[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Figure 1.  S-GIMME Connectivity Results
S-GIMME Connectivity Results

A, Connections fit at the group-level (ie, statistically meaningful for ≥75% of sample) (n = 175). B, Subgroup-level connections for first algorithm-detected subgroup (n = 42). C, Subgroup-level connections for second algorithm-detected subgroup (n = 133). D, Individual-level connections for illustrative participant in first subgroup. E, Individual-level connections for illustrative participant in second subgroup. All connections are directed and contemporaneous. Red nodes are part of the salience network. Blue nodes are part of the default mode network.

Figure 2.  Association Between Childhood Violence Exposure and Reduced Network Density
Association Between Childhood Violence Exposure and Reduced Network Density

Number of connections modeled for each child. Adjusted R2 = 0.059.

Table 1.  Logistic Regression Results for Association Between Violence Exposure and Social Deprivation and Subgroup Membership While Controlling for Motiona
Logistic Regression Results for Association Between Violence Exposure and Social Deprivation and Subgroup Membership While Controlling for Motiona
Table 2.  Regression Results for Association Between Dimensional Exposure to Adversity and Network Density and Node Degreea,b
Regression Results for Association Between Dimensional Exposure to Adversity and Network Density and Node Degreea,b
Individual Model Maps
1.
Finkelhor  D, Turner  HA, Shattuck  A, Hamby  SL.  Prevalence of childhood exposure to violence, crime, and abuse: results from the National Survey of Children’s Exposure to Violence.   JAMA Pediatr. 2015;169(8):746-754. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2015.0676 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
Green  JG, McLaughlin  KA, Berglund  PA,  et al.  Childhood adversities and adult psychiatric disorders in the national comorbidity survey replication I: associations with first onset of DSM-IV disorders.   Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67(2):113-123. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.186 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
Nusslock  R, Miller  GE.  Early-life adversity and physical and emotional health across the lifespan: a neuroimmune network hypothesis.   Biol Psychiatry. 2016;80(1):23-32. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.05.017 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
McLaughlin  KA, Sheridan  MA, Lambert  HK.  Childhood adversity and neural development: deprivation and threat as distinct dimensions of early experience.   Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2014;47:578-591. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.10.012 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Hein  TC. Dimensions of early adversity as distinct predictors of adolescent brain development. Published 2019. Accessed March 19, 2019. https://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/handle/2027.42/149945/heint_1.pdf?sequence=1&isAllowed=y
6.
Goetschius  LG, Hein  TC, Mitchell  C,  et al.  Childhood violence exposure and social deprivation predict adolescent amygdala-orbitofrontal cortex white matter connectivity.   Dev Cogn Neurosci. Published online July 8, 2020. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2020.100822Google ScholarCrossref
7.
Uddin  LQ. Functions of the salience network. In:  Salience Network of the Human Brain. Academic Press; 2017:11-16. doi:10.1016/B978-0-12-804593-0.00003-5
8.
Menon  V.  Large-scale brain networks and psychopathology: a unifying triple network model.   Trends Cogn Sci. 2011;15(10):483-506. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2011.08.003 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Marusak  HA, Etkin  A, Thomason  ME.  Disrupted insula-based neural circuit organization and conflict interference in trauma-exposed youth.   Neuroimage Clin. 2015;8:516-525. doi:10.1016/j.nicl.2015.04.007 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Marusak  HA, Martin  KR, Etkin  A, Thomason  ME.  Childhood trauma exposure disrupts the automatic regulation of emotional processing.   Neuropsychopharmacology. 2015;40(5):1250-1258. doi:10.1038/npp.2014.311 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
van der Werff  SJA, Pannekoek  JN, Veer  IM,  et al.  Resting-state functional connectivity in adults with childhood emotional maltreatment.   Psychol Med. 2013;43(9):1825-1836. doi:10.1017/S0033291712002942 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Falk  EB, Hyde  LW, Mitchell  C,  et al.  What is a representative brain? neuroscience meets population science.   Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2013;110(44):17615-17622. doi:10.1073/pnas.1310134110 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Beltz  AM, Gates  KM.  Network mapping with GIMME.   Multivariate Behav Res. 2017;52(6):789-804. doi:10.1080/00273171.2017.1373014 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
Sporns  O.  Networks of the Brain. MIT Press; 2010.
15.
Power  JD, Fair  DA, Schlaggar  BL, Petersen  SE.  The development of human functional brain networks.   Neuron. 2010;67(5):735-748. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2010.08.017 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Fair  DA, Cohen  AL, Power  JD,  et al.  Functional brain networks develop from a “local to distributed” organization.   PLoS Comput Biol. 2009;5(5):e1000381. doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000381 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
Marder  E, Goaillard  J-M.  Variability, compensation and homeostasis in neuron and network function.   Nat Rev Neurosci. 2006;7(7):563-574. doi:10.1038/nrn1949 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
Molenaar  PCM.  A manifesto on psychology as idiographic science: bringing the person back into scientific psychology, this time forever.   Meas Interdiscip Res Perspect. 2004;2(4):201-218. doi:10.1207/s15366359mea0204_1 Google ScholarCrossref
19.
Gordon  EM, Laumann  TO, Gilmore  AW,  et al.  Precision functional mapping of individual human brains.   Neuron. 2017;95(4):791-807.e7. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2017.07.011 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Hughes  K, Bellis  MA, Hardcastle  KA,  et al.  The effect of multiple adverse childhood experiences on health: a systematic review and meta-analysis.   Lancet Public Health. 2017;2(8):e356-e366. doi:10.1016/S2468-2667(17)30118-4 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
Gee  DG, Gabard-Durnam  LJ, Flannery  J,  et al.  Early developmental emergence of human amygdala-prefrontal connectivity after maternal deprivation.   Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2013;110(39):15638-15643. doi:10.1073/pnas.1307893110 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
Hanson  JL, Nacewicz  BM, Sutterer  MJ,  et al.  Behavioral problems after early life stress: contributions of the hippocampus and amygdala.   Biol Psychiatry. 2015;77(4):314-323. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.04.020 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
23.
Hanson  JL, Hariri  AR, Williamson  DE.  Blunted ventral striatum development in adolescence reflects emotional neglect and predicts depressive symptoms.   Biol Psychiatry. 2015;78(9):598-605. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.05.010 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Lane  ST, Gates  KM, Molenaar  PCM. GIMME: group iterative multiple model estimation. 2019. Accessed February 28, 2019. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=gimme
25.
Gates  KM, Molenaar  PCM, Hillary  FG, Ram  N, Rovine  MJ.  Automatic search for fMRI connectivity mapping: an alternative to Granger causality testing using formal equivalences among SEM path modeling, VAR, and unified SEM.   Neuroimage. 2010;50(3):1118-1125. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.117 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
Reichman  NE, Teitler  JO, Garfinkel  I, McLanahan  SS.  Fragile families: sample and design.   Child Youth Serv Rev. 2001;23(4-5):303-326. doi:10.1016/S0190-7409(01)00141-4 Google ScholarCrossref
27.
Princeton University. Fragile families and child wellbeing study. Published 2019. Accessed October 20, 2019. https://fragilefamilies.princeton.edu/data-and-documentation/public-data-documentation
28.
Peckins  MK, Roberts  AG, Hein  TC,  et al.  Violence exposure and social deprivation is associated with cortisol reactivity in urban adolescents.   Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2020;111:104426. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2019.104426PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
29.
McLaughlin  KA, Sheridan  MA.  Beyond cumulative risk: a dimensional approach to childhood adversity.   Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2016;25(4):239-245. doi:10.1177/0963721416655883 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
30.
Carskadon  MA, Acebo  C.  A self-administered rating scale for pubertal development.   J Adolesc Health. 1993;14(3):190-195. doi:10.1016/1054-139X(93)90004-9 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
31.
Shaw  DS, Gilliom  M, Ingoldsby  EM, Nagin  DS.  Trajectories leading to school-age conduct problems.   Dev Psychol. 2003;39(2):189-200. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.39.2.189 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
32.
Masten  AS, Neemann  J, Andenas  S.  Life events and adjustment in adolescents: the significance of event independence, desirability, and chronicity.   J Res Adolesc. 1994;4(1):71-97. doi:10.1207/s15327795jra0401_5 Google ScholarCrossref
33.
Farrell  AD, Ampy  LA, Meyer  AL.  Identification and assessment of problematic interpersonal situations for urban adolescents.   J Clin Child Psychol. 1998;27(3):293-305. doi:10.1207/s15374424jccp2703_6 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
34.
Glover  GH, Law  CS.  Spiral-in/out BOLD fMRI for increased SNR and reduced susceptibility artifacts.   Magn Reson Med. 2001;46(3):515-522. doi:10.1002/mrm.1222 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
35.
Jenkinson  M, Beckmann  CF, Behrens  TEJ, Woolrich  MW, Smith  SM.  FSL.   Neuroimage. 2012;62(2):782-790. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.015 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
36.
Beltz  AM, Dotterer  HL, Goetschius  LG. GIMME preprocessing: initial release. Published May 9, 2019.
37.
Jenkinson  M, Bannister  P, Brady  M, Smith  S.  Improved optimization for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain images.   Neuroimage. 2002;17(2):825-841. doi:10.1006/nimg.2002.1132 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
38.
Pruim  RHR, Mennes  M, van Rooij  D, Llera  A, Buitelaar  JK, Beckmann  CF.  ICA-AROMA: a robust ICA-based strategy for removing motion artifacts from fMRI data.   Neuroimage. 2015;112(suppl C):267-277. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.02.064 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
39.
Yarkoni  T, Poldrack  RA, Nichols  TE, Van Essen  DC, Wager  TD.  Large-scale automated synthesis of human functional neuroimaging data.   Nat Methods. 2011;8(8):665-670. doi:10.1038/nmeth.1635 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
40.
Gates  KM, Lane  ST, Varangis  E, Giovanello  K, Guskiewicz  K.  Unsupervised classification during time-series model building.   Multivariate Behav Res. 2017;52(2):129-148. doi:10.1080/00273171.2016.1256187 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
41.
Sörbom  D.  Model modification.   Psychometrika. 1989;54(3):371-384. doi:10.1007/BF02294623 Google ScholarCrossref
42.
Gates  KM, Molenaar  PCM.  Group search algorithm recovers effective connectivity maps for individuals in homogeneous and heterogeneous samples.   Neuroimage. 2012;63(1):310-319. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.06.026 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
43.
Brown  TA.  Confirmatory Factor Analysis for Applied Research. Guilford Press; 2006.
44.
Gates  KM, Molenaar  PCM, Iyer  SP, Nigg  JT, Fair  DA.  Organizing heterogeneous samples using community detection of GIMME-derived resting state functional networks.   PLoS One. 2014;9(3):e91322. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0091322 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
45.
Price  RB, Lane  S, Gates  K,  et al.  Parsing heterogeneity in the brain connectivity of depressed and healthy adults during positive mood.   Biol Psychiatry. 2017;81(4):347-357. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2016.06.023 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
46.
Smith  SM.  The future of FMRI connectivity.   Neuroimage. 2012;62(2):1257-1266. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.022 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
47.
McCarty  R.  Learning about stress: neural, endocrine and behavioral adaptations.   Stress. 2016;19(5):449-475. doi:10.1080/10253890.2016.1192120 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
48.
Gard  AM, Waller  R, Shaw  DS, Forbes  EE, Hariri  AR, Hyde  LW.  The long reach of early adversity: parenting, stress, and neural pathways to antisocial behavior in adulthood.   Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2017;2(7):582-590. doi:10.1016/j.bpsc.2017.06.005 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
49.
Holz  NE, Boecker-Schlier  R, Buchmann  AF,  et al.  Ventral striatum and amygdala activity as convergence sites for early adversity and conduct disorder.   Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2017;12(2):261-272. doi:10.1093/scan/nsw120 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
50.
Sridharan  D, Levitin  DJ, Menon  V.  A critical role for the right fronto-insular cortex in switching between central-executive and default-mode networks.   Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2008;105(34):12569-12574. doi:10.1073/pnas.0800005105 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
51.
Philip  NS, Sweet  LH, Tyrka  AR,  et al.  Exposure to childhood trauma is associated with altered n-back activation and performance in healthy adults: implications for a commonly used working memory task.   Brain Imaging Behav. 2016;10(1):124-135. doi:10.1007/s11682-015-9373-9 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
52.
Teicher  MH, Anderson  CM, Ohashi  K, Polcari  A.  Childhood maltreatment: altered network centrality of cingulate, precuneus, temporal pole and insula.   Biol Psychiatry. 2014;76(4):297-305. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.09.016 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
53.
Eavani  H, Satterthwaite  TD, Filipovych  R, Gur  RE, Gur  RC, Davatzikos  C.  Identifying sparse connectivity patterns in the brain using resting-state fMRI.   Neuroimage. 2015;105:286-299. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.09.058 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
54.
Chugani  HT, Behen  ME, Muzik  O, Juhász  C, Nagy  F, Chugani  DC.  Local brain functional activity following early deprivation: a study of postinstitutionalized Romanian orphans.   Neuroimage. 2001;14(6):1290-1301. doi:10.1006/nimg.2001.0917 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
55.
Sheridan  MA, McLaughlin  KA.  Dimensions of early experience and neural development: deprivation and threat.   Trends Cogn Sci. 2014;18(11):580-585. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2014.09.001 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
Original Investigation
Pediatrics
September 23, 2020

Association of Childhood Violence Exposure With Adolescent Neural Network Density

Author Affiliations
  • 1Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor
  • 2Serious Mental Illness Treatment Resource and Evaluation Center, Office of Mental Health and Suicide Prevention, Department of Veterans Affairs, Ann Arbor, Michigan
  • 3Department of Sociology, Princeton University, Princeton, New Jersey
  • 4Teachers College & College of Physicians and Surgeons, Columbia University, New York, New York
  • 5Survey Research Center of the Institute for Social Research, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor
  • 6Population Studies Center of the Institute for Social Research, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor
  • 7Neuroscience Graduate Program, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor
  • 8Department of Psychiatry, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor
JAMA Netw Open. 2020;3(9):e2017850. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2020.17850
Key Points

Question  Are violence exposure and social deprivation associated with person-specific patterns (heterogeneity) of adolescent resting-state functional connectivity?

Findings  In this cohort study of 175 adolescents, childhood violence exposure, but not social deprivation, was associated with reduced adolescent resting-state density of the salience and default mode networks. A data-driven algorithm, blinded to childhood adversity, identified youth with heightened violence exposure based on resting-state connectivity patterns.

Meaning  Childhood violence exposure appears to be associated with adolescent functional connectivity heterogeneity, which may reflect person-specific neural plasticity and should be considered in neuroscience-based interventions.

Abstract

Importance  Adverse childhood experiences are a public health issue with negative sequelae that persist throughout life. Current theories suggest that adverse childhood experiences reflect underlying dimensions (eg, violence exposure and social deprivation) with distinct neural mechanisms; however, research findings have been inconsistent, likely owing to variability in how the environment interacts with the brain.

Objective  To examine whether dimensional exposure to childhood adversity is associated with person-specific patterns in adolescent resting-state functional connectivity (rsFC), defined as synchronized activity across brain regions when not engaged in a task.

Design, Setting, and Participants  A sparse network approach in a large sample with substantial representation of understudied, underserved African American youth was used to conduct an observational, population-based longitudinal cohort study. A total of 183 adolescents aged 15 to 17 years from Detroit, Michigan; Toledo, Ohio; and Chicago, Illinois, who participated in the Fragile Families and Child Wellbeing Study were eligible for inclusion. Environmental data from birth to adolescence were collected via telephone and in-person interviews, and neuroimaging data collected at a university lab. The study was conducted from February 1, 1998, to April 26, 2017, and data analysis was performed from January 3, 2019, to May 22, 2020.

Exposures  Composite variables representing violence exposure and social deprivation created from primary caregiver reports on children at ages 3, 5, and 9 years.

Main Outcomes and Measures  Resting-state functional connectivity person-specific network metrics (data-driven subgroup membership, density, and node degree) focused on connectivity among a priori regions of interest in 2 resting-state networks (salience network and default mode) assessed with functional magnetic resonance imaging.

Results  Of the 183 eligible adolescents, 175 individuals (98 girls [56%]) were included in the analysis; mean (SD) age was 15.88 (0.53) years and 127 participants (73%) were African American. Adolescents with high violence exposure were 3.06 times more likely (95% CI, 1.17-8.92) to be in a subgroup characterized by high heterogeneity (few shared connections) and low network density (sparsity). Childhood violence exposure, but not social deprivation, was associated with reduced rsFC density (β = −0.25; 95% CI, −0.41 to −0.05; P = .005), with fewer salience network connections (β = −0.26; 95% CI, −0.43 to −0.08; P = .005) and salience network-default mode connections (β = −0.20; 95% CI, −0.38 to −0.03; P = .02). Violence exposure was associated with node degree of right anterior insula (β = −0.29; 95% CI, −0.47 to −0.12; P = .001) and left inferior parietal lobule (β = −0.26; 95% CI, −0.44 to −0.09; P = .003).

Conclusions and Relevance  The findings of this study suggest that childhood violence exposure is associated with adolescent neural network sparsity. A community-detection algorithm, blinded to child adversity, grouped youth exposed to heightened violence based only on patterns of rsFC. The findings may have implications for understanding how dimensions of adverse childhood experiences impact individualized neural development.

Introduction

Adversity during childhood is a common, detrimental public health issue. Adverse childhood experiences negatively impact physical and mental health, and effects likely persist into adulthood.1-3 Early adverse environments have underlying dimensions, such as violence exposure (eg, neighborhood violence) and social deprivation (eg, neglect),4,5 which have distinct neural correlates related to emotion, fear, and reward processing.4,6 For instance, violence exposure and social deprivation are associated with blunted amygdala and ventral striatum reactivity, respectively.5 However, it is unclear how these dimensions affect neural circuitry. Adverse childhood experiences not measured dimensionally are associated with alterations in resting-state functional connectivity (rsFC) of the salience network (SN), which is a task-positive network including the anterior insula that is involved in identifying and integrating salient input,7 and the default mode network (DMN), which is a task-negative network including the inferior parietal lobule that is linked to internal thought, memory, and social-cognitive processes.8-11 However, inferences are limited by relatively small, homogeneous samples focused on few brain regions using retrospective reports of adversity.12 Thus, there are significant knowledge gaps concerning the ways in which early violence exposure and social deprivation impact later functioning of neural circuits and how that impact varies across individuals.

Neural circuits are typically studied using a network framework, with key features including density (ie, number of connections13) and node degree (ie, number of connections involving a specific brain region).14,15 Across development, network density increases between distal nodes and node degree increases for hub regions, such as the anterior insula.16 Understanding how early adversity relates to network density has a potential for revealing how the environment affects brain development.14

These influences are likely to be person specific because there is considerable variability in neural responses to environmental stress,17 and thus, mean-based analyses may not accurately reflect an individual’s circuitry.18 Data from neuroimaging projects, such as the Midnight Scan Club, have illustrated that the organization of an individual’s rsFC is unique and qualitatively different from the group average.19 Moreover, consistent with behavioral studies of early adversity,20 average estimates of adversity’s effects on the brain often have high variances.21-23 This variance raises the question of whether there is information about individual differences and their causes—important for eventual prevention and intervention—that is not being conveyed by mean-based conclusions.

In the present study, we examined the association between dimensional indexes of childhood exposure and individualized adolescent rsFC networks. We used a large, longitudinal sample of adolescents with a substantial representation of African American and low-income participants, who are often underrepresented in neuroimaging research,12 and a person-specific rsFC approach that detects meaningful connections among brain regions while identifying subgroups of participants that share network features (group iterative multiple model estimation [GIMME]).24,25 We hypothesized that childhood violence exposure and social deprivation would be associated with person-specific indexes of SN and DMN density, respectively. This study was preregistered with the Center for Open Science.

Methods

Participants were from the Fragile Families and Child Wellbeing Study (FFCWS), a population-based cohort study of children born in large US cities, with an oversample of nonmarital births as well as a large proportion of families of racial/ethnic minorities with low resources.26 This study followed the Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) reporting guideline for cohort studies. This study was approved by the University of Michigan Institutional Review Board. Caregivers and participants provided informed consent or assent. Participants received financial compensation.

In the FFCWS, data were collected from the primary caregiver (94% biological mothers) and focal child at their birth and at ages 1, 3, 5, 9, and 15 years through in-home visits and phone calls.27 Data for the violence exposure and social deprivation composites, which have been previously reported,5,6,28 came from surveys collected at ages 3, 5, and 9 years. During wave 6 (when the focal child was approximately age 15 years), 237 adolescents from Detroit, Michigan; Toledo, Ohio; and Chicago, Illinois, participated in a supplementary visit during which rsFC data were collected. The study was conducted from February 1, 1998, to April 26, 2017, and data analysis was performed from January 3, 2019, to May 22, 2020. To our knowledge, rsFC data and their association with early adversity have not been previously published. Owing to the representative sampling of the FFCWS, youth were not preselected based on their willingness to participate in a magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) study, which is a common procedure in neuroimaging. This sampling approach led to missing or incomplete MRI data (n = 54) but was not statistically significantly different from the recruited sample (eMethods and eTable 1 in the Supplement).

Participants and Procedure

A total of 183 adolescents aged 15 to 17 years were eligible for inclusion. The participants and their primary caregiver came to a university lab and completed questionnaires regarding the focal child’s current life stress, pubertal development, and other demographic variables. During an MRI scan, 8 minutes of data were collected while the adolescents were instructed to remain still and focus on a white fixation cross on a black screen.

Violence exposure and social deprivation were each operationalized by composite z scores calculated from FFCWS data at ages 3, 5, and 9 years; thus, 0 is the approximate mean. Both constructs included primary caregiver’s report of experiences that directly (eg, physical abuse) and indirectly (eg, community support) affect the child (eMethods in the Supplement). Violence exposure was operationalized as physical or emotional abuse directed at the child, exposure to intimate partner violence, and witnessing or being victimized by community violence. Social deprivation was operationalized as emotional or physical neglect, lack of romantic partner support for the mother, and lack of neighborhood cohesion.5 To reflect a comprehensive assessment of cumulative, dimensional childhood exposure to violence and social deprivation, both constructs included experiences with several levels of proximity to the child (eg, home and neighborhood) at multiple ages.5,29

To address potential confounding factors, sensitivity analyses adjusted for sex, race, pubertal development,30 adolescent life stress,31-33 maternal educational level at the child’s birth, and maternal marital status at the child’s birth26 (eMethods in the Supplement).

Neuroimaging Measures
MR Acquisition

Magnetic resonance data were acquired using a 3T scanner with an 8-channel head coil (GE Discovery MR750, GE Healthcare). Head movement was minimized through instructions to the participant and padding was placed around the head. Functional T2*-weighted blood oxygenation level–dependent images were acquired using a reverse spiral sequence34 of 40 contiguous axial 3-mm slices (eMethods in the Supplement).

Imaging Data Analysis

Preprocessing was primarily conducted in FSL, version 5.0.7.35,36 Structural images were skull-stripped and segmented. Functional images were skull-stripped, spatially smoothed, registered to subject-specific structural and Montreal Neurological Institute space, and corrected for motion using MCFLIRT37 and ICA-AROMA.38 Nuisance signal from white matter and cerebrospinal fluid was removed. Data were high-pass filtered. As an additional precaution against motion-related artifacts, participants with an average relative framewise displacement greater than 0.5 mm before motion preprocessing were excluded (n = 4) (eMethods in the Supplement).

Participant-specific time series (235 functional volumes) from 7 regions of interest (ROIs) per hemisphere (14 total) were extracted. The ROIs and their locations were selected using Neurosynth39 and preregistered. Four bilateral ROIs defined the SN: amygdala, insula, dorsal anterior cingulate cortex, and dorsal prefrontal cortex. Three bilateral ROIs defined the DMN: inferior parietal lobule (IPL), posterior cingulate cortex, and medial temporal gyrus. Regions of interest were 6.5-mm spheres around central coordinates (eTable 2 in the Supplement) linearly adjusted for participant brain volume.

GIMME

Subgrouping GIMME (S-GIMME, version 0.5.1)24,25,40 in R, version 3.5.1 (The R Project for Statistical Computing) was used for rsFC analyses (eFigure 1 in the Supplement). Beginning with an empty network, S-GIMME fits person-specific unified structural equation models25 in a data-driven manner by using Lagrange multiplier tests41 to add directed connections among ROIs that are contemporaneous (occurring at the same functional volume) or lagged (occurring at the previous volume, including autoregressives) and that apply to the group level (everyone in the sample), the subgroup level (everyone in a data-derived subsample), or individual level (unique to an individual). S-GIMME is a sparse mapping approach in which only connections that account for a significant amount of variance are added to each participant’s network until the model fits the data well according to standard fit indexes (root-mean-square error of approximation ≤0.05; standard root mean residual ≤0.05; confirmatory fit index ≥0.95; and nonnormed fit index ≥0.95).42,43 During model generation, connections that have become nonsignificant with the addition of new connections are pruned. S-GIMME uses a community detection algorithm (Walktrap) to detect subgroups of participants and determine their shared connectivity patterns. S-GIMME has been described and validated in large-scale simulations and applied to empirical data.40,42,44,45 Network density and node degree of only the contemporaneous connections were extracted from person-specific S-GIMME networks, because lagged connections control for sequential dependencies (eg, hemodynamic response function).25,46

Statistical Analysis

Inferential analyses were completed in R, version 3.5.1 and examined whether childhood adversity statistically significantly predicted adolescent neural network features. First, binary logistic regression was used to statistically predict S-GIMME–detected subgroup membership from childhood violence exposure and social deprivation. Second, multiple regression was used to statistically predict density (ie, number of connections) within the SN, within the DMN, and between the SN and DMN from childhood violence exposure and social deprivation. Third, multiple regression was used to statistically predict node degree (ie, number of connections involving a specified node) for each of the 14 ROIs from violence exposure and social deprivation using a Bonferroni-corrected significance threshold (P < .004). In follow-up sensitivity analyses, covariates were added to all regressions to assess the robustness of observed effects.

Results

Of the 183 participants, 175 adolescents aged 15 to 17 years (mean [SD] age, 15.88 [0.53] years) were included in the analysis. Of the 183 SAND adolescents with resting-state MRI data, 3 were excluded owing to artifacts in the functional or structural MRI data, 4 had excessive motion (as defined by average relative framewise displacement >0.5 mm), and 1 had signal dropout in the areas of the brain included in the present analysis. Of these 175 adolescents, 98 participants (56%) were girls, 77 participants (44%) were boys, and 127 participants (73%) were African American. All person-specific resting state networks fit the data well, according to average indexes: root-mean-square error of approximation = 0.06, standard root mean residual = 0.05, confirmatory fit index = 0.93, and nonnormed fit index = 0.96 (individual indexes in eTable 7 in the Supplement). Group-level connections were detected within and between the SN and DMN (Figure 1A), 2 subgroups of participants with subgroup-level connections were identified (Figure 1B, C), and all person-specific maps contained individual-level connections (mean [SD], 11.61 [5.32]) (eFigure 2 in the Supplement and the Video show individual maps). Final maps revealed that the first subgroup (n = 42) (Figure 1B, D) was qualitatively homogeneous with 27 subgroup-level connections and few individual-level connections (mean [SD], 5.6 [03.19]), and that the second subgroup (n = 133) Figure 1C, E) was qualitatively heterogeneous, with 8 subgroup-level connections and many individual-level connections (mean [SD], 13.50 [4.36]). Possible extreme outliers or overfit models did not impact results (eMethods in the Supplement).

Violence exposure was associated with subgroup membership (b = 1.12, P = .03). With a unit increase in violence exposure, participants were 3.06 (95% CI, 1.17-8.92) times more likely to be classified in the larger, heterogeneous subgroup (Table 1). In sensitivity analyses, the odds ratio was 2.54 (eTable 6 in the Supplement). Members of the heterogeneous subgroup experienced higher levels of childhood violence exposure (mean [SD], 0.09 [0.53]) than those in the homogeneous subgroup (mean [SD], −0.15 [0.43]). Social deprivation was not associated with subgroup membership (Table 1).

Childhood violence exposure was related to reduced density (ie, sparsity) in the person-specific maps (β = −0.25; 95% CI, −0.41 to −0.05; P = .005; adjusted R2 = 0.059) (Figure 2). Specifically, violence exposure was associated with sparsity within the SN (β = −0.26; 95% CI, −0.43 to −0.08; P = .005) and between the SN and DMN (β = −0.20; 95% CI, −0.38 to −0.03; P = .02) (Table 2), including in sensitivity analyses (eTable 3 in the Supplement). Violence exposure was not associated with DMN density, and social deprivation was not related to network metrics (Table 2).

Childhood violence exposure was related to reduced node degree for the right insula (β = −0.29; 95% CI, −0.47 to −0.12; P = .001) and left IPL (β = −0.26; 95% CI, −0.44 to −0.09; P = .003) using a Bonferroni-corrected significance threshold (Table 2; eTable 4 in the Supplement), including in sensitivity analyses (eTable 5 in the Supplement). There were no significant associations with social deprivation.

Discussion

Results from a predominantly understudied and underserved sample with high rates of poverty suggest that childhood violence exposure, but not social deprivation, is associated with adolescent neural circuitry. Data-driven analyses identified a subset of adolescents with heterogeneous patterns of connectivity (ie, few shared and many individual connections) in 2 key neural networks associated with salience detection, attention, and social-cognitive processes (ie, the SN and DMN).7,8 This subgroup of adolescents was exposed to more violence in childhood than the other subgroup, whose patterns of neural connectivity were relatively more homogeneous (ie, had many connections in common), suggesting that violence exposure may lead to more person-specific alterations in neural circuitry. Beyond subgroups, network density within the SN and between the SN and DMN was sparse for adolescents with high violence exposure, likely due to few connections involving the right insula and the left IPL. These factors could not be accounted for by social deprivation, in-scanner motion, race, sex, pubertal development, current life stress, or maternal marital status or educational level at the time of the participant’s birth.

Findings regarding the neural network subgroups are noteworthy because the community detection algorithm within GIMME detected rsFC patterns in the brain from exposures that occurred at least 6 years earlier. Moreover, high childhood violence exposure in the subgroup characterized by neural heterogeneity likely reflects the person-specific outcomes of early adversity on the brain and suggests that research on the developmental sequelae of adverse childhood experiences should consider individual differences in neural compensatory responses to stress.17 Although it is important to replicate these findings in other samples, S-GIMME has reliably classified subgroups in empirical data,40,45 and there is evidence from simulations that modeling connections at the subgroup level, in addition to the group level, improves the validity and reliability of results.40

Considering the sample as a whole, results also suggest that violence exposure is associated with blunted connectivity within the SN and between the SN and DMN. As expected, the observed reduced SN density in adolescents with heightened childhood violence exposure differs from typical developmental patterns that show stronger rsFC within SN nodes and increased density of connections with hub regions, such as the anterior insula, as the brain matures.8,16 It is difficult, however, to align the present findings with previous work that reported increased SN rsFC in trauma-exposed youth9,10 because those samples were small, used different metrics of connectivity, and had different sample compositions. Moreover, the present sample was likely experiencing chronic adversity, and research from animal models of chronic stress proposes that, over time, the body’s stress response (eg, hypothalamic pituitary adrenal axis reactivity) becomes blunted or habituated to typical stressors.47 Previous research on hypothalamic pituitary adrenal axis reactivity in this sample revealed a blunted cortisol response in adolescents with heightened childhood violence exposure,28 and work in other high-risk samples showed blunted activation of the amygdala, an SN node, to threatening stimuli.48,49 The present study expands this notion to the function of threat detection neural circuits, and future research should examine whether this is compensatory or even adaptive.

Beyond density, childhood violence exposure was associated with reduced node degree of the right anterior insula and left IPL. These results are consistent with the extant literature because the right anterior insula in the SN facilitates shifting between the DMN and central executive network,50 which contributes to higher-level executive function.8 Moreover, early life stress has been linked to insular connectivity within the SN,9 DMN (specifically, the left IPL, which plays a role in working memory51), and other neural ROIs.52 These results also show differences in the way that the anterior insula is integrated within and between neural networks in youth exposed to violence in their homes and neighborhoods using longitudinal data from a population-based sample.

This study represents a person-specific approach to the neuroscientific investigation of the sequelae of early adversity. Past research on early adversity and rsFC assumed that the same connectivity patterns characterize all, or a majority of participants, but if this assumption is violated (as is likely the case in studies of diverse populations and biopsychosocial phenomena), then results may not accurately describe any individual.18,53 The presence of group- and subgroup-level connections in the present study suggests that there is some consistency in the connections within and between the SN and DMN, aligning with an assumption of homogeneity that is prevalent in rsFC research, but the large number of individual-level connections, especially in adolescents with high levels of early violence exposure, show that there was also notable heterogeneity that required person-specific analyses to accurately reflect rsFC, encouraging future research using person-specific modeling approaches.

All significant findings concerned violence exposure, and there were no detected associations between social deprivation and rsFC. This set of results could indicate that social deprivation has a less salient influence on patterns of spontaneous neural fluctuations. Some studies have identified links between social deprivation and functional connectivity, but they concerned extreme, nonnormative deprivation (eg, previous institutionalization).21,54 This deprivation may be qualitatively different from deprivation operationalized in the present study, and may operate through different mechanisms. In addition, because a hypothesis-driven approach to node selection was taken in this study, it is possible that deprivation is associated with rsFC of SN or DMN nodes not measured here, with other networks (eg, central executive), or in different populations (eg, with extreme or heightened variability of deprivation). It is also tenable that there are other dimensions of adversity that would have differential associations with rsFC (eg, those linked to emotionality), which future research should explore. Nonetheless, these findings present evidence for dimensional frameworks of adversity5,55 because there were distinct neural correlates for violence exposure.

Limitations

This study had limitations. Based on the demographic characteristics of the sample (eg, 73% African American, born in Midwestern cities), it is not clear whether findings will generalize beyond low-income, urban, African American youth; nonetheless, the present work is important because these populations are often underrepresented in neuroimaging research and underserved by the medical community.12 Resting-state functional MRI was collected on only a single occasion in adolescence; thus, it is unclear whether connectivity patterns reflect stable or changing neural features. In addition, it is not possible to know the direction of association (eg, whether neural differences predate exposure to adversity). Violence exposure and social deprivation composites were derived from parent reports. Exposures between the FFCWS collection waves at ages 9 and 15 years could not be accounted for in this study. Owing to changes in the FFCWS questionnaire at year 15, current adversity could not be controlled using the composite scores created for earlier ages.5 To compensate, a life stress scale was used as a covariate; however, that confounding variable did not impact associations. The ecologic pattern of poverty-related adversity is complex; thus, there are unmeasured variables that may explain these associations or contribute to cascades of risk (eg, parental psychopathologic factors).

Conclusions

In this prospective, longitudinal study, childhood violence exposure, but not social deprivation, was associated with person-specific differences in how the adolescent brain functions in regions involved in salience detection and higher-level cognitive processes. These differences were potent enough that a data-driven algorithm, blinded to child adversity, grouped youth with heightened violence exposure based on the heterogeneity of their neural networks, suggesting that the impact of violence exposure may have divergent and personalized associations with functional neural architecture. These findings have implications for understanding how dimensions of adversity affect brain development, which may inform future neuroscience-based policy interventions.

Back to top
Article Information

Accepted for Publication: July 13, 2020.

Published: September 23, 2020. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2020.17850

Correction: This article was corrected on December 7, 2020, to fix an error in the subject area, and on April 26, 2023, to add a missing symbol in eTable 2 in the Supplement. .

Open Access: This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the CC-BY License. © 2020 Goetschius LG et al. JAMA Network Open.

Corresponding Author: Christopher S. Monk, PhD, Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, 530 Church St, 2000 E Hall, Ann Arbor, MI 48109 (csmonk@umich.edu).

Author Contributions: Ms Goetschius had full access to all of the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.

Concept and design: Goetschius, Hein, Brooks-Gunn, McLoyd, Mitchell, Hyde, Monk, Beltz.

Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: Goetschius, Hein, McLanahan, Brooks-Gunn, Dotterer, Lopez-Duran, Mitchell, Hyde, Monk, Beltz.

Drafting of the manuscript: Goetschius, Dotterer, Monk, Beltz.

Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: All authors.

Statistical analysis: Goetschius, Hein, Brooks-Gunn, Dotterer, Beltz.

Obtained funding: Lopez-Duran, Mitchell, Hyde, Monk.

Administrative, technical, or material support: McLanahan, Dotterer, Lopez-Duran, Mitchell, Hyde, Monk, Beltz.

Supervision: Brooks-Gunn, Lopez-Duran, Mitchell, Hyde, Monk, Beltz.

Conflict of Interest Disclosures: Dr McLanahan reported receiving grants from the National Institutes of Health (NIH) during the conduct of the study. Dr Mitchell reported receiving grants from the NIH and the Jacobs Foundation during the conduct of the study. Dr Hyde reported receiving grants from the NIH during the conduct of the study. Dr Monk reported receiving grants from the NIH during the conduct of the study. Dr Beltz reported receiving grants from the Jacobs Foundation during the conduct of the study. No other disclosures were reported.

Funding/Support: This study was supported by NIH grants R01MH103761 (Monk), T32HD007109 (McLoyd and Monk), and S10OD012240 (Noll), as well as a Doris Duke Fellowship for the Promotion of Child Well-being (Hein). Investigators have also been funded by the Jacobs Foundation (Mitchell and Beltz). Funding for the Fragile Families and Child Wellbeing Study was provided by National Institute of Child Health and Human Development grants R01-HD36916, R01-HD39135, and R01-HD40421 and a consortium of private foundations.

Role of the Funder/Sponsor: The NIH, Doris Duke Foundation, and Jacobs Foundation had no role in design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; or decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

Additional Information: The Center for Open Science preregistration can be viewed at https://osf.io/mrwhn/, and the data will be openly available at https://nda.nih.gov/edit_collection.html?id=2106.

References
1.
Finkelhor  D, Turner  HA, Shattuck  A, Hamby  SL.  Prevalence of childhood exposure to violence, crime, and abuse: results from the National Survey of Children’s Exposure to Violence.   JAMA Pediatr. 2015;169(8):746-754. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2015.0676 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
Green  JG, McLaughlin  KA, Berglund  PA,  et al.  Childhood adversities and adult psychiatric disorders in the national comorbidity survey replication I: associations with first onset of DSM-IV disorders.   Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67(2):113-123. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.186 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
Nusslock  R, Miller  GE.  Early-life adversity and physical and emotional health across the lifespan: a neuroimmune network hypothesis.   Biol Psychiatry. 2016;80(1):23-32. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.05.017 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
McLaughlin  KA, Sheridan  MA, Lambert  HK.  Childhood adversity and neural development: deprivation and threat as distinct dimensions of early experience.   Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2014;47:578-591. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.10.012 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Hein  TC. Dimensions of early adversity as distinct predictors of adolescent brain development. Published 2019. Accessed March 19, 2019. https://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/handle/2027.42/149945/heint_1.pdf?sequence=1&isAllowed=y
6.
Goetschius  LG, Hein  TC, Mitchell  C,  et al.  Childhood violence exposure and social deprivation predict adolescent amygdala-orbitofrontal cortex white matter connectivity.   Dev Cogn Neurosci. Published online July 8, 2020. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2020.100822Google ScholarCrossref
7.
Uddin  LQ. Functions of the salience network. In:  Salience Network of the Human Brain. Academic Press; 2017:11-16. doi:10.1016/B978-0-12-804593-0.00003-5
8.
Menon  V.  Large-scale brain networks and psychopathology: a unifying triple network model.   Trends Cogn Sci. 2011;15(10):483-506. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2011.08.003 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Marusak  HA, Etkin  A, Thomason  ME.  Disrupted insula-based neural circuit organization and conflict interference in trauma-exposed youth.   Neuroimage Clin. 2015;8:516-525. doi:10.1016/j.nicl.2015.04.007 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Marusak  HA, Martin  KR, Etkin  A, Thomason  ME.  Childhood trauma exposure disrupts the automatic regulation of emotional processing.   Neuropsychopharmacology. 2015;40(5):1250-1258. doi:10.1038/npp.2014.311 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
van der Werff  SJA, Pannekoek  JN, Veer  IM,  et al.  Resting-state functional connectivity in adults with childhood emotional maltreatment.   Psychol Med. 2013;43(9):1825-1836. doi:10.1017/S0033291712002942 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Falk  EB, Hyde  LW, Mitchell  C,  et al.  What is a representative brain? neuroscience meets population science.   Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2013;110(44):17615-17622. doi:10.1073/pnas.1310134110 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Beltz  AM, Gates  KM.  Network mapping with GIMME.   Multivariate Behav Res. 2017;52(6):789-804. doi:10.1080/00273171.2017.1373014 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
Sporns  O.  Networks of the Brain. MIT Press; 2010.
15.
Power  JD, Fair  DA, Schlaggar  BL, Petersen  SE.  The development of human functional brain networks.   Neuron. 2010;67(5):735-748. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2010.08.017 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Fair  DA, Cohen  AL, Power  JD,  et al.  Functional brain networks develop from a “local to distributed” organization.   PLoS Comput Biol. 2009;5(5):e1000381. doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000381 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
Marder  E, Goaillard  J-M.  Variability, compensation and homeostasis in neuron and network function.   Nat Rev Neurosci. 2006;7(7):563-574. doi:10.1038/nrn1949 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
Molenaar  PCM.  A manifesto on psychology as idiographic science: bringing the person back into scientific psychology, this time forever.   Meas Interdiscip Res Perspect. 2004;2(4):201-218. doi:10.1207/s15366359mea0204_1 Google ScholarCrossref
19.
Gordon  EM, Laumann  TO, Gilmore  AW,  et al.  Precision functional mapping of individual human brains.   Neuron. 2017;95(4):791-807.e7. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2017.07.011 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Hughes  K, Bellis  MA, Hardcastle  KA,  et al.  The effect of multiple adverse childhood experiences on health: a systematic review and meta-analysis.   Lancet Public Health. 2017;2(8):e356-e366. doi:10.1016/S2468-2667(17)30118-4 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
Gee  DG, Gabard-Durnam  LJ, Flannery  J,  et al.  Early developmental emergence of human amygdala-prefrontal connectivity after maternal deprivation.   Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2013;110(39):15638-15643. doi:10.1073/pnas.1307893110 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
Hanson  JL, Nacewicz  BM, Sutterer  MJ,  et al.  Behavioral problems after early life stress: contributions of the hippocampus and amygdala.   Biol Psychiatry. 2015;77(4):314-323. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.04.020 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
23.
Hanson  JL, Hariri  AR, Williamson  DE.  Blunted ventral striatum development in adolescence reflects emotional neglect and predicts depressive symptoms.   Biol Psychiatry. 2015;78(9):598-605. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.05.010 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Lane  ST, Gates  KM, Molenaar  PCM. GIMME: group iterative multiple model estimation. 2019. Accessed February 28, 2019. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=gimme
25.
Gates  KM, Molenaar  PCM, Hillary  FG, Ram  N, Rovine  MJ.  Automatic search for fMRI connectivity mapping: an alternative to Granger causality testing using formal equivalences among SEM path modeling, VAR, and unified SEM.   Neuroimage. 2010;50(3):1118-1125. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.117 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
Reichman  NE, Teitler  JO, Garfinkel  I, McLanahan  SS.  Fragile families: sample and design.   Child Youth Serv Rev. 2001;23(4-5):303-326. doi:10.1016/S0190-7409(01)00141-4 Google ScholarCrossref
27.
Princeton University. Fragile families and child wellbeing study. Published 2019. Accessed October 20, 2019. https://fragilefamilies.princeton.edu/data-and-documentation/public-data-documentation
28.
Peckins  MK, Roberts  AG, Hein  TC,  et al.  Violence exposure and social deprivation is associated with cortisol reactivity in urban adolescents.   Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2020;111:104426. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2019.104426PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
29.
McLaughlin  KA, Sheridan  MA.  Beyond cumulative risk: a dimensional approach to childhood adversity.   Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2016;25(4):239-245. doi:10.1177/0963721416655883 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
30.
Carskadon  MA, Acebo  C.  A self-administered rating scale for pubertal development.   J Adolesc Health. 1993;14(3):190-195. doi:10.1016/1054-139X(93)90004-9 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
31.
Shaw  DS, Gilliom  M, Ingoldsby  EM, Nagin  DS.  Trajectories leading to school-age conduct problems.   Dev Psychol. 2003;39(2):189-200. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.39.2.189 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
32.
Masten  AS, Neemann  J, Andenas  S.  Life events and adjustment in adolescents: the significance of event independence, desirability, and chronicity.   J Res Adolesc. 1994;4(1):71-97. doi:10.1207/s15327795jra0401_5 Google ScholarCrossref
33.
Farrell  AD, Ampy  LA, Meyer  AL.  Identification and assessment of problematic interpersonal situations for urban adolescents.   J Clin Child Psychol. 1998;27(3):293-305. doi:10.1207/s15374424jccp2703_6 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
34.
Glover  GH, Law  CS.  Spiral-in/out BOLD fMRI for increased SNR and reduced susceptibility artifacts.   Magn Reson Med. 2001;46(3):515-522. doi:10.1002/mrm.1222 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
35.
Jenkinson  M, Beckmann  CF, Behrens  TEJ, Woolrich  MW, Smith  SM.  FSL.   Neuroimage. 2012;62(2):782-790. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.015 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
36.
Beltz  AM, Dotterer  HL, Goetschius  LG. GIMME preprocessing: initial release. Published May 9, 2019.
37.
Jenkinson  M, Bannister  P, Brady  M, Smith  S.  Improved optimization for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain images.   Neuroimage. 2002;17(2):825-841. doi:10.1006/nimg.2002.1132 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
38.
Pruim  RHR, Mennes  M, van Rooij  D, Llera  A, Buitelaar  JK, Beckmann  CF.  ICA-AROMA: a robust ICA-based strategy for removing motion artifacts from fMRI data.   Neuroimage. 2015;112(suppl C):267-277. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.02.064 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
39.
Yarkoni  T, Poldrack  RA, Nichols  TE, Van Essen  DC, Wager  TD.  Large-scale automated synthesis of human functional neuroimaging data.   Nat Methods. 2011;8(8):665-670. doi:10.1038/nmeth.1635 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
40.
Gates  KM, Lane  ST, Varangis  E, Giovanello  K, Guskiewicz  K.  Unsupervised classification during time-series model building.   Multivariate Behav Res. 2017;52(2):129-148. doi:10.1080/00273171.2016.1256187 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
41.
Sörbom  D.  Model modification.   Psychometrika. 1989;54(3):371-384. doi:10.1007/BF02294623 Google ScholarCrossref
42.
Gates  KM, Molenaar  PCM.  Group search algorithm recovers effective connectivity maps for individuals in homogeneous and heterogeneous samples.   Neuroimage. 2012;63(1):310-319. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.06.026 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
43.
Brown  TA.  Confirmatory Factor Analysis for Applied Research. Guilford Press; 2006.
44.
Gates  KM, Molenaar  PCM, Iyer  SP, Nigg  JT, Fair  DA.  Organizing heterogeneous samples using community detection of GIMME-derived resting state functional networks.   PLoS One. 2014;9(3):e91322. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0091322 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
45.
Price  RB, Lane  S, Gates  K,  et al.  Parsing heterogeneity in the brain connectivity of depressed and healthy adults during positive mood.   Biol Psychiatry. 2017;81(4):347-357. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2016.06.023 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
46.
Smith  SM.  The future of FMRI connectivity.   Neuroimage. 2012;62(2):1257-1266. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.022 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
47.
McCarty  R.  Learning about stress: neural, endocrine and behavioral adaptations.   Stress. 2016;19(5):449-475. doi:10.1080/10253890.2016.1192120 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
48.
Gard  AM, Waller  R, Shaw  DS, Forbes  EE, Hariri  AR, Hyde  LW.  The long reach of early adversity: parenting, stress, and neural pathways to antisocial behavior in adulthood.   Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2017;2(7):582-590. doi:10.1016/j.bpsc.2017.06.005 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
49.
Holz  NE, Boecker-Schlier  R, Buchmann  AF,  et al.  Ventral striatum and amygdala activity as convergence sites for early adversity and conduct disorder.   Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2017;12(2):261-272. doi:10.1093/scan/nsw120 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
50.
Sridharan  D, Levitin  DJ, Menon  V.  A critical role for the right fronto-insular cortex in switching between central-executive and default-mode networks.   Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2008;105(34):12569-12574. doi:10.1073/pnas.0800005105 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
51.
Philip  NS, Sweet  LH, Tyrka  AR,  et al.  Exposure to childhood trauma is associated with altered n-back activation and performance in healthy adults: implications for a commonly used working memory task.   Brain Imaging Behav. 2016;10(1):124-135. doi:10.1007/s11682-015-9373-9 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
52.
Teicher  MH, Anderson  CM, Ohashi  K, Polcari  A.  Childhood maltreatment: altered network centrality of cingulate, precuneus, temporal pole and insula.   Biol Psychiatry. 2014;76(4):297-305. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.09.016 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
53.
Eavani  H, Satterthwaite  TD, Filipovych  R, Gur  RE, Gur  RC, Davatzikos  C.  Identifying sparse connectivity patterns in the brain using resting-state fMRI.   Neuroimage. 2015;105:286-299. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.09.058 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
54.
Chugani  HT, Behen  ME, Muzik  O, Juhász  C, Nagy  F, Chugani  DC.  Local brain functional activity following early deprivation: a study of postinstitutionalized Romanian orphans.   Neuroimage. 2001;14(6):1290-1301. doi:10.1006/nimg.2001.0917 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
55.
Sheridan  MA, McLaughlin  KA.  Dimensions of early experience and neural development: deprivation and threat.   Trends Cogn Sci. 2014;18(11):580-585. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2014.09.001 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
×