[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Figure 1.  Search Strategy Flow Chart
Search Strategy Flow Chart
Figure 2.  Cognitive Development Among Young Children With and Without Opioid Exposure
Cognitive Development Among Young Children With and Without Opioid Exposure

SMD indicates standardized mean difference.

Table 1.  General Characteristics of Included Studies and 16 Cohorts Comparing Infants With and Without Opioid Exposure
General Characteristics of Included Studies and 16 Cohorts Comparing Infants With and Without Opioid Exposure
Table 2.  Mother and Infant Characteristics of Medication for Addition Treatment Exposed vs Unexposed Groups
Mother and Infant Characteristics of Medication for Addition Treatment Exposed vs Unexposed Groups
Table 3.  Meta-analytic Comparison of More Comparable vs Less Comparable Recruitment for Maternal and Infant Confounding Variables
Meta-analytic Comparison of More Comparable vs Less Comparable Recruitment for Maternal and Infant Confounding Variables
1.
Haight  SC, Ko  JY, Tong  VT, Bohm  MK, Callaghan  WM.  Opioid use disorder documented at delivery hospitalization—United States, 1999-2014.  MMWR Morb Mortal Wkly Rep. 2018;67(31):845-849. doi:10.15585/mmwr.mm6731a1PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
American College of Obstetricians and Gynecologists; American Society of Addiction Medicine. ACOG Committee opinion: opioid use and opioid use disorder in pregnancy. Accessed February 6, 2020. https://www.acog.org/Clinical-Guidance-and-Publications/Committee-Opinions/Committee-on-Obstetric-Practice/Opioid-Use-and-Opioid-Use-Disorder-in-Pregnancy?IsMobileSet=false
3.
Sutter  MB, Leeman  L, Hsi  A.  Neonatal opioid withdrawal syndrome.  Obstet Gynecol Clin North Am. 2014;41(2):317-334. doi:10.1016/j.ogc.2014.02.010PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Raffaeli  G, Cavallaro  G, Allegaert  K,  et al.  Neonatal abstinence syndrome: update on diagnostic and therapeutic strategies.  Pharmacotherapy. 2017;37(7):814-823. doi:10.1002/phar.1954PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Wachman  EM, Schiff  DM, Silverstein  M.  Neonatal abstinence syndrome: advances in diagnosis and treatment.  JAMA. 2018;319(13):1362-1374. doi:10.1001/jama.2018.2640PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Baldacchino  A, Arbuckle  K, Petrie  DJ, McCowan  C.  Erratum: neurobehavioral consequences of chronic intrauterine opioid exposure in infants and preschool children: a systematic review and meta-analysis.  BMC Psychiatry. 2015;15:134. doi:10.1186/s12888-015-0438-5PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Konijnenberg  C, Lund  IO, Melinder  A.  Behavioural outcomes of four-year-old children prenatally exposed to methadone or buprenorphine: a test of three risk models.  Early Child Dev Care. 2015;185(10):1641-1657. doi:10.1080/03004430.2015.1016506Google ScholarCrossref
8.
Hatzis  D, Dawe  S, Harnett  P, Barlow  J.  Quality of caregiving in mothers with illicit substance use: a systematic review and meta-analysis.  Subst Abuse. 2017;11:1178221817694038. doi:10.1177/1178221817694038PubMedGoogle Scholar
9.
Kingston  D, McDonald  S, Austin  M-P, Tough  S.  Association between prenatal and postnatal psychological distress and toddler cognitive development: a systematic review.  PLoS One. 2015;10(5):e0126929. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0126929PubMedGoogle Scholar
10.
Lindenberg  CS, Keith  AB.  Opiate abuse in pregnancy.  Annu Rev Nurs Res. 1993;11:249-279. doi:10.1891/0739-6686.11.1.249PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
Green  CM, Berkule  SB, Dreyer  BP,  et al.  Maternal literacy and associations between education and the cognitive home environment in low-income families.  Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2009;163(9):832-837. doi:10.1001/archpediatrics.2009.136PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Yeoh  SL, Eastwood  J, Wright  IM,  et al.  Cognitive and motor outcomes of children with prenatal opioid exposure: a systematic review and meta-analysis.  JAMA Netw Open. 2019;2(7):e197025. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2019.7025PubMedGoogle Scholar
13.
Winklbaur  B, Baewert  A, Jagsch  R,  et al.  Association between prenatal tobacco exposure and outcome of neonates born to opioid-maintained mothers: implications for treatment.  Eur Addict Res. 2009;15(3):150-156. doi:10.1159/000216466PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
Blumenshine  P, Egerter  S, Barclay  CJ, Cubbin  C, Braveman  PA.  Socioeconomic disparities in adverse birth outcomes: a systematic review.  Am J Prev Med. 2010;39(3):263-272. doi:10.1016/j.amepre.2010.05.012PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Mactier  H, Shipton  D, Dryden  C, Tappin  DM.  Reduced fetal growth in methadone-maintained pregnancies is not fully explained by smoking or socio-economic deprivation.  Addiction. 2014;109(3):482-488. doi:10.1111/add.12400PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Stroup  DF, Berlin  JA, Morton  SC,  et al; Meta-analysis of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (MOOSE) Group.  Meta-analysis of Observational Studies in Epidemiology: a proposal for reporting.  JAMA. 2000;283(15):2008-2012. doi:10.1001/jama.283.15.2008PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
Moher  D, Liberati  A, Tetzlaff  J, Altman  DG; PRISMA Group.  Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement.  PLoS Med. 2009;6(7):e1000097. doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097PubMedGoogle Scholar
18.
Learning Sciences Resource Guide. Components of cognitive theory. Accessed June 17, 2019. https://canvas.vt.edu/courses/62492/pages/components-of-cognitive-theory
19.
Ryan  R, Synnot  A, Prictor  M, Hill  S. Cochrane Consumers and Communication Group Data extraction template for included studies. Accessed February 7, 2020. http://cccrg.cochrane.org/author-resources
20.
Balk  EM, Chung  M, Chen  ML, Trikalinos  TA, Kong Win Chang  L.  Assessing the Accuracy of Google Translate to Allow Data Extraction From Trials Published in Non-English Languages. Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality; 2013. Accessed June 24, 2019. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK121304/
21.
Downs  SH, Black  N.  The feasibility of creating a checklist for the assessment of the methodological quality both of randomised and non-randomised studies of health care interventions.  J Epidemiol Community Health. 1998;52(6):377-384. doi:10.1136/jech.52.6.377PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
Harrer  M, Cuijpers  P, Furukawa  TA, Ebert  DD. Doing meta-analysis in R: a hands-on guide. Accessed June 12, 2019. https://bookdown.org/MathiasHarrer/Doing_Meta_Analysis_in_R/
23.
Bakhireva  LN, Holbrook  BD, Shrestha  S,  et al.  Association between prenatal opioid exposure, neonatal opioid withdrawal syndrome, and neurodevelopmental and behavioral outcomes at 5-8 months of age.  Early Hum Dev. 2019;128:69-76. doi:10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2018.10.010PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Bauman  PS, Levine  SA.  The development of children of drug addicts.  Int J Addict. 1986;21(8):849-863. doi:10.3109/10826088609027399PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
Bunikowski  R, Grimmer  I, Heiser  A, Metze  B, Schäfer  A, Obladen  M.  Neurodevelopmental outcome after prenatal exposure to opiates.  Eur J Pediatr. 1998;157(9):724-730. doi:10.1007/s004310050923PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
Chasnoff  IJ, Schnoll  SH, Burns  WJ, Burns  K.  Maternal nonnarcotic substance abuse during pregnancy: effects on infant development.  Neurobehav Toxicol Teratol. 1984;6(4):277-280.PubMedGoogle Scholar
27.
Bernstein  VJ, Hans  SL.  Predicting the developmental outcome of two-year-old children born exposed to methadone: impact of social and environmental risk factors.  J Clin Child Psychol. 1994;23(4):349-359. doi:10.1207/s15374424jccp2304_1Google ScholarCrossref
28.
Hans  SL.  Developmental consequences of prenatal exposure to methadone.  Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1989;562:195-207. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.1989.tb21018.xPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
29.
Hunt  RW, Tzioumi  D, Collins  E, Jeffery  HE.  Adverse neurodevelopmental outcome of infants exposed to opiate in-utero.  Early Hum Dev. 2008;84(1):29-35. doi:10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2007.01.013PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
30.
Kaltenbach  K, Finnegan  LP.  Children exposed to methadone in utero: assessment of developmental and cognitive ability.  Ann NY Acad Sci. 1989;562(1):360-362. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.1989.tb21039.xGoogle ScholarCrossref
31.
Bernstein  V, Jeremy  RJ, Hans  SL, Marcus  J.  A longitudinal study of offspring born to methadone-maintained women II: dyadic interaction and infant behavior at 4 months.  Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse. 1984;10(2):161-193. doi:10.3109/00952998409002779PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
32.
Bernstein  VJ, Jeremy  RJ, Marcus  J.  Mother-infant interaction in multiproblem families: finding those at risk.  J Am Acad Child Psychiatry. 1986;25(5):631-640. doi:10.1016/S0002-7138(09)60287-9PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
33.
Rosen  TS, Johnson  HL.  Drug-addicted mothers, their infants, and SIDS.  Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1988;533:89-95. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.1988.tb37236.xPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
34.
Johnson  HL, Rosen  TS.  Prenatal methadone exposure: effects on behavior in early infancy.  Pediatr Pharmacol (New York). 1982;2(2):113-120.PubMedGoogle Scholar
35.
Johnson  HL, Diano  A, Rosen  TS.  24-Month neuro-behavioral follow-up of children of methadone-maintained mothers.  Infant Behav Dev. 1984;7(1):115-123. doi:10.1016/S0163-6383(84)80027-2Google ScholarCrossref
36.
Rosen  TS, Johnson  HL.  Long-term effects of prenatal methadone maintenance.  NIDA Res Monogr. 1985;59:73-83.PubMedGoogle Scholar
37.
Rosen  TS, Johnson  HL.  Children of methadone-maintained mothers: follow-up to 18 months of age.  J Pediatr. 1982;101(2):192-196. doi:10.1016/S0022-3476(82)80115-7PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
38.
Salo  S, Politi  J, Tupola  S,  et al.  Early development of opioid-exposed infants born to mothers in buprenorphine-replacement therapy.  J Reprod Infant Psychol. 2010;28(2):161-179. doi:10.1080/02646830903219109Google ScholarCrossref
39.
Strauss  ME, Starr  RH, Ostrea  EM, Chavez  CJ, Stryker  JC.  Behavioural concomitants of prenatal addiction to narcotics.  J Pediatr. 1976;89(5):842-846. doi:10.1016/S0022-3476(76)80822-0PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
40.
Strauss  ME, Lessen-Firestone  JK, Chavez  CJ, Stryker  JC.  Children of methadone-treated women at five years of age.  Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1979;11(suppl):3-6.PubMedGoogle Scholar
41.
van Baar  A.  Development of infants of drug dependent mothers.  J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1990;31(6):911-920. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1990.tb00833.xPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
42.
van Baar  AL, Fleury  P, Ultee  CA.  Behaviour in first year after drug dependent pregnancy.  Arch Dis Child. 1989;64(2):241-245. doi:10.1136/adc.64.2.241PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
43.
van Baar  A, de Graaff  BM.  Cognitive development at preschool-age of infants of drug-dependent mothers.  Dev Med Child Neurol. 1994;36(12):1063-1075. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8749.1994.tb11809.xPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
44.
Whitham  JN.  Prenatal Exposure to Buprenorphine or Methadone: Effects on Physical Growth, Neurological Development and Temperament in Early Childhood [thesis]. University of Adelaide; 2012. Accessed February 7, 2020. https://digital.library.adelaide.edu.au/dspace/handle/2440/72184
45.
Lifschitz  MH, Wilson  GS, Smith  EO, Desmond  MM.  Factors affecting head growth and intellectual function in children of drug addicts.  Pediatrics. 1985;75(2):269-274.PubMedGoogle Scholar
46.
Wilson  GS, Desmond  MM, Wait  RB.  Follow-up of methadone-treated and untreated narcotic-dependent women and their infants: health, developmental, and social implications.  J Pediatr. 1981;98(5):716-722. doi:10.1016/S0022-3476(81)80830-XPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
47.
Wilson  GS.  Clinical studies of infants and children exposed prenatally to heroin.  Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1989;562:183-194. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.1989.tb21017.xPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
48.
Davie-Gray  A.  The Early Development and Family Environments of Children Born to Mothers Engaged in Methadone Maintenance During Pregnancy [thesis]. University of Canterbury; 2011. Accessed July 31, 2018. https://ir.canterbury.ac.nz/handle/10092/5508
49.
Lee  SJ.  The School-Readiness of Children Born to Mothers Maintained on Methadone During Pregnancy [thesis]. University of Canterbury; 2012. Accessed March 2, 2018. https://ir.canterbury.ac.nz/handle/10092/9280
50.
Levine  TA, Woodward  LJ.  Early inhibitory control and working memory abilities of children prenatally exposed to methadone.  Early Hum Dev. 2018;116:68-75. doi:10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2017.11.010PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
51.
Baron  IS, Leonberger  KA.  Assessment of intelligence in the preschool period.  Neuropsychol Rev. 2012;22(4):334-344. doi:10.1007/s11065-012-9215-0PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
52.
Hurks  PPM, Bakker  H.  Assessing intelligence in children and youth living in the Netherlands.  Int J Sch Educ Psychol. 2016;4(4):266-275. doi:10.1080/21683603.2016.1166754Google ScholarCrossref
53.
Quintana  SM, Aboud  FE, Chao  RK,  et al.  Race, ethnicity, and culture in child development: contemporary research and future directions.  Child Dev. 2006;77(5):1129-1141. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00951.xPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
54.
Merhar  SL, McAllister  JM, Wedig-Stevie  KE, Klein  AC, Meinzen-Derr  J, Poindexter  BB.  Retrospective review of neurodevelopmental outcomes in infants treated for neonatal abstinence syndrome.  J Perinatol. 2018;38(5):587-592. doi:10.1038/s41372-018-0088-9PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
55.
Akerman  SC, Brunette  MF, Green  AI, Goodman  DJ, Blunt  HB, Heil  SH.  Treating tobacco use disorder in pregnant women in medication-assisted treatment for an opioid use disorder: a systematic review.  J Subst Abuse Treat. 2015;52:40-47. doi:10.1016/j.jsat.2014.12.002PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
56.
Magnusson  K. Interpreting Cohen’s d effect size. Published February 3, 2014. Accessed June 24, 2019. https://rpsychologist.com/d3/cohend/
57.
Nair  P, Schuler  ME, Black  MM, Kettinger  L, Harrington  D.  Cumulative environmental risk in substance abusing women: early intervention, parenting stress, child abuse potential and child development.  Child Abuse Negl. 2003;27(9):997-1017.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
58.
Schuler  ME, Nair  P, Kettinger  L.  Drug-exposed infants and developmental outcome: effects of a home intervention and ongoing maternal drug use.  Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2003;157(2):133-138. doi:10.1001/archpedi.157.2.133PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
59.
Temple  JA, Reynolds  AJ.  Benefits and costs of investments in preschool education: evidence from the Child-Parent Centers and related programs.  Econ Educ Rev. 2007;26(1):126-144. doi:10.1016/j.econedurev.2005.11.004Google ScholarCrossref
60.
Wong  HS, Santhakumaran  S, Cowan  FM, Modi  N; Medicines for Neonates Investigator Group.  Developmental assessments in preterm children: a meta-analysis.  Pediatrics. 2016;138(2):e20160251. doi:10.1542/peds.2016-0251PubMedGoogle Scholar
61.
Brito  NH, Fifer  WP, Amso  D,  et al.  Beyond the Bayley: neurocognitive assessments of development during infancy and toddlerhood.  Dev Neuropsychol. 2019;44(2):220-247. doi:10.1080/87565641.2018.1564310PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
Original Investigation
Pediatrics
March 18, 2020

Cognitive Outcomes of Young Children After Prenatal Exposure to Medications for Opioid Use Disorder: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis

Author Affiliations
  • 1Addiction Medicine Fellowship Program, Department of Family and Community Medicine, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque
  • 2Honors College, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque
  • 3Combined BA/MD Program, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque
  • 4Division of Public Health, Department of Family and Preventive Medicine, University of Utah, Salt Lake City
  • 5Department of Family and Community Medicine, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque
JAMA Netw Open. 2020;3(3):e201195. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2020.1195
Key Points

Question  Is prenatal exposure to methadone or buprenorphine for treatment of opioid use disorder during pregnancy associated with differences in cognitive development in young children?

Findings  This systematic review and meta-analysis of nearly 50 years of observational research, analyzing 27 studies that included 1086 children, showed an overall negative association of exposure to methadone or buprenorphine with cognitive development. However, subanalyses revealed that this outcome may be associated with imbalances in the recruitment of mothers with different socioeconomic and educational backgrounds, levels of tobacco use in pregnancy, and fetal growth characteristics.

Meaning  The findings of this study suggest that poor recruitment of comparison groups could prevent conclusive determination regarding the association of prenatal exposure to methadone or buprenorphine with cognitive outcomes. Prenatal exposure to methadone or buprenorphine may have minimal direct associations when confounders, particularly tobacco use, are controlled.

Abstract

Importance  The number of children with prenatal opioid exposure to medication for addiction treatment (MAT) with methadone and buprenorphine for maternal opioid use disorder is increasing, but the associations of this exposure with cognitive outcomes are not well understood.

Objective  To examine the strength and consistency of findings in the medical literature regarding the association of prenatal exposure to MAT with early childhood cognitive development, particularly when accounting for variables outside MAT exposure.

Data Sources  A search strategy obtained publications from PubMed, CINAHL, PsycINFO, Web of Science, and Embase from January 1972 to June 2019. Reference lists from identified articles were searched.

Study Selection  Inclusion criteria were cohort studies, studies including children aged 1 to 60 months with at least 2 months of prenatal MAT exposure, studies using standardized direct-observation testing scales, and studies reporting means and SDs. Case reports, case series, historical controls, and reviews were excluded.

Data Extraction and Synthesis  Two authors independently selected studies for inclusion, extracted data, and assessed study quality. Data extracted included demographic characteristics, covariates, sources of bias, and effect estimates. Meta-analysis was performed using random-effects models. This study was conducted according to the Meta-analysis of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (MOOSE) guidelines and the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analysis (PRISMA) statement. Data extraction and synthesis were conducted between January 2018 and August 2019.

Main Outcomes and Measures  Cognitive test scores and demographic variability between exposed and unexposed groups.

Results  A total of 16 unique cohorts, described in 27 articles and including 1086 children (485 [44.7%] with MAT exposure), were included in a quantitative synthesis. On meta-analysis, MAT exposure was associated with lower cognitive development scores (pooled standardized mean difference, −0.57; 95% CI, −0.93 to −0.21; I2 = 81%). Multiple subanalyses on demographic characteristics (ie, maternal education, race/ethnicity, socioeconomic status, prenatal tobacco exposure, infant sex) were conducted. In the subanalysis of studies with comparable prenatal exposure to tobacco smoke, the association of MAT exposure with cognitive scores was no longer statistically significant and became homogeneous (standardized mean difference, −0.11; 95% CI, −0.42 to 0.20; I2 = 0%).

Conclusions and Relevance  In this study, predefined subanalyses demonstrated how poor recruitment, particularly imbalances in maternal tobacco use, could contribute to a negative overall association of cognitive development test scores with prenatal MAT exposure. Promoting tobacco cessation for pregnant women with opioid use disorder should be prioritized in this high-risk population.

Introduction

The effects of the opioid crisis are permeating all areas of medicine in the US, including neonatology and pediatrics. Between 2009 and 2014, the number of women diagnosed with opioid use disorder (OUD) during pregnancy quadrupled from 1.5 to 6.5 cases per 100 000 delivery hospitalizations per year.1 With so many mother-fetal dyads experiencing OUD, it is recommended by the American College of Obstetricians and Gynecologists that pregnant women with OUD be treated with opioid agonists.2 Despite benefits for both mother and fetus, some infants develop neonatal opioid withdrawal syndrome (NOWS) and require opioid medications to alleviate withdrawal symptoms.2-5

After the acute withdrawal phase, the long-term consequences of prenatal exposure to medication for addiction treatment (MAT) with methadone and buprenorphine are less well understood. Some research suggests intrauterine exposure to MAT is associated with detrimental developmental outcomes, including problems with motor skills, language, and attention.6 However, indirect associations of a disordered home environment concomitant with the mother’s substance use disorder have been theorized as a more important factor in cognitive outcomes among these children.7 Women with substance use disorder often have fewer economic and employment opportunities, lower educational attainment, and a history of adverse childhood experiences, all of which may influence mother-infant interactions, maternal stress levels, and early childhood development.8-11

Two previous meta-analyses specific to cognitive outcomes among young children after opioid exposure have been published.6,12 Both identified a significant negative association (ie, lower cognitive development test scores) among children with opioid exposure. Furthermore, both meta-analyses identified that the included articles were of overall poor quality and suggested that differential social, environmental, and familial risks between children with and without exposure may contribute to the observed cognitive differences. The 2019 meta-analysis by Yeoh et al12 performed subanalyses on recruitment of comparable socioeconomic status and found stratification lessened the magnitude of the association of opioid exposure with cognitive development. However, neither prior meta-analysis subanalyzed on other factors associated with developmental risks, such as low maternal education or employment, infant sex, or tobacco smoke exposure, all of which are independently associated with cognitive development.13-15

The goal of this meta-analysis was to determine the consistency of findings regarding the association of prenatal exposure to methadone and buprenorphine with early childhood cognitive developmental when accounting for recruitment imbalances in the included studies. To the degree possible, we quantified the associations of predefined external variables that are associated with cognitive development of children with MAT exposure. We hypothesized that these children would have the same cognitive testing scores as children with no exposure after accounting for external maternal and infant recruitment variables.

Methods
Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria

This systematic review and meta-analysis was conducted according to the Meta-analysis of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (MOOSE) reporting guideline16 and the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analyses (PRISMA) reporting guideline.17 A review protocol was created prior to data extraction. This review was not registered. Per the Common Rule, ethical approval and informed patient consent were not required given that this study was a literature review with no direct patient contact or influence on patient care directly related to this work.

Inclusion criteria were as follows: (1) children aged 1 to 60 months at testing, (2) prenatal exposure to legally prescribed methadone or buprenorphine during at least 2 months during pregnancy, (3) at least 10 children in each group, and (4) use of a previously published and validated direct observation method for measuring cognitive development. Cognitive development was defined as the construction of attention, perception, memory, language, categorization skills, reasoning and decision-making, problem solving, procedural and conceptual learning, and skill acquisition.18

Exclusion criteria were as follows: (1) case series and case studies, (2) use of historical or population-level data for the comparison group, (3) neurological studies without correlation to standard cognitive developmental tests (eg, visual evoked potentials, saccades), (4) parent-reports, and (5) statistics other than means and SDs.

Search Strategy and Data Extraction

One of us (L.F.N.) has prior training and experience with meta-analysis techniques. The other reviewers (V.K.Y. and K.D.P.) had advanced scientific literature review experience from undergraduate coursework and were trained on subject-specific techniques using articles not meeting inclusion criteria. Articles were identified using an electronic and hand-searching strategy. An electronic search was performed of PubMed, CINAHL, PsycINFO, and Web of Science between January 1, 1970, and June 28, 2019 (ie, 49.5 years). Embase was searched through March 30, 2018 (ie, 48.3 years). No language constraints were applied. Search terms are available in the eAppendix in the Supplement and included variations of prenatal exposure, methadone, buprenorphine, child development, and child behavior.

Two of us (L.F.N and V.K.Y) independently reviewed all titles and abstracts for inclusion. Studies meeting inclusion criteria were extracted by 2 independent reviewers (L.F.N., V.K.Y., or K.D.P.) and compared. Data were extracted to a standard form for observational studies based on the Cochrane Group Data Extraction Template for Included Studies.19 Discrepancies were resolved by consensus through referral to the original studies and, if necessary, arbitration by a third reviewer. Reference lists of included articles were screened to find other suitable studies. Email contact with authors was attempted when insufficient data, conference abstracts, or unpublished data were identified. No further data were supplied by contacted authors. A total of 11 non–English language articles were screened for inclusion by translating the abstract using Google Translate (Google) as previously described,20 but none met inclusion criteria.

In some cases, authors published multiple articles on the same group of children over time. Typically, each publication was a cohort study with authors repeating testing as children aged and publishing a second or third article. Essentially, this represents a longitudinal study published at discrete points. To avoid double-counting participants from these articles, a composite extraction form was made to detail which information was extracted from each study (eTable in the Supplement). This cohort merge technique provided a more comprehensive compilation of demographic factors, given that comprehensive baseline characteristics were often described only in the first study published.

Statistical Analysis

When available in the published studies, variables considered relevant confounders, moderators, and mediators were extracted. These were selected a priori based on literature review and clinical experience. Prespecified subgroup analyses included the following: maternal race/ethnicity; education; socioeconomic status; employment; exposure to illicit substances, tobacco, and/or alcohol; and infant sex.

Heterogeneity, ie, the variation in outcomes among studies, was assessed by the I2 index and τ. The presence of publication bias was assessed informally by visual inspection of funnel plots and formally by Egger test of the intercept.

A modified Downs and Black assessment of quality was used to evaluate internal and external validity, bias, and power.21 Two nonmasked reviewers (L.F.N., V.K.Y., or K.D.P.) independently completed the quality assessment form, and consensus was reached as described earlier. For the cohort merge extractions, the highest quality article was used for Downs and Black analysis. No articles were excluded on quality grounds.

Data were abstracted, quantified, coded, and assembled into a Microsoft Excel version 16.32 (Microsoft Corp) database. Statistical analysis was performed using meta, metafor, and dpylr packages in R Studio version 1.2.1335 (R Project for Statistical Computing). Standard meta-analytic techniques for means and SDs were used with the methods presented by Harrer et al.22 When testing was performed at multiple ages, the most recent point was used for meta-analysis. All developmental tests were transformed to a mean of 100 with an SD of 15. Statistical methods included calculation of weighted means and SDs as well as χ2, t tests, and z tests for proportions. Because of significant variation in study methods and small sample sizes, random-effect models were applied using Hedges g statistic for effect size and a Knapp-Hartung-Sidik-Jonkman adjustment for τ. Effect size is presented as standardized mean difference (SMD). Negative SMDs represent worse performance among children with MAT exposure. A 2-tailed α < .05 was used as the threshold for statistical significance. Data extraction and synthesis were conducted between January 2018 and August 2019.

Results

Our literature search yielded 941 nonduplicate potential articles, of which 914 (97.1%) were excluded (Figure 1; eTable in the Supplement). A total of 27 studies met the inclusion criteria and were included in the final review, representing 16 unique cohorts of children from 6 countries.23-50 These cohorts included a total of 1086 children, 485 (44.7%) with exposure to methadone or buprenorphine prenatally and 601 (55.3%) with no exposure. Details of the included studies can be found in Table 1. Included cognitive tests were the Bayley Scales of Infant Development, Mental Development Index (10 cohorts [62.5%]), the Stanford-Binet Intelligence Test (1 cohort [6.3%]), the McCarthy General Cognitive Index (2 cohorts [12.5%]), Griffith Intellectual Performance (1 cohort [6.3%]), the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence–Revised (1 cohort [6.3%]), and the Revisie Amsterdamse Kinder Intelligentie Test (in Dutch; 1 cohort [6.3%]).51,52 In every study, the mean score on cognitive testing scales for children with MAT exposure children was within the normal range (ie, within 1 SD of the mean).

Quality and Publication Bias

The mean (SD) quality of the studies was low (15.2 [4.6] of 24 points), as measured by the modified Downs and Black tool (eFigure 1 in the Supplement).21 Most studies had poor internal validity, particularly regarding selection bias, with recruitment of comparison mothers who were dissimilar to the mothers receiving MAT. As a whole, the included studies inadequately described the study population base, recruitment methods, children lost to follow-up, and adjustment for confounding. Assessment of loss to follow-up was performed by comparing the number of children recruited with the number evaluated at the final point for each study or cohort. Loss to follow-up was higher for children with MAT exposure, with a median (interquartile range) loss to follow-up of 39% (15%-49%) for children with exposure and 15% (7%-33%) for children without exposure. Four studies did not report sufficient baseline recruitment data to calculate losses. No studies adequately reported whether the children who were lost to follow-up differed from those who completed the study.

Visual inspection of the funnel plot (eFigure 2 in the Supplement) and Egger test of the intercept indicated no significant asymmetry (intercept, −2.3; 95% CI, −7.5 to 2.9; P = .40). This finding reduces the likelihood of publication bias, meaning both positive and negative findings were identified by our search strategy.

Demographic Characteristics

Maternal and child characteristics are shown in Table 2. Compared with the nonexposed group, the MAT-exposed group had lower socioeconomic status (108 of 238 [45.3%] vs 171 of 190 [90.0%]; P < .001), lower educational attainment (less than high school: 82 of 241 [34.0%] vs 137 of 206 [66.5%]; P < .001), and a higher proportion of tobacco use (156 of 394 [39.6%] vs 314 of 353 [89.0%]; P < .001) and other drug use (13 of 566 [2.3%] vs 199 of 513 [38.8%]; P < .001) during pregnancy. Compared with infants with no MAT exposure, those with MAT exposure were more likely to be male (249 of 532 [46.8%] vs 295 of 536 [55.0%]; P = .03), to be born at an earlier term mean (SD) gestational age (39.3 [1.8] weeks vs 38.9 [1.9] weeks; P < .001), to have a lower mean (SD) birth weight (3366.6 [444.3] g vs 2966.5 [467.8] g; P < .001), and to have a smaller mean (SD) head circumference (34.7 [1.5] cm vs 33.4 [1.6] cm; P < .001). Approximately half of infants (264 of 542 [48.7%]) with MAT exposure required medical treatment for NOWS.

Association of MAT With Child Cognitive Development

On meta-analysis of overall cognitive development (not accounting for suspected influential variables), MAT exposure was associated with statistically significantly lower cognitive test scores (pooled SMD, −0.57; 95% CI, −0.93 to −0.21). A large amount of heterogeneity between studies was apparent (I2 = 81%) (Figure 2). We evaluated the data for outliers and conduced an influence analysis using a Baujat plot.22 The Rosen cohort34-37 was identified as being very influential and a possible outlier. A sensitivity analysis excluding the Rosen cohort increased the pooled SMD to −0.46 (95% CI, −0.76 to −0.16; I2 = 74%). Because of the minimal improvement in heterogeneity, we elected to include the Rosen cohort in the analyses.

Planned Subanalyses

Given that this study planned multiple subanalyses a priori, we set out to examine the robustness of the overall association when accounting for maternal and infant differences. First, we conducted subanalyses stratifying by whether studies recruited comparable maternal populations (Table 3). Studies were considered comparable if the exposed and unexposed groups had within 10% similarity on maternal race/ethnicity, socioeconomic status, and education level. These factors were chosen because differences in maternal education and socioeconomic status are independently associated with infant development.11 Race/ethnicity are social constructs, not biological characteristics, and as such are not independently associated with developmental outcomes53 but were included in the analysis as a proxy for whether studies recruited mothers from similar populations. As shown in Table 3, the SMD changed minimally in studies with more comparable maternal race/ethnicity, socioeconomic status, or maternal education compared with studies with less comparable characteristics (eg, education level: −0.47 [95% CI, −1.59 to 0.65] vs −0.56 [95% CI, −1.64 to 0.51]), and 95% CIs expanded across 0, becoming nonsignificant. All retained high heterogeneity (eg, education level: 87% vs 79%).

Most studies recruited women during the prenatal period, risking an imbalance in infant characteristics, particularly sex and exposure to tobacco smoke during pregnancy. Sex imbalance can be problematic because female infants tend to score higher on standardized cognitive testing.54 In this subanalysis, when studies had similar proportions of male infants in the exposed and unexposed groups, the SMD improved to −0.40 (95% CI −1.35 to 0.55; I2 = 67%) and became statistically nonsignificant. As many as 85% to 90% of infants of mothers receiving MAT also have prenatal tobacco exposure55; however, only 4 cohorts recruited women to the comparison group who reported regular tobacco use. When these 4 cohorts were meta-analyzed, the SMD was reduced to −0.11 (95% CI, −0.42 to 0.20) with a low heterogeneity of I2 = 0%. Conversely, when poorly matched studies on maternal tobacco use and infant characteristics were pooled for subanalysis, the SMD became more negative and the 95% CI was statistically significant (SMD, −1.19; 95% CI, −2.00 to −0.39).

Discussion

Our study, as with previously published work, found that prenatal exposure to MAT was associated with a statistically significant negative difference in cognitive scores compared with those without exposure, although lower scores among these children do not necessarily indicate developmental delay. The SMD of −0.57 indicated that there is an approximately 66% chance that a child picked at random from the exposed group will have a lower score than a child picked at random from the unexposed comparison group and that there was a 76% overlap between the 2 populations.56 However, the high heterogeneity of 81% makes interpretation difficult. This high degree of heterogeneity remained for the majority of subanalyses, with the notable exception of tobacco smoke exposure, which had an I2 of 0%, indicating a very homogeneous sample.

While consistent with previous research, the subanalyses reported here provide evidence that the overall effect size in a meta-analysis is not a final answer to the question of interest. Conducting predefined subanalyses allowed us to demonstrate how poor study design, especially recruitment, could contribute to a negative overall finding. Tobacco use, low socioeconomic status, low educational attainment, black race, and methadone are all independently associated with poor fetal growth and birth outcomes, which can affect early childhood cognition.13-15

Although we cannot conclude whether MAT has a direct influence on the fetal brain, the well-known deleterious associations of tobacco smoke are again illustrated in this meta-analysis. Tobacco is associated with birth outcomes, early childhood development, and more severe NOWS symptoms.13,55 When we subanalyzed 4 cohorts with comparable tobacco smoke exposure between children exposed and unexposed to MAT, the negative association of MAT exposure with cognitive development approached zero (SMD, −0.11; 95% CI, −0.42 to 0.20), and the heterogeneity decreased to 0%. Conversely, pooling poorly comparable studies on smoking accentuated the negative association (SMD, −1.19; 95% CI, −2.00 to −0.39; I2 = 89%). This indicates 2 critical issues: first, mismatched recruitment on tobacco use in pregnancy is a likely moderator or explanatory variable for the overall negative association of MAT with cognitive development reported in previous studies, and second, intensive smoking cessation efforts should be incorporated into all opioid treatment programs for pregnant women. Previous work has shown successful contingency management strategies with quit rates of more than 30% in 12-week cessation programs for opioid-dependent pregnant women.55

Given that many children with prenatal opioid exposure are born into families with complicated trauma histories, low socioeconomic status, low maternal education, and experiences of racism, early childhood intervention programs should be prioritized, regardless of the presence of gross delay. In the setting of prenatal opioid exposure, home-based early intervention services have been shown to reduce child abuse and promote cognitive development.57,58 School-based programs for children from low-income families and/or belonging to minority groups improve school readiness, which results in lower long-term costs for special education, behavioral problems, unemployment, and later criminal behaviors.59 Therefore, both home-based and school-based programs should be universally available to this at-risk population.

Limitations

This study has limitations. Meta-analyses are only as valid as the studies that contribute, and the included studies had considerable limitations with respect to recruitment of comparable unexposed groups and loss to follow-up. Our subanalyses attempted to control for imbalanced recruitment. Furthermore, included studies were observational cohorts, which are subject to many biases and can have lower internal validity compared with randomized clinical studies. Only 9 of the 16 included cohorts reported masking investigators to the participants’ exposure statuses, possibly introducing an expectancy bias. No randomized studies were identified for inclusion. Additionally, cognitive testing of infants and young children is challenging; test results have poor positive predictive value for later developmental delay.60,61 Therefore, the purpose of this study was not to predict developmental delay but rather to measure cognitive abilities of children with MAT exposure compared with their peers with no exposure.

In addition to problems with the internal validity of the included studies, there are limitations for this systematic review and meta-analysis. A major limitation is that only studies with means and SDs were included. We attempted to contact authors for missing data but were unsuccessful. By excluding studies with other metrics, particularly those with adjusted effect size estimates, we may have excluded data with different conclusions. Another limitation is the high heterogeneity of the overall effect and subanalyses. This is not unexpected given the long time line, variety of developmental tests, clinical factors, and children’s age range. Similar heterogeneity has been reported in previous meta-analyses of this topic.6,12 Next, there is a potential problem of multiple comparisons; however, this is likely limited, given that each subanalysis had a different selection of input data. A final limitation is generalizability. The included studies had a lengthy time range (ie, January 1972 to June 2019) and had a large geographic distribution (ie, North America, Europe, and Oceania), all were English-language, and most were conducted in urban settings. Therefore, it is difficult to generalize these findings to individual children in a clinical setting, particularly for rural or non–English speaking populations.

Conclusions

In conclusion, this meta-analysis, spanning nearly 50 years of research, demonstrated that the developmental detriment reported in observational studies of children with prenatal MAT exposure could be heavily influenced by poor recruitment methods, particularly tobacco exposure. Reducing tobacco use in pregnancy and improving social equity on issues such as education, economics, employment, mental health, and access to early intervention services would likely have the greatest positive effect on children’s cognitive development after prenatal MAT exposure.

Back to top
Article Information

Accepted for Publication: January 28, 2020.

Published: March 18, 2020. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2020.1195

Open Access: This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the CC-BY License. © 2020 Nelson LF et al. JAMA Network Open.

Corresponding Author: Leah F. Nelson, MD, MS, Addiction Medicine Fellowship Program, Department of Family and Community Medicine, University of New Mexico, 1 University of New Mexico, MSC10 5040, Albuquerque, NM 87131 (lfnelson@salud.unm.edu).

Author Contributions: Dr Nelson had full access to all of the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.

Concept and design: Nelson, Hsi, Weitzen.

Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: Nelson, Yocum, Patel, Qeadan.

Drafting of the manuscript: Nelson, Patel.

Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Yocum, Patel, Qeadan, Hsi, Weitzen.

Statistical analysis: Nelson, Patel, Qeadan.

Obtained funding: Nelson.

Administrative, technical, or material support: Nelson.

Supervision: Nelson, Hsi, Weitzen.

Conflict of Interest Disclosures: None reported.

Funding/Support: This study was funded by grant 1H79TI081358-01 from the Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration and award UL1TR001449 from the National Institutes of Health.

Role of the Funder/Sponsor: The funders had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

Disclaimer: The views expressed herein do not necessarily reflect the official policies of the Department of Health and Human Services or the National Institutes of Health; nor does mention of trade names, commercial practices, or organizations imply endorsement by the US government.

Additional Contributions: Shiraz Mishra, MBBS, PhD, and Deirdre Hill, PhD (University of New Mexico School of Medicine), provided discussions during development of the data analysis plan for this project. They were not compensated for their time. We acknowledge the staff and families of children in the University of New Mexico FOCUS early intervention program who inspired this work.

References
1.
Haight  SC, Ko  JY, Tong  VT, Bohm  MK, Callaghan  WM.  Opioid use disorder documented at delivery hospitalization—United States, 1999-2014.  MMWR Morb Mortal Wkly Rep. 2018;67(31):845-849. doi:10.15585/mmwr.mm6731a1PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
American College of Obstetricians and Gynecologists; American Society of Addiction Medicine. ACOG Committee opinion: opioid use and opioid use disorder in pregnancy. Accessed February 6, 2020. https://www.acog.org/Clinical-Guidance-and-Publications/Committee-Opinions/Committee-on-Obstetric-Practice/Opioid-Use-and-Opioid-Use-Disorder-in-Pregnancy?IsMobileSet=false
3.
Sutter  MB, Leeman  L, Hsi  A.  Neonatal opioid withdrawal syndrome.  Obstet Gynecol Clin North Am. 2014;41(2):317-334. doi:10.1016/j.ogc.2014.02.010PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Raffaeli  G, Cavallaro  G, Allegaert  K,  et al.  Neonatal abstinence syndrome: update on diagnostic and therapeutic strategies.  Pharmacotherapy. 2017;37(7):814-823. doi:10.1002/phar.1954PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Wachman  EM, Schiff  DM, Silverstein  M.  Neonatal abstinence syndrome: advances in diagnosis and treatment.  JAMA. 2018;319(13):1362-1374. doi:10.1001/jama.2018.2640PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Baldacchino  A, Arbuckle  K, Petrie  DJ, McCowan  C.  Erratum: neurobehavioral consequences of chronic intrauterine opioid exposure in infants and preschool children: a systematic review and meta-analysis.  BMC Psychiatry. 2015;15:134. doi:10.1186/s12888-015-0438-5PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Konijnenberg  C, Lund  IO, Melinder  A.  Behavioural outcomes of four-year-old children prenatally exposed to methadone or buprenorphine: a test of three risk models.  Early Child Dev Care. 2015;185(10):1641-1657. doi:10.1080/03004430.2015.1016506Google ScholarCrossref
8.
Hatzis  D, Dawe  S, Harnett  P, Barlow  J.  Quality of caregiving in mothers with illicit substance use: a systematic review and meta-analysis.  Subst Abuse. 2017;11:1178221817694038. doi:10.1177/1178221817694038PubMedGoogle Scholar
9.
Kingston  D, McDonald  S, Austin  M-P, Tough  S.  Association between prenatal and postnatal psychological distress and toddler cognitive development: a systematic review.  PLoS One. 2015;10(5):e0126929. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0126929PubMedGoogle Scholar
10.
Lindenberg  CS, Keith  AB.  Opiate abuse in pregnancy.  Annu Rev Nurs Res. 1993;11:249-279. doi:10.1891/0739-6686.11.1.249PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
Green  CM, Berkule  SB, Dreyer  BP,  et al.  Maternal literacy and associations between education and the cognitive home environment in low-income families.  Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2009;163(9):832-837. doi:10.1001/archpediatrics.2009.136PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Yeoh  SL, Eastwood  J, Wright  IM,  et al.  Cognitive and motor outcomes of children with prenatal opioid exposure: a systematic review and meta-analysis.  JAMA Netw Open. 2019;2(7):e197025. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2019.7025PubMedGoogle Scholar
13.
Winklbaur  B, Baewert  A, Jagsch  R,  et al.  Association between prenatal tobacco exposure and outcome of neonates born to opioid-maintained mothers: implications for treatment.  Eur Addict Res. 2009;15(3):150-156. doi:10.1159/000216466PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
Blumenshine  P, Egerter  S, Barclay  CJ, Cubbin  C, Braveman  PA.  Socioeconomic disparities in adverse birth outcomes: a systematic review.  Am J Prev Med. 2010;39(3):263-272. doi:10.1016/j.amepre.2010.05.012PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Mactier  H, Shipton  D, Dryden  C, Tappin  DM.  Reduced fetal growth in methadone-maintained pregnancies is not fully explained by smoking or socio-economic deprivation.  Addiction. 2014;109(3):482-488. doi:10.1111/add.12400PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Stroup  DF, Berlin  JA, Morton  SC,  et al; Meta-analysis of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (MOOSE) Group.  Meta-analysis of Observational Studies in Epidemiology: a proposal for reporting.  JAMA. 2000;283(15):2008-2012. doi:10.1001/jama.283.15.2008PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
Moher  D, Liberati  A, Tetzlaff  J, Altman  DG; PRISMA Group.  Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement.  PLoS Med. 2009;6(7):e1000097. doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097PubMedGoogle Scholar
18.
Learning Sciences Resource Guide. Components of cognitive theory. Accessed June 17, 2019. https://canvas.vt.edu/courses/62492/pages/components-of-cognitive-theory
19.
Ryan  R, Synnot  A, Prictor  M, Hill  S. Cochrane Consumers and Communication Group Data extraction template for included studies. Accessed February 7, 2020. http://cccrg.cochrane.org/author-resources
20.
Balk  EM, Chung  M, Chen  ML, Trikalinos  TA, Kong Win Chang  L.  Assessing the Accuracy of Google Translate to Allow Data Extraction From Trials Published in Non-English Languages. Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality; 2013. Accessed June 24, 2019. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK121304/
21.
Downs  SH, Black  N.  The feasibility of creating a checklist for the assessment of the methodological quality both of randomised and non-randomised studies of health care interventions.  J Epidemiol Community Health. 1998;52(6):377-384. doi:10.1136/jech.52.6.377PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
Harrer  M, Cuijpers  P, Furukawa  TA, Ebert  DD. Doing meta-analysis in R: a hands-on guide. Accessed June 12, 2019. https://bookdown.org/MathiasHarrer/Doing_Meta_Analysis_in_R/
23.
Bakhireva  LN, Holbrook  BD, Shrestha  S,  et al.  Association between prenatal opioid exposure, neonatal opioid withdrawal syndrome, and neurodevelopmental and behavioral outcomes at 5-8 months of age.  Early Hum Dev. 2019;128:69-76. doi:10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2018.10.010PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Bauman  PS, Levine  SA.  The development of children of drug addicts.  Int J Addict. 1986;21(8):849-863. doi:10.3109/10826088609027399PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
Bunikowski  R, Grimmer  I, Heiser  A, Metze  B, Schäfer  A, Obladen  M.  Neurodevelopmental outcome after prenatal exposure to opiates.  Eur J Pediatr. 1998;157(9):724-730. doi:10.1007/s004310050923PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
Chasnoff  IJ, Schnoll  SH, Burns  WJ, Burns  K.  Maternal nonnarcotic substance abuse during pregnancy: effects on infant development.  Neurobehav Toxicol Teratol. 1984;6(4):277-280.PubMedGoogle Scholar
27.
Bernstein  VJ, Hans  SL.  Predicting the developmental outcome of two-year-old children born exposed to methadone: impact of social and environmental risk factors.  J Clin Child Psychol. 1994;23(4):349-359. doi:10.1207/s15374424jccp2304_1Google ScholarCrossref
28.
Hans  SL.  Developmental consequences of prenatal exposure to methadone.  Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1989;562:195-207. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.1989.tb21018.xPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
29.
Hunt  RW, Tzioumi  D, Collins  E, Jeffery  HE.  Adverse neurodevelopmental outcome of infants exposed to opiate in-utero.  Early Hum Dev. 2008;84(1):29-35. doi:10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2007.01.013PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
30.
Kaltenbach  K, Finnegan  LP.  Children exposed to methadone in utero: assessment of developmental and cognitive ability.  Ann NY Acad Sci. 1989;562(1):360-362. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.1989.tb21039.xGoogle ScholarCrossref
31.
Bernstein  V, Jeremy  RJ, Hans  SL, Marcus  J.  A longitudinal study of offspring born to methadone-maintained women II: dyadic interaction and infant behavior at 4 months.  Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse. 1984;10(2):161-193. doi:10.3109/00952998409002779PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
32.
Bernstein  VJ, Jeremy  RJ, Marcus  J.  Mother-infant interaction in multiproblem families: finding those at risk.  J Am Acad Child Psychiatry. 1986;25(5):631-640. doi:10.1016/S0002-7138(09)60287-9PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
33.
Rosen  TS, Johnson  HL.  Drug-addicted mothers, their infants, and SIDS.  Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1988;533:89-95. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.1988.tb37236.xPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
34.
Johnson  HL, Rosen  TS.  Prenatal methadone exposure: effects on behavior in early infancy.  Pediatr Pharmacol (New York). 1982;2(2):113-120.PubMedGoogle Scholar
35.
Johnson  HL, Diano  A, Rosen  TS.  24-Month neuro-behavioral follow-up of children of methadone-maintained mothers.  Infant Behav Dev. 1984;7(1):115-123. doi:10.1016/S0163-6383(84)80027-2Google ScholarCrossref
36.
Rosen  TS, Johnson  HL.  Long-term effects of prenatal methadone maintenance.  NIDA Res Monogr. 1985;59:73-83.PubMedGoogle Scholar
37.
Rosen  TS, Johnson  HL.  Children of methadone-maintained mothers: follow-up to 18 months of age.  J Pediatr. 1982;101(2):192-196. doi:10.1016/S0022-3476(82)80115-7PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
38.
Salo  S, Politi  J, Tupola  S,  et al.  Early development of opioid-exposed infants born to mothers in buprenorphine-replacement therapy.  J Reprod Infant Psychol. 2010;28(2):161-179. doi:10.1080/02646830903219109Google ScholarCrossref
39.
Strauss  ME, Starr  RH, Ostrea  EM, Chavez  CJ, Stryker  JC.  Behavioural concomitants of prenatal addiction to narcotics.  J Pediatr. 1976;89(5):842-846. doi:10.1016/S0022-3476(76)80822-0PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
40.
Strauss  ME, Lessen-Firestone  JK, Chavez  CJ, Stryker  JC.  Children of methadone-treated women at five years of age.  Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1979;11(suppl):3-6.PubMedGoogle Scholar
41.
van Baar  A.  Development of infants of drug dependent mothers.  J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1990;31(6):911-920. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1990.tb00833.xPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
42.
van Baar  AL, Fleury  P, Ultee  CA.  Behaviour in first year after drug dependent pregnancy.  Arch Dis Child. 1989;64(2):241-245. doi:10.1136/adc.64.2.241PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
43.
van Baar  A, de Graaff  BM.  Cognitive development at preschool-age of infants of drug-dependent mothers.  Dev Med Child Neurol. 1994;36(12):1063-1075. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8749.1994.tb11809.xPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
44.
Whitham  JN.  Prenatal Exposure to Buprenorphine or Methadone: Effects on Physical Growth, Neurological Development and Temperament in Early Childhood [thesis]. University of Adelaide; 2012. Accessed February 7, 2020. https://digital.library.adelaide.edu.au/dspace/handle/2440/72184
45.
Lifschitz  MH, Wilson  GS, Smith  EO, Desmond  MM.  Factors affecting head growth and intellectual function in children of drug addicts.  Pediatrics. 1985;75(2):269-274.PubMedGoogle Scholar
46.
Wilson  GS, Desmond  MM, Wait  RB.  Follow-up of methadone-treated and untreated narcotic-dependent women and their infants: health, developmental, and social implications.  J Pediatr. 1981;98(5):716-722. doi:10.1016/S0022-3476(81)80830-XPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
47.
Wilson  GS.  Clinical studies of infants and children exposed prenatally to heroin.  Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1989;562:183-194. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.1989.tb21017.xPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
48.
Davie-Gray  A.  The Early Development and Family Environments of Children Born to Mothers Engaged in Methadone Maintenance During Pregnancy [thesis]. University of Canterbury; 2011. Accessed July 31, 2018. https://ir.canterbury.ac.nz/handle/10092/5508
49.
Lee  SJ.  The School-Readiness of Children Born to Mothers Maintained on Methadone During Pregnancy [thesis]. University of Canterbury; 2012. Accessed March 2, 2018. https://ir.canterbury.ac.nz/handle/10092/9280
50.
Levine  TA, Woodward  LJ.  Early inhibitory control and working memory abilities of children prenatally exposed to methadone.  Early Hum Dev. 2018;116:68-75. doi:10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2017.11.010PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
51.
Baron  IS, Leonberger  KA.  Assessment of intelligence in the preschool period.  Neuropsychol Rev. 2012;22(4):334-344. doi:10.1007/s11065-012-9215-0PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
52.
Hurks  PPM, Bakker  H.  Assessing intelligence in children and youth living in the Netherlands.  Int J Sch Educ Psychol. 2016;4(4):266-275. doi:10.1080/21683603.2016.1166754Google ScholarCrossref
53.
Quintana  SM, Aboud  FE, Chao  RK,  et al.  Race, ethnicity, and culture in child development: contemporary research and future directions.  Child Dev. 2006;77(5):1129-1141. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00951.xPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
54.
Merhar  SL, McAllister  JM, Wedig-Stevie  KE, Klein  AC, Meinzen-Derr  J, Poindexter  BB.  Retrospective review of neurodevelopmental outcomes in infants treated for neonatal abstinence syndrome.  J Perinatol. 2018;38(5):587-592. doi:10.1038/s41372-018-0088-9PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
55.
Akerman  SC, Brunette  MF, Green  AI, Goodman  DJ, Blunt  HB, Heil  SH.  Treating tobacco use disorder in pregnant women in medication-assisted treatment for an opioid use disorder: a systematic review.  J Subst Abuse Treat. 2015;52:40-47. doi:10.1016/j.jsat.2014.12.002PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
56.
Magnusson  K. Interpreting Cohen’s d effect size. Published February 3, 2014. Accessed June 24, 2019. https://rpsychologist.com/d3/cohend/
57.
Nair  P, Schuler  ME, Black  MM, Kettinger  L, Harrington  D.  Cumulative environmental risk in substance abusing women: early intervention, parenting stress, child abuse potential and child development.  Child Abuse Negl. 2003;27(9):997-1017.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
58.
Schuler  ME, Nair  P, Kettinger  L.  Drug-exposed infants and developmental outcome: effects of a home intervention and ongoing maternal drug use.  Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2003;157(2):133-138. doi:10.1001/archpedi.157.2.133PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
59.
Temple  JA, Reynolds  AJ.  Benefits and costs of investments in preschool education: evidence from the Child-Parent Centers and related programs.  Econ Educ Rev. 2007;26(1):126-144. doi:10.1016/j.econedurev.2005.11.004Google ScholarCrossref
60.
Wong  HS, Santhakumaran  S, Cowan  FM, Modi  N; Medicines for Neonates Investigator Group.  Developmental assessments in preterm children: a meta-analysis.  Pediatrics. 2016;138(2):e20160251. doi:10.1542/peds.2016-0251PubMedGoogle Scholar
61.
Brito  NH, Fifer  WP, Amso  D,  et al.  Beyond the Bayley: neurocognitive assessments of development during infancy and toddlerhood.  Dev Neuropsychol. 2019;44(2):220-247. doi:10.1080/87565641.2018.1564310PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
×