24 Individuals with primary RP have been reported to acquire this ability and to reduce the frequency of attacks,12,15,25-29 especially when exposure to cold stressors is included in training.27,29,30 This approach might be desirable for patients with primary RP who do not tolerate vasodilators or who prefer nondrug treatments. However, its comparability to nifedipine treatment has not been investigated, and its efficacy has been evaluated only in small, single-center trials with patients likely to be interested in biofeedback. This article presents results of a randomized clinical trial, the Raynaud's Treatment Study (RTS), comparing the effectiveness of sustained-release nifedipine and temperature biofeedback for the treatment of primary RP.

Participants and methods
Participants

The 5 participating sites were in different geographic areas and climates. Participants were recruited from local clinics and by advertising. Those who passed a telephone interview were invited for detailed examination. Diagnosis of RP involved a structured interview assisted by color charts31 to improve the reliability of the diagnosis that is usually based on the patient's history alone and to make a differential diagnosis between RP and acrocyanosis. Only those who reported at least 2 attacks on an average day during the previous cold season were enrolled. The medical evaluation (history, physical examination, and appropriate laboratory tests) focused on ruling out patients with secondary RP. Special attention was given to patients with possible early scleroderma (systemic sclerosis) in whom RP may precede by years the diagnosis of this connective tissue disease. To exclude such patients, nailfold capillary microscopy32 was performed to detect the scleroderma capillary pattern (an early marker of scleroderma), and antinuclear antibody titers were required to be 1:320 or less. Participants were eligible if they completed at least 75% of the required 1-month baseline record of attacks and had no contraindications to the study treatments. Of 313 participants, 113 were enrolled during the cold season months of November through February in 1993 and 1994 (cohort 1) and 200 were enrolled during the 1994 and 1995 cold season months (cohort 2).

Treatments

Participants were randomly assigned to 1 of 4 treatment groups: sustained-release nifedipine (N) (Procardia XL; Pfizer Labs, New York, NY), matching pill placebo (Np), temperature biofeedback (B), or control (frontalis muscle surface electromyographic [EMG]) biofeedback (Bc). The RTS Coordinating Center generated a set of allocations for each clinical center, with block size varying randomly so that the number of patients allocated to each of the 4 treatment arms could be balanced over time. Whenever a patient became eligible and gave consent, clinical center staff obtained a treatment assignment from the Coordinating Center using the Automated Telephone Randomization System. Treatment was begun immediately after the treatment assignment was received. To safeguard against therapists' expectation effects, staff providing treatment did not collect any outcome data.

Drug Treatment

Nifedipine and placebo were administered in a double-blind manner. Participants were scheduled to be seen weekly for 5 visits by a physician masked to treatment assignment. Treatment began with 30 mg of nifedipine (or matching placebo) daily. If no adverse reactions occurred in the first week, pill use was increased to 2 per day (60 mg). Based on tolerance, dosage was adjusted to 2, 1, or 0 pills per day during the 4 subsequent weekly visits, then continued for 1 year. Adherence was recorded as percentage of pills returned at quarterly clinic visits.

Biofeedback Treatment

For patients and therapists to understand the biofeedback methods and goals, neither temperature biofeedback nor its control could be masked. Participants were asked to attend ten 1-hour sessions over a 5- to 10-week period. Temperature and control feedback sessions were similar. Thermistors and EMG sensors were attached and calibrated with participants seated in a comfortable easy chair in a temperature-controlled room. After resting for 16 minutes to establish a finger temperature or EMG baseline, 16 minutes of feedback training was given during which participants were asked to alter finger temperature (or frontalis muscle EMG) while observing the output of a temperature (or EMG) display device. Progress during the session was discussed with the therapist. Homework included daily 10-minute practice sessions without instrumentation and "applied practice," in which participants were to practice their technique in situations they thought were associated with a high probability of RP attacks. Four booster sessions were scheduled the next fall for individuals who did not meet a predefined criterion of successful learning or who missed at least half of the spring sessions. Minimum biofeedback training was completion of 6 winter and spring sessions or at least 2 assigned fall booster sessions. The RTS temperature biofeedback protocol was tested in about 10 healthy individuals at each RTS clinic (N=46).

For participants in cohort 1, therapists remained silent during the feedback phase so as not to be distracting. Verbal coaching was limited to the postfeedback phase. For participants in cohort 2, coaching during the feedback phase was allowed because successful learning in the first cohort was less than expected.

Patients were told during informed consent that it was not known whether any of the treatments were superior. During the first therapy session, patients undergoing biofeedback were given the rationale for their assigned treatment and were told that many patients experience a benefit.

End points

End points were assessed 2 months (first winter and spring) and 1 year (second winter) after initiating treatment. Data on adverse effects and symptom severity were collected quarterly. A coordinating center analyzed all data. The study design and informed consent procedures were approved by an independent RTS Data and Safety Monitoring Board and by the institutional review boards of all centers.

Primary End Point

The number of verified attacks (defined below) reported during the 4 weeks immediately after the 1-year assessment constituted the primary end point. All other data collected were secondary end points. Attacks of RP were recorded on attack cards that participants were instructed to keep with them at all times, together with an 8.25 × 13.33-cm (3¼ × 5¼ in) plastic card showing 9 color photographs of hands, some illustrating RP attacks and others showing color changes not characteristic of RP. Each photograph was identified by a letter, without identifying which images illustrated RP attacks. To be counted as a verified attack, an attack card entry had to (1) include a letter code that matched one of the photographs demonstrating true RP and (2) have occurred at least 30 minutes after a previously recorded entry.

Secondary End Points

These included verified attacks reported during the 4 weeks immediately after the 2-month assessment, all attacks (ie, all recorded attacks, whether or not they met verified attack criteria) reported in connection with the 2-month and 1-year assessments, and attacks reported in a daily diary at the 2-month and 1-year assessments. The daily diary was completed every evening for 4 weeks before the baseline, 2-month, and 1-year visits to summarize the number, severity, and duration of attacks and the coping strategies undertaken by the patient. Separate clinical rating assessment forms were completed by participants and physicians at quarterly visits. Participants estimated (1) the number of RP attacks they had during an average week, (2) the severity of their RP on a 4-point scale, (3) the impact of RP on their lives, (4) their improvement compared with baseline, and (5) their general health. Physicians assessed only items 2 through 5.

The Short Form Health Survey,33 modified slightly to address RP rather than general health, was used as the main quality-of-life measure. Physical symptom reports were obtained using the Cohen-Hoberman Inventory of Physical Symptoms,34 modified to exclude clearly psychological symptoms (eg, "felt nervous"). Items relevant to RP or potential adverse effects of nifedipine were added. Participants indicated how much each symptom bothered them during the past 4 weeks.

Antinuclear antibody titers, using the Hep-2 cell line in a microscopically interpreted immunofluorescent assay,35 were determined by a central laboratory. Physicians who performed the nail capillaroscopy were trained and certified to conform to a uniform standard.

Statistical methods

The data analysis plan was specified in advance.36 A global F test determined the simultaneous probability that (1) the outcome for the temperature biofeedback group (B) differed from that of the control biofeedback group (Bc) or (2) the outcome for the nifedipine group (N) differed from that of the placebo group (Np). If this global F test was significant at P<.05, individual t tests (ie, B vs Bc and N vs Np) were performed to assess the treatment-control differences in the behavioral and drug treatment arms, respectively. A final test determined whether the treatment vs control group differences for the drug and behavioral treatments (ie, [B − Bc] vs [N − Np]) were significantly different. This contrast was chosen because it compares the effect of each treatment free from nonspecific effects that might arise from unique aspects of the 2 different treatment modalities, one behavioral and the other pharmacological.36 Attack rates of RP were transformed logarithmically: ln[(No. of attacks + 0.5)/(No. of days)] to stabilize the variance. Summary data for attack rates are presented in the original units, yielding geometric means.37 The sample size was set at 300 so that with α=.05 for the global F test, power would be 0.8 to detect a 42% reduction in attack rate (or a change of 0.545 units on the logarithmic scale, with an SD of 1.00) in the drug or behavioral arm compared with the respective control arms. The percentage reduction corresponds to the noncentrality parameter in the analysis of the log-attack rates. As such, the percentage reduction applies to any starting rate. During design, we anticipated an average baseline attack rate of 2 per day. A 42% reduction would result in 1.16 attacks per day. If the baseline attack rate were 1.0 per day, a 42% reduction would correspond to a final attack rate of 0.58 attacks per day; if the baseline rate were 0.6 per day, a 42% reduction would yield 0.34 attacks per day.

Statistical analyses followed an intention-to-treat principle and corrected for possible bias caused by differences in missing data among groups by using a regression equation based on baseline variables to impute values for the patient with missing data (Bruce Thompson, PhD, Nancy Geller, PhD, and Sally Hunsberger, PhD, et al, unpublished manuscript, September 1999). Estimation proceeds using weighted least squares, but reweighting analyses for missing data variance estimates, are larger than those obtained using standard weighted least squares. Results based on the new reweighting analyses for missing data procedure were almost identical to those based on the more widely used multiple imputation method.38

The primary and secondary end point results were adjusted using covariates for baseline attack rate and clinical center. Analyses were performed using SAS routines.39

Results
Patient characteristics and compliance

Seventy percent of participants were women, 95% were white, and mean age was 45 years (Table 1). At baseline, participants assessed their RP symptoms as mild (22%), moderate (50%), or severe (28%). During baseline clinical examinations, physicians assessed participants' RP symptoms as mild (25%), moderate (60%), or severe (15%). The mean attack rate for the previous cold season, reported by participants at the screening visit, was 3.7 per day, with a minimum of 2.0 per day mandated by eligibility criteria. By comparison, the arithmetic mean for all attacks based on attack card entries at baseline was 1.0 per day (Table 1). Self-rated severity of RP symptoms at baseline had a correlation of 0.43 with baseline log-verified attack rate. At baseline, verified attack rates between the groups were similar (P=.47), as were other attributes listed in Table 1.

The median time from randomization to the "1-year" assessment was 13.5 months. For the primary end point evaluation, 85% of biofeedback and 83% of medication participants completed attack cards (for an average of 26.3 of 28.0 possible days) (Table 2). Most patients (93%) completed their attack cards in January or February, the rest in March or April. For the secondary end point evaluation (2-month follow-up), 68% of biofeedback and 82% of medication participants completed attack cards (P=.02) (Table 2). Data from daily diaries were almost identical to "all attack" data collected through attack cards, and will not be presented further.

The amount of follow-up data differs by end point. Of 313 participants, 83 (26%) formally requested to discontinue study participation, 67 within the first 5 weeks of treatment (Table 2). In response to efforts aimed at obtaining as much data as possible, many participants agreed to perform end point assessments even though they were no longer receiving study treatment.

Of 81 participants assigned to the temperature biofeedback group, 53 (65%) completed all 10 training sessions compared with 59 (80%) of those in the EMG biofeedback group (P=.048). All 4 booster sessions were completed by 23 (39%) of 59 temperature and 14 (45%) of 31 EMG biofeedback participants eligible. In all, 80% of temperature and 86% of EMG biofeedback participants completed the minimum number of training sessions as defined in the "Participants and Methods" section.

Of participants assigned to the nifedipine group, 73% completed the 5 required initial treatment visits compared with 75% of those assigned to the placebo group (Table 2). Pill counts of vials returned for refills during clinic visits showed that 82% of nifedipine pills and 83% of placebo pills were taken. After completing 5 initial medication visits, the distribution of pill usage as indicated at the patient's final visit was as follows: 57% of nifedipine participants continued taking the full 60-mg dose, 18% took 30 mg, and 17% took no pills. Corresponding values for placebo participants were 65%, 7%, and 16%, respectively. Data from 8% of nifedipine and 11% of placebo participants were missing. Adverse effects requiring discontinuation of treatment occurred in 14% of participants taking nifedipine and 9% taking placebo.

Primary end point

The nifedipine group had the lowest verified attack rate at 1 year, both unadjusted (Figure 1) and adjusted for baseline verified attack and clinical center (Table 3). The global F test of the null hypothesis that both of the differences in RP attack rates (ie, between [1] temperature biofeedback and EMG biofeedback and [2] nifedipine and placebo) are zero for the primary end point at 1 year was significant (P=.002). Specifically, the 66% reduction in the number of verified attacks observed in the nifedipine group compared with the placebo group was significant (P<.001). The 32% reduction in attacks in temperature biofeedback participants compared with EMG biofeedback participants was not significant (P=.37). The comparison of temperature biofeedback and nifedipine treatments (ie, [B − Bc] − [N − Np]) showed 56% fewer attacks in the nifedipine group relative to the temperature biofeedback group, a trend toward statistical significance (P=.08).

Secondary end points

The planned comparison ([B − Bc] − [N − Np]) between temperature biofeedback and nifedipine treatment for all attacks shows an advantage for nifedipine treatment at 1 year (P=.003) and a smaller difference at 2 months (P=.14). For verified attacks at 2 months, nifedipine treatment showed a 58% reduction relative to temperature biofeedback treatment (P=.03). Reductions in attack rates for temperature biofeedback treatment relative to control for the 4 outcomes in Table 3 are from 7% to 32% (smallest P=.36). Significantly more participants (P=.001) and physicians (P=.006) judged that nifedipine treatment had caused improvement than did those judging that biofeedback treatment had done so (Table 4). However, this difference might be attributed in part to the substantially higher incidence of reported improvement in the biofeedback control group than in the nifedipine control group.

Adverse effects and quality of life

Participants taking nifedipine had significantly higher incidences of edema (24%) and flushing (8%) than did those in the other 3 groups (0%) (P<.001), and 2 participants taking nifedipine reported tachycardia. Thirteen (17%) of 77 participants assigned to nifedipine reported headache compared with 11% of the placebo group (P=.10) and only 1 participant undergoing biofeedback treatment. The incidence of dizziness was slightly lower in the nifedipine group (7%) than in the placebo group (8%).

Scores from the RP-specific Short Form Health Survey showed that treatment had little effect on quality of life. The only significant difference was that participants assigned to nifedipine treatment rated their change in RP symptoms as more improved than did participants assigned to the placebo (P<.001) or temperature biofeedback (P=.01) groups at the 2-month and 1-year assessments. There was a trend (P=.02) for those taking nifedipine to report less severe changes in RP-related pain than those taking placebo.

Subgroup analyses

The outcome for the primary end point was not different between subgroups based on (1) whether randomization was to the preferred treatment, (2) number of baseline attacks, (3) sex, (4) enrollment in the first or second cohort, and (5) whether biofeedback control was learned to criterion. All but the last of these subgroup analyses were specified beforehand in the RTS protocol.

Finger temperature

Only 35% of those in the temperature biofeedback group satisfied the criterion for "successful learning." The results were equivalent for cohort 1 (32%) and cohort 2 (36%). By contrast, 31 (67%) of the 46 healthy individuals taught the temperature biofeedback protocol achieved "successful learning" (P<.001).

Comment

To our knowledge, this is the first multicenter study comparing pharmacological and behavioral treatments and the first long-term, randomized, controlled trial of a sustained-release calcium channel blocker to treat primary RP. Few therapeutic trials have followed RP for a period spanning 2 winter seasons. We found that participants taking nifedipine reported a greater than 60% reduction in RP attacks at the end of the first and the second winter after beginning treatment compared with those taking placebo. In contrast, participants treated with finger temperature biofeedback did not report a significant reduction in RP attacks compared with the control biofeedback procedure.

Calcium channel blockers are the vasodilating drugs used most often for treating RP. Immediate-release nifedipine has been studied most,40,41 but others42-44 have also been investigated. Although studies using immediate-release nifedipine given 3 times daily also reported about 50% fewer RP attacks and significantly reduced severity,4-6 a trend toward diminished effectiveness over time has been suggested.4 In our study, sustained-release nifedipine treatment has a comparable initial effect but sustained over 1 year and with a lower incidence of adverse effects than immediate-release nifedipine treatment.4-6,9-11 Specifically, in our study, 24%, 8%, and 3% of participants taking sustained-release nifedipine reported edema, flushing, and tachycardia, respectively, compared with 56%, 30%, and 23%, respectively, in studies4,5 of immediate-release nifedipine treatment. Although only 57% of participants taking nifedipine continued taking the full 60-mg dose after 5 initial medication visits, this level of compliance clearly was high enough to produce significant remission of symptoms.

Results of laboratory studies22,25-27,45-47 of temperature biofeedback in control subjects show that temperature biofeedback with instructions about warming can produce small but significant increases in skin temperature and blood flow, probably by promoting peripheral vasodilation.12,45 Once learned, hand warming can be produced without feedback,23,26 generalized to locations outside the laboratory,23 and retained over time,12,25,26 suggesting a robust effect that might be used to control RP symptoms. Published studies20,27,48 provided initial support for this hypothesis.

We considered several possibilities for the lack of benefit from temperature biofeedback observed in the RTS. One is possibly inadequate training or acquisition of the biofeedback response. In this clinical trial, only 35% of participants undergoing biofeedback met study criteria for increasing finger temperature. Increased interaction between biofeedback therapists and patients in cohort 2, as the therapists suggested, did not increase the proportion of patients reaching criterion. However, when we administered the same temperature biofeedback protocol to 46 healthy persons in the RTS clinics, they performed as healthy individuals22,23 and as RP patients25,49 in other studies. This indicates that the RTS biofeedback intervention was adequate. The fact that RP patients as diagnosed in the RTS did not acquire the temperature response as healthy individuals do may be relevant for the further study of the nature of primary RP.

Although compliance with treatment was not ideal in this study, it speaks to the question of effectiveness, namely, the extent to which these interventions can be implemented in clinical practice. Comparing those who completed 6 or more of their biofeedback sessions with those completing fewer sessions did not demonstrate any differences in outcome. Another secondary analysis showed that participants who learned to raise finger temperature through biofeedback did not report fewer attacks than did participants who did not learn. Although the number of participants in these subgroups is small, these findings suggest that the outcome is more likely explained by a lack of efficacy than by inadequacy of the biofeedback intervention.

Because the RTS defined and adhered to a rigorous, standardized diagnosis of RP at every site, we might have enrolled participants who differed from those enrolled in previous biofeedback studies, eg, participants with more severe RP or those more difficult to treat using biofeedback. In either case, a relationship should have been seen between successful learning of biofeedback and the number of attacks after treatment. Other factors that could affect treatment outcome also were explored by subgroup analyses, including the potential effect of preference for one or another treatment. No characteristics were found that might identify individuals who benefit from biofeedback treatment.

The disparity between the historical cold weather attack rate reported by RTS participants during screening (3.7/d) and the actual rate for all attacks recorded during baseline data collection (1.0/d) was not surprising. Patients often overestimate symptoms when asked to recall them from memory. The attack rate based on a daily diary or attack card system provides a more accurate estimate of symptom frequency.

In this study, self-reported symptoms were validated using standard color charts. More than 1 method was used to score attacks and their severity. In addition to the RP attack rate, we obtained subjective indications of treatment outcome through estimates of improvement by study participants and their physicians. These data consistently show significant improvement with nifedipine use but not with temperature biofeedback treatment. The increase in patient- and physician-reported improvement in the biofeedback control group compared with the nifedipine control group, which is not seen in the attack rate data, might be due to a greater subjective expectation of success produced by biofeedback intervention, whether temperature or EMG. This finding supports the importance of separate control groups for behavioral and pharmacological interventions and for the more objective recording of verified attacks to serve as the primary end point.

The efficacy and durability of the temperature biofeedback intervention is suggested by previously published work.27,49 Because healthy subjects can learn to raise finger temperature by biofeedback, it is conceivable that improved training methods, methods to improve compliance, changes in instrumentation, or more intensive treatment involving daily biofeedback practice as long as symptoms persist might lead to better outcomes. However, this remains a matter for further investigation.

In conclusion, the present clinical trial demonstrates that temperature biofeedback is not better than EMG biofeedback and is inferior to sustained-release nifedipine for treating primary RP. It also demonstrates the effectiveness of sustained-release nifedipine for treating primary RP. Use of this drug was well accepted, had fewer adverse effects than immediate-release formulations, and reduced RP attack frequency by more than half.

Accepted for publication July 19, 1999.

The Procardia XL used in this study was donated by Pfizer Labs, New York, NY. High-quality reproductions of the original set of copyrighted color photographs for Raynaud phenomenon diagnosis and for use by patients were provided to participating clinical centers by Hildegard R. Maricq, MD.

RTS Clinical CentersJohns Hopkins University Medical Center, Baltimore, Md: Fredrick M. Wigley, MD (principal investigator); Robert Wise, MD; Jennifer Haythornthwaite, PhD; and Cynthia Rand, PhD; Medical University of South Carolina, Charleston: Edwin A. Smith, MD (principal investigator); Hildegard R. Maricq, MD; Susan Middaugh, PhD; Richard M. Silver, MD; Ivo Valter, MD; Gayle Allenback, BS; June Arnold; Susan Steedman, RPh; Andra Risinger, RN; and Ann King, RN; University of Medicine and Dentistry of New Jersey (UMDNJ)–New Jersey Medical School, Newark: Leonard Bielory, MD (principal investigator); Virginia Attanasio, PhD; June Tarantola, LPN; and Ligaya Centeno, MD; University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, Pa: Rolf G. Jacob, MD (principal investigator); Thomas Medsger, Jr, MD; Richard Jennings, PhD; Michael Scheier, PhD; Charles Vaughan, PhD; Mary Chester Wasko, MD; Virginia D. Steen, MD; Jacqueline Dunbar-Jacob, PhD, RN; and Lisa Tamres; and Wayne State University, Detroit, Mich: Robert R. Freedman, PhD (principal investigator); Maureen D. Mayes, MD; Carol Brejnak; and Carol Tindall, RN.

RTS Central UnitsUMDNJ–New Jersey Medical School (RTS study chair): Leonard Bielory, MD (principal investigator); Clinical Trials and Surveys Corp, Baltimore, Md (clinical coordinating center): Bruce Thompson, PhD (principal investigator); Margaret Frederick, PhD; Martha Canner, MS; Joseph Canner, MHS; and Paul Canner, PhD; US Department of Health and Human Services, Public Health Service, Indian Health Service, Supply Center, Perry Point, Md (drug distribution center): Thomas R. Shaffer; Behavioral Medicine Scientific Research Group, Division of Epidemiology and Clinical Applications, National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute (NHLBI), Bethesda, Md (project office): D. Robin Hill, PhD (project officer); Peter G. Kaufmann, PhD; and Susan Czajkowski, PhD; Office of Biostatistics Research (NHLBI): Nancy Geller, PhD, and Sally Hunsberger, PhD; Division of Extramural Affairs (NHLBI): Cheryl Jennings and William M. Stevens; and the Data and Safety Monitoring Board: Evelyn V. Hess, MD (chair), Division of Immunology, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, Ohio; John A. Corson, PhD, Department of Psychiatry, Dartmouth Medical School, Hanover, NH; Jared B. Jobe, PhD, Behavioral and Social Research, National Institute on Aging, Bethesda, Md; Harmon Smith, PhD, The Divinity School, Duke University, Durham, NC; and Barbara Tilley, PhD, Division of Biostatistics and Research Epidemiology, Henry Ford Hospital, Detroit, Mich.

Reprints: Bruce Thompson, PhD, Clinical Trials and Surveys Corp, 350 W Quadrangle, Village of Cross Keys, Baltimore, MD 21210.

References
1.
LeRoy  ECMedsger  TA  Jr Raynaud's phenomenon: a proposal for classification.  Clin Exp Rheumatol. 1992;10485- 488Google Scholar
2.
Maricq  HRCarpentier  PHWeinrich  MC  et al.  Geographic variation in the prevalence of Raynaud's phenomenon: Charleston, SC, USA vs.Tarentaise, Savoie, France.  J Rheumatol. 1993;2070- 76Google Scholar
3.
Creager  MAHalperin  JLCoffman  JD Vasospastic diseases. Loscalzo  JCreager  MADzau  VJeds. Vascular Medicine Boston, Mass Little Brown & Co Inc1992;975- 1010Google Scholar
4.
Sarkozi  JBookman  AAMahon  WRamsay  CDetsky  ASKeystone  EC Nifedipine in the treatment of idiopathic Raynaud's syndrome.  J Rheumatol. 1986;13331- 336Google Scholar
5.
Gjorup  TKelbaek  HHartling  OJNielsen  SL Controlled double-blind trial of the clinical effect of nifedipine in the treatment of idiopathic Raynaud's phenomenon.  Am Heart J. 1986;111742- 745Google ScholarCrossref
6.
Corbin  DOWood  DAMacintyre  CCHousley  E A randomized double-blind cross-over trial of nifedipine in the treatment of primary Raynaud's phenomenon.  Eur Heart J. 1986;7165- 170Google Scholar
7.
Rademaker  MCooke  EDAlmond  NE  et al.  Comparison of intravenous infusions of iloprost and oral nifedipine in treatment of Raynaud's phenomenon in patients with systemic sclerosis: a double blind randomised study.  BMJ. 1989;298561- 564Google ScholarCrossref
8.
Kallenberg  CGWouda  AAKuitert  JJTijssen  JWesseling  H Nifedipine in Raynaud's phenomenon: relationship between immediate, short-term and long-term effects.  J Rheumatol. 1987;14284- 290Google Scholar
9.
Ettinger  WHWise  RASchaffhauser  DWigley  FM Controlled double-blind trial of dazoxiben and nifedipine in the treatment of Raynaud's phenomenon.  Am J Med. 1984;77451- 456Google ScholarCrossref
10.
Rodeheffer  RJRommer  JAWigley  FSmith  CR Controlled double-blind trial of nifedipine in the treatment of Raynaud's phenomenon.  N Engl J Med. 1983;308880- 883Google ScholarCrossref
11.
Gjorup  THartling  OJKelbaek  HNielsen  SL Controlled double blind trial of nisoldipine in the treatment of idiopathic Raynaud's phenomenon.  Eur J Clin Pharmacol. 1986;31387- 389Google ScholarCrossref
12.
Freedman  RR Physiological mechanisms of temperature biofeedback.  Biofeedback Self Regul. 1991;1695- 115Google ScholarCrossref
13.
Jobe  JBSampson  JBRoberts  DEBeetham  WP  Jr Induced vasodilation as treatment for Raynaud's disease.  Ann Intern Med. 1982;97706- 709Google ScholarCrossref
14.
Appiah  RHiller  SCaspary  LAlexander  KCreutzig  A Treatment of primary Raynaud's syndrome with traditional Chinese acupuncture.  J Intern Med. 1997;241119- 124Google ScholarCrossref
15.
Surwit  RS Biofeedback: a possible treatment for Raynaud's disease.  Semin Psychiatry. 1973;5483- 490Google Scholar
16.
Jacobson  AMHackett  TPSurman  OSSilverberg  EL Raynaud phenomenon: treatment with hypnotic and operant technique.  JAMA. 1973;225739- 740Google ScholarCrossref
17.
Blanchard  EBHaynes  M Biofeedback treatment of a case of Raynaud's disease.  J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 1975;6230- 234Google ScholarCrossref
18.
Shapiro  DSchwartz  GE Biofeedback and visceral learning: clinical applications.  Semin Psychiatry. 1972;4171- 184Google Scholar
19.
Jacobson  AMManschreck  TCSilverberg  E Behavioral treatment for Raynaud's disease: a comparative study with long-term follow-up.  Am J Psychiatry. 1979;136844- 846Google Scholar
20.
Surwit  RSPilon  RNFenton  CH Behavioral treatment of Raynaud's disease.  J Behav Med. 1978;1323- 335Google ScholarCrossref
21.
Keefe  FJSurwit  RSPilon  RN A 1-year follow-up of Raynaud's patients treated with behavioral therapy techniques.  J Behav Med. 1979;2385- 391Google ScholarCrossref
22.
Keefe  FGardner  E Learned control of skin temperature: effects of short- and long-term biofeedback training.  Behav Ther. 1979;10202- 210Google ScholarCrossref
23.
Freedman  RRIanni  P Self-control of digital temperature: physiological factors and transfer effects.  Psychophysiology. 1983;20682- 689Google ScholarCrossref
24.
Grabert  JCBregman  NJMcAllister  HA Skin temperature regulation: the effects of suggestion and feedback.  Int J Neurosci. 1980;10217- 221Google ScholarCrossref
25.
Keefe  FJ Biofeedback vs. instructional control of skin temperature.  J Behav Med. 1978;1383- 390Google ScholarCrossref
26.
Stoffer  GRJensen  JANesset  BL Effects of contingent versus yoked temperature feedback on voluntary temperature control and cold stress tolerance.  Biofeedback Self Regul. 1979;451- 61Google ScholarCrossref
27.
Freedman  RRIanni  PWenig  P Behavioral treatment of Raynaud's disease.  J Consult Clin Psychol. 1983;51539- 549Google ScholarCrossref
28.
Stambrook  MHamel  ERCarter  SA Training to vasodilate in a cooling environment: a valid treatment for Raynaud's phenomenon?  Biofeedback Self Regul. 1988;139- 23Google ScholarCrossref
29.
King  NJMontgomery  RB Biofeedback-induced control of human peripheral temperature: a critical review of the literature.  Psych Bull. 1980;88738- 752Google ScholarCrossref
30.
Rose  GDCarlson  JG The behavioral treatment of Raynaud's disease: a review.  Biofeedback Self Regul. 1987;12257- 272Google ScholarCrossref
31.
Maricq  HRCarpentier  PHWeinrich  MC  et al.  Geographic variation in the prevalence of Raynaud's phenomenon: a 5 region comparison.  J Rheumatol. 1997;24879- 889Google Scholar
32.
Maricq  HR Capillaroscopy in Raynaud's syndrome.  Vasc Med Rev. 1992;33- 20Google Scholar
33.
Ware  JESnow  KKKosinski  MGandek  B SF-36 Health Survey: Manual and Interpretation Guide.  Boston, Mass Nimrod Press1993;
34.
Cohen  SHoberman  HM Positive events and social supports as buffers of life change stress.  J Appl Soc Psychol. 1983;1399- 125Google ScholarCrossref
35.
Friou  GJ Clinical application of lupus serum-nucleoprotein reaction using fluorescent antibody technique [abstract].  J Clin Invest. 1957;36890Google Scholar
36.
Thompson  BGeller  NHunsberger  S  et al.  Designing clinical trials with behavioral and pharmacologic interventions: the Raynaud's Treatment Study.  Control Clin Trials. 1999;2052- 63Google ScholarCrossref
37.
Kotz  SJohnson  NLRead  CB Encyclopedia of Statistical Sciences.  New York, NY John Wiley & Sons Inc1983;397- 400
38.
Rubin  D Multiple imputation after 18+ years.  J Am Stat Assoc. 1996;91473- 489Google ScholarCrossref
39.
SAS Institute Inc, SAS Language: Reference, Version 6.  Cary, NC SAS Institute Inc1990;
40.
Wigley  FM Raynaud's phenomenon. Fortuin  NJed. Current Therapy in Cardiovascular Disease Philadelphia, Pa BC Decker Inc1987;319- 322Google Scholar
41.
Adee  AC Managing Raynaud's phenomenon: a practical approach.  Am Fam Physician. 1993;47823- 829Google Scholar
42.
Ferri  CCecchetti  RCini  G  et al.  Slow-releasing nicardipine in the treatment of Raynaud's phenomena without underlying diseases.  Clin Rheumatol. 1992;1176- 80Google ScholarCrossref
43.
Rhedda  AMcCans  JWillan  ARFord  PM A double blind placebo controlled crossover randomized trial of diltiazem in Raynaud's phenomenon.  J Rheumatol. 1985;12724- 727Google Scholar
44.
Leppert  JJonasson  TNilsson  HRingqvist  I The effect of isradipine, a new calcium-channel antagonist, in patients with primary Raynaud's phenomenon: a single-blind dose-response study.  Cardiovasc Drugs Ther. 1989;3397- 401Google ScholarCrossref
45.
Freedman  RRKeegan  DMigaly  PGalloway  MMayes  M Plasma catecholamines during behavioral treatments for Raynaud's disease.  Psychosom Med. 1991;53433- 439Google ScholarCrossref
46.
Freedman  RRSabharwal  SIanni  PDesai  NWenig  PMayes  M Non-neural beta-adrenergic vasodilating mechanism in temperature biofeedback.  Psychosom Med. 1988;50394- 401Google ScholarCrossref
47.
Freedman  RR Quantitative measurements of finger blood flow during behavioral treatments for Raynaud's disease.  Psychophysiology. 1989;26437- 441Google ScholarCrossref
48.
Otis  JDRasey  HWVrochopoulos  SWincze  JPAndrasik  F Temperature acquisition as a function of the computer-based biofeedback system utilized: an exploratory analysis.  Biofeedback Self Regul. 1995;20185- 190Google ScholarCrossref
49.
Freedman  RRIanni  PWenig  P Behavioral treatment of Raynaud's disease: long-term follow-up.  J Consult Clin Psychol. 1985;53136Google ScholarCrossref
×