Key PointsQuestion
What value do patients with advanced cancer place on tumor variation profiling?
Findings
In this survey study of 777 patients with advanced cancer, 689 individuals (89%) indicated that they would have tumor molecular profiling for as little as a 1% actionable return rate, but fewer were willing to pay for it. Country of birth, medical- or science-related occupation, attitude toward uncertainty, and perceived importance of tumor molecular profiling were associated with willingness to pay.
Meaning
Ability to pay may limit access to tumor molecular profiling; ongoing societal debate is required to establish the value of tumor molecular profiling and whether subsidization is necessary to ensure equity of access.
Importance
Use of tumor molecular profiling (MP) is entering routine clinical practice; however, little is known about how much and why patients value MP.
Objective
To examine the perceived value of MP to patients with advanced cancer and factors associated with perceived value.
Design, Setting, and Participants
A cross-sectional survey that included willingness-to-pay trade-off scenarios was administered to participants after consent and before MP. A total of 777 participants (94% response rate) were recruited from the Molecular Screening and Therapeutics Program. Eligible patients had advanced solid cancers of any histologic type, were receiving or had completed their last line of effective therapy, had an Eastern Cooperative Oncology Group Performance Status 0 to 3, and had sufficient accessible tissue for MP. The participants were recruited between October 24, 2017, and March 12, 2019, and data analysis was conducted from March 13 to April 14, 2019.
Main Outcomes and Measures
Willingness to pay for MP was assessed via hypothetical trade-off scenarios varying in the actionable return rate (1%, 20%, or 40%) and cost (A$0, A$300 [US$210], A$1000 [US $700], A$3000 [US $2100], or A$10 000 [US $7000]). Ordinal regressions were used to explore factors associated with willingness to have and pay for MP.
Results
Of 777 participants (405 women [52%]; mean [SD] age, 55.47 [14.26] years), 689 patients (89%) would have MP for as little as a 1% actionable return rate. Fifty-six patients (7%) would require at least a 20% return rate and 11 patients (1%) would require at least a 40% return rate. Fifteen patients (2%) consistently chose not to have the test; 6 participants (0.8%) had missing values on this item. Participants were willing to pay a median of A$1000 if the actionable return rate was 1% and A$3000 for an actionable return rate of 20% to 40%. Of 762 individuals who agreed to testing, 482 patients (64%) were consistently unwilling to pay A$10 000, regardless of the actionable return rate. Patients born in Australia or New Zealand were more likely to want MP (eg, participants born in South Asia had an ordered odds for the tipping point of 7.74 [95% CI, 1.67-36.05; P = .009] times higher than Australian- and/or New Zealand–born participants). Patients born in Australia or New Zealand were also more willing to pay A$1000 or A$3000 (eg, participants born in Western Europe had an ordered odds for the tipping point for paying A$1000 of 1.74 [95% CI, 1.01-3.00; P = .048] times higher than Australian- and/or New Zealand–born participants). People with a medical- or science-related occupation and with more negative attitudes toward uncertainty were more likely to pay A$10 000 (eg, A$10 000 tipping point–ordered odds of participants with a medical- or science-related occupation was 0.49 [95% CI, 0.7-0.87; P = .02] times that of participants without a medical- or science-related occupation).
Conclusions and Relevance
This study found apparent high interest in but lower willingness to pay for MP among patients with advanced cancer. Ability to pay may limit access to MP. Ongoing societal debate is required to establish the value of MP and whether subsidization is needed to ensure equity of access.
Oncology is leading the clinical integration of genomic technologies into medicine with an increasing focus on defining cancer by its molecular signatures as opposed to its histologic characteristics.1 In the advanced cancer setting, this process generally involves somatic molecular profiling (MP) via panel testing of solid tumors to identify cancer-specific gene variations to guide targeted treatment. While the potential benefit of MP is high, challenges to its routine use exist, in particular, assessment of the utility of returned results.
Molecular profiling of tumors can identify variants that (1) guide treatment through a standard regimen or clinical trial (clinically actionable), (2) cannot guide treatment (nonactionable), (3) have uncertain therapeutic potential, and (4) occasionally have a germline origin and, therefore, have relevance to the patient’s family. Currently, the likelihood of finding a clinically actionable variant and obtaining improved outcomes is unknown and evolving rapidly. In a study of patients with advanced breast cancer,2 of 297 participants with biopsies for genomic analysis, personalized treatment was available for 55 individuals (18.5%). Of 43 patients treated with 16 different targeted regimens, 4 patients had objective responses and 9 patients had stable disease. Similarly, in a population-based study of 875 patients with advanced non–small cell lung cancer who received broad-based genomic sequencing,3 less than 5% of the participants were given nonapproved targeted treatments based on sequencing results. These treatments were not independently associated with higher 12-month survival rates. However, a more recent study of biomarker-driven therapy in the I-PREDICT trial4 resulted in matched therapy in 49% of patients screened. Targeting of molecular alterations was correlated with significantly improved disease control rates, as well as longer progression-free and survival rates. As evidence advances quickly, the perceived value of MP has become a topic of heightened debate.
While there a number of stakeholders whose perspectives might inform this debate,5 the patient’s perspective is a highly relevant factor. In considering the value of MP, patients need to consider financial and other costs against perceived clinical benefits. Recently, the cost of MP in Australia has fallen from A$6000 (US $4200) to under A$1000 (US $700) (David M. Thomas, PhD, email communication, June 18, 2019). Nevertheless, the lower cost still represents a significant investment for the health system and potential financial hardship for patients. Another perceived cost may be time spent waiting for results6 during which living with uncertainty can cause uneasiness and anxiety.7 Patients may have additional concerns related to the privacy, security, and ethics of testing.8
Psychological theories suggest that individual differences may be factors in how patients evaluate clinical benefits vs costs. Protection motivation theory,9 for example, would suggest that individuals will value MP more and place less weight on costs if they perceive cancer to be a significant threat, feel vulnerable to cancer progression, are confident that MP-guided treatment will be effective, and are confident that they will cope with MP-guided treatment. Protection motivation theory has been successful in estimating health- and safety-related intentions and behaviors in a variety of contexts.9
Few studies have empirically assessed patients’ views on MP. One useful method for establishing value is the willingness-to pay approach. Willingness to pay is a measure of strength of preference for or value given to a commodity such as a medical treatment.10 Cuffe et al11 used this approach in examining the value of pharmacogenomic testing to identify the likelihood of experiencing significant chemotherapy-related toxic effects to patients with metastatic cancers. The investigators used hypothetical time, efficacy, and toxic effect trade-offs and willingness-to-pay scenarios. In 121 participants, 97.4% wanted pharmacogenomic testing that could detect the risk of severe toxic effects and were willing to accept a median cost of A$1000 (range, A$0-A$10 000) and a waiting time for results of 14 days (range, 1-90 days).
The present study aimed to examine the perceived value of MP by exploring willingness to have and pay for MP and associated factors in a cohort of patients who have consented to MP as part of a genomic study. On the basis of the literature12 and the protection motivation theory, we hypothesized that participants would highly value MP and that those who have children, a medical or other scientific background, a feeling of vulnerability to cancer progression, good knowledge of genomics, high self-efficacy in coping with MP-guided treatment, a negative attitude toward uncertainty, and high fear of cancer progression may value MP more than those without these characteristics.
This project is a substudy of the Molecular Screening and Therapeutics (MoST) Program,13 an ongoing study at the Garvan Institute of Medical Research in Sydney, Australia. The MoST Program is enrolling adult participants with pathologically confirmed advanced or metastatic solid cancers of any histologic type, either during or after their last line of effective therapy; participants have an Eastern Cooperative Oncology Group (ECOG) Performance Status of 0 to 3 (ie, 0, no restrictions; 1, patient is restricted in physically strenuous activity but ambulatory and able to carry out light or sedentary work; 2, patient is ambulatory more than 50% of waking hours and capable of all self-care but unable to carry out any work activities; 3, patient is capable of only limited self-care, confined to bed or chair more than 50% of waking hours)14 and sufficient accessible tissue for MP. If actionable findings are reported, participants may be enrolled in a targeted therapeutic trial or treatment. Recruitment for the MoST Program is through oncologist referral.
The Psychosocial Issues in Genomics in Oncology (PiGeOn) Project is a longitudinal, mixed-methods, psychosocial substudy of the MoST Program that aims to examine the psychosocial, behavioral, and ethical aspects of MP.15 Exclusion criteria are inability to adhere to study requirements, including completing assessments, or to provide written informed consent. Participants give written informed consent to participate in the PiGeOn Project while giving consent to participate in the MoST Program; no financial compensation was provided. The PiGeOn Project was approved by the St Vincent’s Hospital Human Research Ethics Committee, Sydney, Australia. This study followed the American Association for Public Opinion Research (AAPOR) reporting guideline.
PiGeOn Project participants complete a questionnaire at baseline (within 2 weeks of consenting to MP but before receipt of results), 2 to 3 months post baseline (shortly after receiving MP results), and 2 to 3 months later. This article reports results only from the baseline survey.
A hypothetical trade-off scenario using a contingent valuation willingness-to-pay approach12 based on previous studies11 was used to measure the degree to which participants value MP. Two approaches to willingness to pay have primarily been used in the literature: contingent valuation and discrete choice experiments. These approaches both have pros and cons and produce slightly different results. We chose contingent valuation because it is closer to the real choices consumers make.12 Patients were presented with 3 different scenarios with varying likelihoods that MP would find an actionable variant (1%, 20%, or 40%): If MP found a gene variant to guide treatment in about 1 in 100 people (1%), would you have the test? If MP found a gene variant to guide treatment in about 20 in 100 people (20%), would you have the test? If MP found a gene variant to guide treatment in about 40 in 100 people (40%), would you have the test? For each actionable result return rate, participants indicated whether they would have the test (yes/no) and, if yes, the highest amount they would pay (A$0, A$300 [US $210], A$1000 [US $700], A$3000 [US $2100], or A$10 000 [US $7000]). It was emphasized to participants that the choices were hypothetical, as they would be provided with MP at no cost within the MoST Program.
Demographic and psychological variables were collected. These measures included sex, age, educational level, cultural and linguistic background, socioeconomic status, country of birth, urban vs rural status, previous visit to a family cancer clinic, occupation, and parental status.
A 2-item measure adapted from Hay et al16 assessed perceived importance of genetic information to the participant, using a Likert-type scale. Higher scores indicate greater importance. Participants indicate their perceived likelihood of cancer progression on a visual analog scale from 0% (no chance of progression) to 100% (will definitely progress) adapted from a previous study.17
An 8-item, multiple-choice, purposively developed measure assessed knowledge of the purpose of MP, likely frequency of informative results, cancers in which informative results are more likely to be found, and availability of tailored treatment options. High scores (summed) indicate greater knowledge. The 3-item Concerns About Recurrence Questionnaire18 was adapted to measure fear of cancer progression. High scores indicate greater fear. The 7-item Attitude Toward Uncertainty scale19 measures uncertainty in the context of medical testing. Higher scores indicate negative attitudes toward uncertainty.
Four items adapted from Rosenberg et al20 assessed perceived ability to cope if actionable or nonactionable results are found. High scores indicate greater perceived ability to cope. The 6-item Satisfaction With Decision scale21 measures satisfaction with the person’s decision to have MP. High scores indicate greater satisfaction.
Descriptive statistics were used to summarize responses. Four tipping-point scores were computed for each participant using the hypothetical trade-off scenarios. The first tipping-point score was determined using the minimum actionable result return rate (1%, 20%, and 40%) required for patients to say yes to having the MP test. Patient responses formed 4 ordered categories: those who would have the test with a 1% likelihood, a 20% likelihood, or a 40% likelihood of finding an actionable variant, and those who consistently would not have the test. Hence, if a patient said no to the first 2 scenarios (1% and 20%) and then yes at 40%, they would receive a tipping-point score of 40, placing them in the third category. Three monetary tipping points were computed based on the actionable result return rate required for patients to spend A$1000, A$3000, and A$10 000 for the test. Lower tipping-point categories represent greater value placed on MP tests.
Pearson correlation coefficients were used to explore associations between variables of interest. Ordinal regressions were conducted on the trade-off categories, that is, participants’ tipping points for when they would have the test and for when they would pay A$1000, A$3000, or A$10 000 for MP as the outcome variables. All demographic and psychological factors measured were included as independent variables in these analyses. Listwise deletion was chosen to manage missing data as this process is known to produce unbiased estimates and conservative results.22 Wald tests were used to examine whether explanatory variables added significant explanatory power to the model.
The sample size and power calculation for this study are reported in the protocol.15 Owing to the large number of P values reported in this article, readers should be aware that the global type I error rates would be larger than expected by choosing a fixed significance-level cutoff. We therefore recommend that P values be treated as an indicator for the level of evidence for a difference in any comparison rather than as a binary outcome of significant and not significant. The American Statistical Association statement on P values provides further guidance on interpreting statistical results including P values.23 Data analysis was conducted using SPSS Statistics, version 25 (IBM Corp).
In this cross-sectional survey, using willingness-to-pay trade-off scenarios, 777 participants were recruited between October 24, 2017, and March 12, 2019, and completed the baseline questionnaire as part of the PiGeOn Project study (a 94% response rate). Data were analyzed between March 13 and April 14, 2019. Of the 52 nonresponders, 6 died shortly after consenting and the remainder chose not to participate, citing lack of interest, capacity, or time.
Participants had a mean (SD) age of 55.47 (14.26) years (interquartile range, 19-90 years), with 372 men (48%) and 405 women (52%) (Table 1). Nearly half (345 [44%]) had a university education, 59 patients (8%) had a medical/scientific background, and 86 patients (11%) had previously visited a family cancer clinic. There were 247 participants (32%) not born in Australia or New Zealand, with 184 individuals (24%) speaking a language other than English at home. Seventy-five patients (10%) lived in rural/remote areas of Australia and 574 patients (74%) had children. There were few missing data, the highest being 10 responses for any variable.
Responses to scenarios for receiving MP and willingness to pay are shown in Table 2, and tipping points are summarized in Table 3. Six hundred eighty-nine participants (89%) would have MP with only a 1% likelihood of finding an actionable variant. An additional 56 participants (7%) would require at least a 20% return rate and 11 (1%) participants would require at least a 40% return rate to have MP. Fifteen patients (2%) consistently chose not to have the test despite the increasing likelihood for detection, while 6 participants (0.8%) showed irregularly patterned responses.
For participants who would have the test, the median amount they were prepared to pay if the actionable return rate was 1% was A$1000, and if the actionable return rate was 20% to 40%, they were willing to pay A$3000 (Figure). Of 762 patients willing to undergo testing, 555 individuals (73%) would pay at least A$1000 for MP, with 434 patients (57%) willing to do so for a 1% chance of returning a clinically actionable result. Only 417 participants (55%) were willing to pay A$3000 for MP, of whom 286 patients (38%) were willing to do so for a 1% return of a clinically actionable result. Only 254 patients (33%) were willing to pay A$10 000, of whom 166 of 254 individuals (22%) were willing to do so for a 1% return rate. In all cases, the increase in return rate from 1% to 20% or 40% had a minor association with the decision to pay for the test.
Factors Associated With the Tipping Point
Country of birth and perceived importance of MP were associated with the tipping point for having MP (eFigure in the Supplement; Table 4). Participants born in South Asia had an ordered odds for the tipping point of 7.74 (95% CI, 1.67-36.05; P = .009) times higher than Australian- or New Zealand–born participants. Similarly, compared with participants born in Australia or New Zealand, the ordered odds for the tipping point for participants born in the Middle East or Northern Africa were 5.56 (95% CI, 1.75-17.62; P = .004) times greater; for those born in Central or South America, the ordered odds increased by a factor of 6.01 (95% CI, 1.21-29.90; P = .03); and for participants born in Western Europe, the ordered odds increased by a factor of 2.40 (95% CI, 1.06-5.45; P = .04). Thus, fewer participants born in these regions chose to have the test than participants born in Australia or New Zealand when there was a low likelihood of finding a result.
For each category increase in participants’ perceived importance of MP (eg, from agree to strongly agree), the tipping point–ordered odds decreased by a factor of 0.64 (95% CI, 0.44-0.92; P = .02). Hence, participants who perceived MP as being of greater importance were eager to have the test even when there was a low likelihood of finding a result. There was a significant, although low, correlation (r = 0.13, P = .001) between perceived importance of MP and thinking that MP can always guide treatment decisions (response to a question on the knowledge scale), suggesting that some participants with positive attitudes toward MP may not recognize the low likelihood of finding a result.
Tipping Point for Willingness to Pay
Participants’ country of birth was associated with the tipping point for paying A$1000 for MP. For participants born in Western Europe, the A$1000 tipping point–ordered odds increased by a factor of 1.74 (95% CI, 1.01-3.00; P = .048). Hence, these participants were less willing than those born in Australia or New Zealand to pay A$1000, even with a low likelihood of finding a result.
Country of birth, attitudes toward uncertainty, and perceived importance were associated with the tipping point for paying A$3000 for MP. For participants born in East Asia, the A$3000 tipping point–ordered odds increased by a factor of 2.13 (95% CI, 1.10-4.10; P = .02) compared with Australian- or New Zealand–born participants. For every 1-category increase in attitudes toward uncertainty (eg, from agree to strongly agree), the A$3000 tipping point–ordered odds decreased by a factor of 0.56 (95% CI, 0.40-0.80; P = .001). For every 1-category increase in perceived importance, the A$3000 tipping point–ordered odds decreased by a factor of 0.71 (95% CI, 0.52-0.96; P = .03). Thus, compared with East Asia participants, those born in Australia or New Zealand who had negative attitudes to uncertainty or who perceived MP as being of greater importance were willing to pay A$3000 at a low result return rate.
A medical- or science-related occupation and attitudes toward uncertainty were the only factors assocaited with the tipping point for paying A$10 000 for MP. The A$10 000 tipping point–ordered odds of participants with a medical- or science-related occupation was 0.49 (95% CI, 0.27-0.87; P = .02) times that of those without a medical- or science-related occupation. Thus, participants with a medical- or science-related occupation were more likely to pay A$10 000 for MP even at low rates of success, placing greater value on MP.
For each category increase in attitudes toward uncertainty, the A$10 000 tipping point–ordered odds decreased by a factor of 0.47 (95% CI, 0.32-0.70; Wald χ21 = 13.91; P < .001). Hence, participants with more negative attitudes toward uncertainty required a low likelihood of finding a result to pay A$10 000.
In this sample of 777 patients with advanced cancer who chose to receive MP as part of a large genomic study, most (89%) would choose to have MP, even if the likelihood of finding a gene variant that could guide treatment for their cancer was as low as 1%. Of the few who considered the likelihood of receiving an actionable result, relatively low actionable return rates of 20% or 40% tipped the balance.
These results complement the PiGeOn Project qualitative findings reported in 2019,24 which indicated that, for this group of participants who were reaching the end of their conventional cancer treatment options, any hope was better than none. As one man noted, “Well, it’s pretty simple. When you’ve got no other hope or no other opportunity and no other idea what the hell is going on, you grab every chance you can grab.”24 These patients saw MP as an opportunity to receive a tailored therapy that might increase their survival, and since the test was done on previously collected tumor tissue and was therefore noninvasive, to consent to it was described as a “no-brainer.”24 More unexpected in the present analysis were the few participants (n = 15) who chose not to undergo MP for any reason; responses were missing on 6 participants. Perhaps these patients were participating in the MoST Program for altruistic reasons and had greater faith in their oncologist to guide their care than in genomics.
More participants born in Australia or New Zealand chose to have the test when there was a low actionable return rate than participants born in other parts of the world. Similarly, these individuals were more willing to pay A$1000 or A$3000 for MP. Other studies have found that individuals of minority racial/ethnic groups or those who were immigrants had less-positive attitudes to genetic testing, feared discrimination as a result of testing, and anticipated a more negative emotional reaction to receipt of results than native-born participants,25-27 which may explain our result. We have also found higher levels of anxiety and depression in patients with cancer who were immigrants compared with native-born, as well as difficulty navigating the health system and understanding the English language,28 which may make genomic and treatment decisions more difficult.
Those who perceived MP as important were also more likely to choose to have MP, even with low return rates. The significant correlation between perceived importance and agreeing that MP is always helpful for guiding treatment decisions for cancer suggests that these participants may have believed there was a high likelihood of finding an actionable result, even when they were responding to a hypothetical low return rate.
Despite the strong value placed on MP among this cohort, when faced with a monetary value, significantly fewer were prepared to pay for MP, with 180 participants (24%) not willing to pay even A$1000, which is a realistic cost in Australia at the time of publication. The median amounts participants were prepared to pay was A$1000 if the actionable return rate was 1% and A$3000 for a 20% to 40% actionable return rate. Cuffe et al11 reported similar amounts when they asked patients with advanced cancer what they were willing to pay for genomic tests to identify the likelihood of experiencing significant chemotherapy-related toxic effects. Thus, across both treatment and toxicity outcomes, patients with cancer appear to be constrained in what they are willing to pay.
Low willingness to pay may reflect an underlying perception of the low likelihood of extended benefit from any treatment, tailored or not (and therefore a low benefit/cost ratio). However, we did not find associations between tipping points and knowledge, which included items evaluating understanding of the current limitations of MP. Unwillingness to pay may also reflect the lack of ability to pay. Cancer treatment is costly, and financial toxicity is a growing concern. A recent US study29 found that 42% of patients with cancer used all their available funds—an average of A$92 000—within 2 years of diagnosis. We did not ask patients about their income; however, socioeconomic status, based on postcode, was not associated with the tipping points. Postcode-based estimates provide only a moderate estimate of socioeconomic status according to a large, population-based study in the UK,30 so this variable does not conclusively rule out income barriers. We found that patients without a medical- or science-related occupation required greater benefit offered by the test to pay A$10 000 than those with a medical- or science-related occupation. However, this association likely reflected a greater understanding of and belief in science in those who worked in the field.
A recent survey reported by Chow-White and colleagues31 found that 61% of Canadian medical oncologists thought a major challenge to routine genomic testing was cost. Already some academics are questioning the amount of money being spent on genomic testing in the Canadian health system, given the relatively low return rates and improved outcomes.32 As a society, we need to continue to monitor the clinical utility of tests and the extent to which they can be implemented in the most cost-effective ways in clinical practice. Nonetheless, as 166 of our participants (22%) were prepared to pay A$10 000 regardless of actionable return rates, there is a clear need to openly discuss the option of MP with patients, if it were to become routinely available. Patients for whom uncertainty was more aversive were more willing to pay A$3000 or A$10 000 for MP at lower levels of return rate (eg, 1%), perhaps feeling that greater certainty on any front, regardless of eventual treatment benefit, was worthwhile in the context of failing standard treatment options and desperation.
Limitations and Strengths
Limitations of this study include the hypothetical nature of the questions posed, since these patients were receiving MP as part of a genomic study at no cost. Responses to hypothetical scenarios have been criticized as not accurately reflecting what people do in real life,30 but these scenarios are also recognized as having important advantages as a practical and efficient way to explore complex situations.33,34 Our sample had relatively good performance status (almost all were Eastern Cooperative Oncology Group status 0 or 1), disallowing analysis of performance status as a potential factor associated with perceived value. However, this cohort likely represents those who would be offered MP in clinical practice.
The study strengths include the fact that these participants had diseases for which MP might be used in routine practice and were offered MP; they were provided relevant information, including the low likelihood of an actionable result being found; and they were given time to consider the pros and cons of MP before reaching a decision regarding study participation. Furthermore, we had a large cohort with statistical power to assess factors with smaller effect sizes that were potentially associated with tipping points.
To our knowledge, this is the first study to quantitatively examine the value that patients with advanced cancer place on MP to guide treatment in the context of the failure of other therapies. We found that many participants were highly interested in MP, but were substantially influenced by considerations of cost. Nevertheless, some participants were prepared to pay a large amount for MP, particularly as the rate of informative results increased. This variability in response suggests that it is difficult for physicians to judge on behalf of patients what their treatment preferences will be in the context of complex trade-offs. This finding suggests that a policy of open disclosure regarding the option of MP should be fostered, but this practice is not straightforward to implement in the clinic. In our practice, we are reviewing consent processes to ensure that information is available and tailored to patient need to ensure informed consent.
Accepted for Publication: March 9, 2020.
Published: May 14, 2020. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2020.4721
Open Access: This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the CC-BY License. © 2020 Butow P et al. JAMA Network Open.
Corresponding Author: Phyllis Butow, PhD, Centre for Medical Psychology and Evidence-Based Decision-Making, The University of Sydney, Lifehouse C39Z, Level 6 North, Sydney, NSW 2006, Australia (phyllis.butow@sydney.edu.au).
Author Contributions: Dr Butow had full access to all of the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.
Concept and design: Butow, Davies, Best, Juraskova, Meiser, Goldstein, Thomas.
Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: Butow, Davies, Napier, Schlub, Best, Bartley, Juraskova, Meiser, Ballinger, Biesecker, Thomas.
Drafting of the manuscript: Butow, Davies, Napier, Best, Goldstein, Thomas.
Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Butow, Napier, Schlub, Best, Bartley, Juraskova, Meiser, Ballinger, Biesecker, Goldstein, Thomas.
Statistical analysis: Davies, Schlub.
Obtained funding: Butow, Best, Juraskova, Meiser, Thomas.
Administrative, technical, or material support: Napier, Best, Bartley, Ballinger, Thomas.
Supervision: Butow, Schlub, Best, Ballinger, Thomas.
Conflict of Interest Disclosures: Dr Butow reported receiving grants from the National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC) during the conduct of the study. Dr Thomas reported receiving grants, personal fees, and nonfinancial support from Roche, Pfizer, Bayer, and Astra Zeneca; grants and nonfinancial support from Eisai; and nonfinancial support from Amgen outside the submitted work; in addition, Dr Thomas reported serving as the chief executive officer of a nonprofit organization funded from multiple sources (government, philanthropic, and industry) to establish precision oncology infrastructure in Australia. No other disclosures were reported.
Funding/Support: This project was funded by NHMRC project grant 1124749. Dr Best is funded by a postdoctoral research fellowship from the Cancer Institute New South Wales. Dr Butow is funded by an NHMRC senior principal research fellowship. Dr Meiser holds an NHMRC senior research fellowship, level B. Dr Ballinger is supported by a Cancer Institute New South Wales career development fellowship. Dr Thomas holds an NHMRC principal research fellowship.
Role of the Funder/Sponsor: The NHMRC had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.
Group Information: Members of the PiGeOn Project include the named authors and Ainsley Newson, PhD: Sydney Health Ethics, Sydney School of Public Health, Faculty of Medicine and Health, The University of Sydney, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia; Katherine Tucker, MBBS, FRACP: Hereditary Cancer Clinic, Prince of Wales Hospital, Randwick, New South Wales, Australia; Richard Vines, BSc: Rare Cancers Australia, Bowral, New South Wales, Australia; Kate Vines: Rare Cancers Australia, Bowral, New South Wales, Australia; Judy Kirk, MBBS, FRACP: Familial Cancer Service, Crown Prince Mary Cancer Centre, Westmead Hospital, Westmead, New South Wales, Australia and Westmead Clinical School, Department of Medicine, The Westmead Institute for Medical Research, Westmead, New South Wales, Australia; Mary-Anne Young, MHSc: Cancer Division, Garvan Institute of Medical Research, Darlinghurst, New South Wales, Australia; Jacqueline Savard, PhD: Deakin University School of Medicine, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia; and Chris Jacobs, PhD: University of Technology Sydney Graduate School of Health, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia and Prince of Wales Clinical School, University of New South Wales, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia.
Additional Contributions: We thank the participants of the MoST Program PiGeOn Project for sharing their experiences and views.
1.McGowan
ML, Settersten
RA
Jr, Juengst
ET, Fishman
JR. Integrating genomics into clinical oncology: ethical and social challenges from proponents of personalized medicine.
Urol Oncol. 2014;32(2):187-192. doi:
10.1016/j.urolonc.2013.10.009
PubMedGoogle Scholar 2.André
F, Bachelot
T, Commo
F,
et al. Comparative genomic hybridisation array and DNA sequencing to direct treatment of metastatic breast cancer: a multicentre, prospective trial (SAFIR01/UNICANCER).
Lancet Oncol. 2014;15(3):267-274. doi:
10.1016/S1470-2045(13)70611-9
PubMedGoogle Scholar 3.Presley
CJ, Tang
D, Soulos
PR,
et al. Association of broad-based genomic sequencing with survival among patients with advanced non–small cell lung cancer in the community oncology setting.
JAMA. 2018;320(5):469-477. doi:
10.1001/jama.2018.9824
PubMedGoogle Scholar 5.Fuchs
VR.
Who Shall Live? Health Economics and Social Choice. 2nd expanded edition. World Scientific Publishing Co Pte Ltd; 2011. doi:
10.1142/8167 8.Clayton
EW, Halverson
CM, Sathe
NA, Malin
BA. A systematic literature review of individuals’ perspectives on privacy and genetic information in the United States.
PLoS One. 2018;13(10):e0204417. doi:
10.1371/journal.pone.0204417
PubMedGoogle Scholar 12.Danyliv
A, Pavlova
M, Gryga
I, Groot
W. Willingness to pay for physician services: comparing estimates from a discrete choice experiement and contingent valuation.
Soc Econ. 2012;34(2):339-357. doi:
10.1556/SocEc.34.2012.2.9
Google Scholar 13.Thavaneswaran
S, Sebastian
L, Ballinger
M,
et al. Cancer Molecular Screening and Therapeutics (MoST): a framework for multiple, parallel signal-seeking studies of targeted therapies for rare and neglected cancers.
Med J Aust. 2018;209(8):354-355. doi:
10.5694/mja18.00227
PubMedGoogle Scholar 15.Best
M, Newson
AJ, Meiser
B,
et al. The PiGeOn project: protocol for a longitudinal study examining psychosocial, behavioural and ethical issues and outcomes in cancer tumour genomic profiling.
BMC Cancer. 2018;18(1):389. doi:
10.1186/s12885-018-4310-0
PubMedGoogle Scholar 16.Hay
J, Kaphingst
KA, Baser
R, Li
Y, Hensley-Alford
S, McBride
CM. Skin cancer concerns and genetic risk information-seeking in primary care.
Public Health Genomics. 2012;15(2):57-72. doi:
10.1159/000330403
PubMedGoogle Scholar 17.Kasparian
NA, Meiser
B, Butow
PN, Simpson
JM, Mann
GJ. Genetic testing for melanoma risk: a prospective cohort study of uptake and outcomes among Australian families.
Genet Med. 2009;11(4):265-278. doi:
10.1097/GIM.0b013e3181993175
PubMedGoogle Scholar 18.Thewes
B, Zachariae
R, Christensen
S, Nielsen
T, Butow
P. The Concerns About Recurrence Questionnaire: validation of a brief measure of fear of cancer recurrence amongst Danish and Australian breast cancer survivors.
J Cancer Surviv. 2015;9(1):68-79. doi:
10.1007/s11764-014-0383-1
PubMedGoogle Scholar 19.Braithwaite
D, Sutton
S, Steggles
N. Intention to participate in predictive genetic testing for hereditary cancer: the role of attitude toward uncertainty.
Psychol Health. 2002;17(6):761-772. doi:
10.1080/0887044021000054764
Google Scholar 25.Peters
N, Rose
A, Armstrong
K. The association between race and attitudes about predictive genetic testing.
Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev. 2004;13(3):361-365.
PubMedGoogle Scholar 26.Thompson
HS, Valdimarsdottir
HB, Jandorf
L, Redd
W. Perceived disadvantages and concerns about abuses of genetic testing for cancer risk: differences across African American, Latina and Caucasian women.
Patient Educ Couns. 2003;51(3):217-227. doi:
10.1016/S0738-3991(02)00219-7
PubMedGoogle Scholar 27.Sussner
KM, Thompson
HS, Jandorf
L,
et al. The influence of acculturation and breast cancer-specific distress on perceived barriers to genetic testing for breast cancer among women of African descent.
Psychooncology. 2009;18(9):945-955. doi:
10.1002/pon.1492
PubMedGoogle Scholar 28.Butow
PN, Aldridge
L, Bell
ML,
et al. Inferior health-related quality of life and psychological well-being in immigrant cancer survivors: a population-based study.
Eur J Cancer. 2013;49(8):1948-1956. doi:
10.1016/j.ejca.2013.01.011
PubMedGoogle Scholar 30.Danesh
J, Gault
S, Semmence
J, Appleby
P, Peto
R. Postcodes as useful markers of social class: population based study in 26 000 British households.
BMJ. 1999;318(7187):843-844. doi:
10.1136/bmj.318.7187.843
PubMedGoogle Scholar 33.Ulrich
CM, Ratcliffe
SJ. Hypothetical vignettes in empirical bioethics research. In: Jacoby L, LA Siminoff LA, eds. Empirical Methods for Bioethics: A Primer. Elsevier; 2007: 161-181.