[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Figure 1.  PRISMA Diagram
PRISMA Diagram

RCT indicates randomized clinical trial.

Figure 2.  Risk of Bias
Risk of Bias
Figure 3.  Pooled Effects of Abstraction and Executive Function, Attention and Working Memory, Memory, and Motor Skills
Pooled Effects of Abstraction and Executive Function, Attention and Working Memory, Memory, and Motor Skills

CCT indicates computerized cognitive training; NA, not applicable; and SMD, standardized mean difference.

Figure 4.  Pooled Effects of Sensory and Perceptual Skills, Information Processing Speed, Verbal and Language Skills, and Daily Function
Pooled Effects of Sensory and Perceptual Skills, Information Processing Speed, Verbal and Language Skills, and Daily Function

CCT indicates computerized cognitive training; NA, not applicable; and SMD, standardized mean difference.

Table.  Study and Participant Characteristics
Study and Participant Characteristics
1.
Bonnet  F, Amieva  H, Marquant  F,  et al; S CO3 Aquitaine Cohort.  Cognitive disorders in HIV-infected patients: are they HIV-related?   AIDS. 2013;27(3):391-400. doi:10.1097/QAD.0b013e32835b1019PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
Snider  WD, Simpson  DM, Nielsen  S, Gold  JW, Metroka  CE, Posner  JB.  Neurological complications of acquired immune deficiency syndrome: analysis of 50 patients.   Ann Neurol. 1983;14(4):403-418. doi:10.1002/ana.410140404PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
McArthur  JC, Hoover  DR, Bacellar  H,  et al.  Dementia in AIDS patients: incidence and risk factors. multicenter AIDS cohort study.   Neurology. 1993;43(11):2245-2252. doi:10.1212/WNL.43.11.2245PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Wei  J, Hou  J, Su  B,  et al. The prevalence of Frascati-criteria–based HIV-associated neurocognitive disorder (HAND) in HIV-infected adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Front Neurol. 2020;11:581346. doi:10.3389/fneur.2020.581346
5.
Antinori  A, Arendt  G, Becker  JT,  et al.  Updated research nosology for HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders.   Neurology. 2007;69(18):1789-1799. doi:10.1212/01.WNL.0000287431.88658.8bPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Miller  TR, Weiss  JJ, Bräu  N, Dieterich  DT, Stivala  A, Rivera-Mindt  M.  Greater decline in memory and global neurocognitive function in HIV/hepatitis C co-infected than in hepatitis C mono-infected patients treated with pegylated interferon and ribavirin.   J Neurovirol. 2017;23(2):260-272. doi:10.1007/s13365-016-0494-8PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Kanmogne  GD, Fonsah  JY, Umlauf  A,  et al.  Attention/working memory, learning and memory in adult Cameroonians: normative data, effects of HIV infection and viral genotype.   J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2020;26(6):607-623. doi:10.1017/S1355617720000120PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Marquine  MJ, Flores  I, Kamat  R,  et al.  A composite of multisystem injury and neurocognitive impairment in HIV infection: association with everyday functioning.   J Neurovirol. 2018;24(5):549-556. doi:10.1007/s13365-018-0643-3PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Laverick  R, Haddow  L, Daskalopoulou  M,  et al; Cognitive Impairment in People with HIV in the European Region (CIPHER) Study Group.  Self-reported decline in everyday function, cognitive symptoms, and cognitive function in people with HIV.   J Acquir Immune Defic Syndr. 2017;76(3):e74-e83. doi:10.1097/QAI.0000000000001468PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Milanini  B, Catella  S, Perkovich  B,  et al.  Psychiatric symptom burden in older people living with HIV with and without cognitive impairment: the UCSF HIV over 60 cohort study.   AIDS Care. 2017;29(9):1178-1185. doi:10.1080/09540121.2017.1281877PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
Barclay  TR, Hinkin  CH, Castellon  SA,  et al.  Age-associated predictors of medication adherence in HIV-positive adults: health beliefs, self-efficacy, and neurocognitive status.   Health Psychol. 2007;26(1):40-49. doi:10.1037/0278-6133.26.1.40PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Kamat  R, Morgan  E, Marcotte  TD,  et al; HNRP Group.  Implications of apathy and depression for everyday functioning in HIV/AIDS in Brazil.   J Affect Disord. 2013;150(3):1069-1075. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2012.11.040PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Antoni  MH.  Stress management and psychoneuroimmunology in HIV infection.   CNS Spectr. 2003;8(1):40-51. doi:10.1017/S1092852900023440PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
Tesch-Romer  C, Wahl  HW. Toward a more comprehensive concept of successful aging: disability and care needs. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2017;72(2):310-318. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbw162
15.
Wang  Y, Liu  M, Lu  Q,  et al.  Global prevalence and burden of HIV-associated neurocognitive disorder: a meta-analysis.   Neurology. 2020;95(19):e2610-e2621. doi:10.1212/WNL.0000000000010752PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Robertson  KR, Miyahara  S, Lee  A,  et al; AIDS Clinical Trials Group (ACTG) 5303 Team.  Neurocognition with maraviroc compared with tenofovir in HIV.   AIDS. 2016;30(15):2315-2321. doi:10.1097/QAD.0000000000001189PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
Saylor  D, Dickens  AM, Sacktor  N,  et al.  HIV-associated neurocognitive disorder—pathogenesis and prospects for treatment.   Nat Rev Neurol. 2016;12(4):234-248. doi:10.1038/nrneurol.2016.27PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
Chan  T, Marta  M, Hawkins  C, Rackstraw  S.  Cognitive and neurologic rehabilitation strategies for central nervous system HIV infection.   Curr HIV/AIDS Rep. 2020;17(5):514-521. doi:10.1007/s11904-020-00515-0PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
19.
Kaur  N, Dendukuri  N, Fellows  LK, Brouillette  MJ, Mayo  N.  Association between cognitive reserve and cognitive performance in people with HIV: a systematic review and meta-analysis.   AIDS Care. 2020;32(1):1-11. doi:10.1080/09540121.2019.1612017PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Gates  NJ, Rutjes  AW, Di Nisio  M,  et al.  Computerised cognitive training for maintaining cognitive function in cognitively healthy people in midlife.   Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2019;3(3):CD012278. doi:10.1002/14651858.CD012278.pub2PubMedGoogle Scholar
21.
Martin  DM, Kumar  D, Wong  A, Loo  CK.  A comparison of computerized versus pen-and-paper cognitive tests for monitoring electroconvulsive therapy–related cognitive side effects.   J ECT. 2020;36(4):260-264. doi:10.1097/YCT.0000000000000687PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
Dardiotis  E, Nousia  A, Siokas  V,  et al.  Efficacy of computer-based cognitive training in neuropsychological performance of patients with multiple sclerosis: a systematic review and meta-analysis.   Mult Scler Relat Disord. 2018;20:58-66. doi:10.1016/j.msard.2017.12.017PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
23.
Hill  NTM, Mowszowski  L, Naismith  SL, Chadwick  VL, Valenzuela  M, Lampit  A.  Computerized cognitive training in older adults with mild cognitive impairment or dementia: a systematic review and meta-analysis.   Am J Psychiatry. 2017;174(4):329-340. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2016.16030360PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Clare  L, Woods  RT, Moniz Cook  ED, Orrell  M, Spector  A.  Cognitive rehabilitation and cognitive training for early-stage Alzheimer’s disease and vascular dementia.   Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2003;(4):CD003260. doi:10.1002/14651858.CD003260PubMedGoogle Scholar
25.
Bredesen  DE.  Reversal of cognitive decline: a novel therapeutic program.   Aging (Albany NY). 2014;6(9):707-717. doi:10.18632/aging.100690PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
Bahar-Fuchs  A, Barendse  MEA, Bloom  R,  et al. Computerized cognitive training for older adults at higher dementia risk due to diabetes: findings from a randomized controlled trial. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2020;75(4):747-754. doi:10.1093/gerona/glz073
27.
Gates  NJ, Rutjes  AW, Di Nisio  M,  et al.  Computerised cognitive training for 12 or more weeks for maintaining cognitive function in cognitively healthy people in late life.   Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2020;2(2):CD012277. doi:10.1002/14651858.CD012277.pub3PubMedGoogle Scholar
28.
Lenze  EJ, Stevens  A, Waring  JD,  et al.  Augmenting computerized cognitive training with vortioxetine for age-related cognitive decline: a randomized controlled trial.   Am J Psychiatry. 2020;177(6):548-555. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2019.19050561PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
29.
Cavallo  M, Hunter  EM, van der Hiele  K, Angilletta  C.  Computerized structured cognitive training in patients affected by early-stage Alzheimer’s disease is feasible and effective: a randomized controlled study.   Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2016;31(8):868-876. doi:10.1093/arclin/acw072PubMedGoogle Scholar
30.
D’Antonio J, Simon-Pearson L, Goldberg T, et al. Cognitive training and neuroplasticity in mild cognitive impairment (COG-IT): protocol for a two-site, blinded, randomised, controlled treatment trial. BMJ Open. 2019;9(8):e028536. doi:10.1136/bmjopen-2018-028536
31.
Smith  SJ, McMillan  I, Leroi  I,  et al.  Feasibility and acceptability of computerised cognitive training of everyday cognition in Parkinson’s disease.   Parkinsons Dis. 2019;2019:5258493. doi:10.1155/2019/5258493PubMedGoogle Scholar
32.
Zimmermann  R, Gschwandtner  U, Benz  N,  et al.  Cognitive training in Parkinson disease: cognition-specific vs nonspecific computer training.   Neurology. 2014;82(14):1219-1226. doi:10.1212/WNL.0000000000000287PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
33.
Simone  M, Viterbo  RG, Margari  L, Iaffaldano  P.  Computer-assisted rehabilitation of attention in pediatric multiple sclerosis and ADHD patients: a pilot trial.   BMC Neurol. 2018;18(1):82. doi:10.1186/s12883-018-1087-3PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
34.
Bogdanova  Y, Yee  MK, Ho  VT, Cicerone  KD.  Computerized cognitive rehabilitation of attention and executive function in acquired brain injury: a systematic review.   J Head Trauma Rehabil. 2016;31(6):419-433. doi:10.1097/HTR.0000000000000203PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
35.
Cherif  A, Roelands  B, Meeusen  R, Chamari  K.  Effects of intermittent fasting, caloric restriction, and Ramadan intermittent fasting on cognitive performance at rest and during exercise in adults.   Sports Med. 2016;46(1):35-47. doi:10.1007/s40279-015-0408-6PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
36.
Darweesh  SKL, Wolters  FJ, Ikram  MA, de Wolf  F, Bos  D, Hofman  A.  Inflammatory markers and the risk of dementia and Alzheimer’s disease: a meta-analysis.   Alzheimers Dement. 2018;14(11):1450-1459. doi:10.1016/j.jalz.2018.02.014PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
37.
Zhang  H, Huntley  J, Bhome  R,  et al.  Effect of computerised cognitive training on cognitive outcomes in mild cognitive impairment: a systematic review and meta-analysis.   BMJ Open. 2019;9(8):e027062. doi:10.1136/bmjopen-2018-027062PubMedGoogle Scholar
38.
Lutsey  PL, Misialek  JR, Mosley  TH,  et al.  Sleep characteristics and risk of dementia and Alzheimer’s disease: the Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities study.   Alzheimers Dement. 2018;14(2):157-166. doi:10.1016/j.jalz.2017.06.2269PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
39.
Vance  DE, Fazeli  PL, Cheatwood  J, Nicholson  WC, Morrison  SA, Moneyham  LD.  Computerized cognitive training for the neurocognitive complications of HIV infection: a systematic review.   J Assoc Nurses AIDS Care. 2019;30(1):51-72. doi:10.1097/JNC.0000000000000030PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
40.
Chang  L, Lohaugen  GC, Andres  T,  et al.  Adaptive working memory training improved brain function in human immunodeficiency virus–seropositive patients.   Ann Neurol. 2017;81(1):17-34. doi:10.1002/ana.24805PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
41.
Cody  SL, Fazeli  PL, Crowe  M,  et al.  Effects of speed of processing training and transcranial direct current stimulation on global sleep quality and speed of processing in older adults with and without HIV: a pilot study.   Appl Neuropsychol Adult. 2020;27(3):267-278. doi:10.1080/23279095.2018.1534736PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
42.
Ezeamama  AE, Sikorskii  A, Sankar  PR,  et al.  Computerized cognitive rehabilitation training for Ugandan seniors living with HIV: a validation study.   J Clin Med. 2020;9(7):2137. doi:10.3390/jcm9072137PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
43.
Cody  SL, Fazeli  PL, Vance  DE.  Feasibility of a home-based speed of processing training program in middle-aged and older adults with HIV.   J Neurosci Nurs. 2015;47(4):247-254. doi:10.1097/JNN.0000000000000147PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
44.
Vance  DE, Fazeli  PL, Ross  LA, Wadley  VG, Ball  KK.  Speed of processing training with middle-age and older adults with HIV: a pilot study.   J Assoc Nurses AIDS Care. 2012;23(6):500-510. doi:10.1016/j.jana.2012.01.005PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
45.
Livelli  A, Orofino  GC, Calcagno  A,  et al.  Evaluation of a cognitive rehabilitation protocol in HIV patients with associated neurocognitive disorders: efficacy and stability over time.   Front Behav Neurosci. 2015;9:306. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00306PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
46.
Towe  SL, Patel  P, Meade  CS.  The acceptability and potential utility of cognitive training to improve working memory in persons living with HIV: a preliminary randomized trial.   J Assoc Nurses AIDS Care. 2017;28(4):633-643. doi:10.1016/j.jana.2017.03.007PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
47.
Towe  SL, Hartsock  JT, Xu  Y, Meade  CS. Web-based cognitive training to improve working memory in persons with co-occurring HIV infection and cocaine use disorder: outcomes from a randomized controlled trial. AIDS Behav. 2021;25(5):1542-1551. doi:10.1007/s10461-020-02993-0
48.
Pope  CN, Stavrinos  D, Vance  DE,  et al. A pilot investigation on the effects of combination transcranial direct current stimulation and speed of processing cognitive remediation therapy on simulated driving behavior in older adults with HIV. Transport Res Part F Traffic Psychol Behav. 2018;58:1061-1073. doi:10.1016/j.trf.2018.08.002
49.
Fazeli  PL, Woods  AJ, Pope  CN, Vance  DE, Ball  KK.  Effect of transcranial direct current stimulation combined with cognitive training on cognitive functioning in older adults with HIV: a pilot study.   Appl Neuropsychol Adult. 2019;26(1):36-47. doi:10.1080/23279095.2017.1357037PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
50.
Moher  D, Liberati  A, Tetzlaff  J, Altman  DG; PRISMA Group.  Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement.   PLoS Med. 2009;6(7):e1000097. doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097PubMedGoogle Scholar
51.
Bonnechere  B, Langley  C, Sahakian  BJ.  The use of commercial computerised cognitive games in older adults: a meta-analysis.   Sci Rep. 2020;10(1):15276. doi:10.1038/s41598-020-72281-3PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
52.
Deng  L, Zhang  X, Gao  Y,  et al.  Association of HIV infection and cognitive impairment in older adults: a meta-analysis.   Ageing Res Rev. 2021;68:101310. doi:10.1016/j.arr.2021.101310PubMedGoogle Scholar
53.
Lezak  MD, Howieson  DB, Loring  DW. Neuropsychological Assessment. 4th ed. Oxford University Press; 2004.
54.
Ownby  RL, Rivera  J, Mejia  E, Acevedo  A.  Proceedings #5. a pilot study of cognitive training with or without tDCS for cognitive deficits in older persons with HIV.   Brain Stimul. 2017;10(4):e50-e52. doi:10.1016/j.brs.2017.04.098Google ScholarCrossref
55.
Vance  D, Fazeli  P, Azuero  A,  et al.  Can individualized-targeted computerized cognitive training improve everyday functioning in adults with HIV-associated neurocognitive disorder?   Appl Neuropsychol Adult. 2021:1-12. doi:10.1080/23279095.2021.1906678PubMedGoogle Scholar
56.
Vance  DE, Fazeli  PL, Azuero  A, Wadley  VG, Raper  JL, Ball  KK.  Can individualized-targeted computerized cognitive training benefit adults with HIV-associated neurocognitive disorder? the Training on Purpose Study (TOPS).   AIDS Behav. 2021;25(12):3898-3908. doi:10.1007/s10461-021-03230-yPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
57.
Lampit  A, Hallock  H, Valenzuela  M.  Computerized cognitive training in cognitively healthy older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis of effect modifiers.   PLoS Med. 2014;11(11):e1001756. doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.1001756PubMedGoogle Scholar
58.
Ball  K, Berch  DB, Helmers  KF,  et al; Advanced Cognitive Training for Independent and Vital Elderly Study Group.  Effects of cognitive training interventions with older adults: a randomized controlled trial.   JAMA. 2002;288(18):2271-2281. doi:10.1001/jama.288.18.2271PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
59.
van Balkom  TD, van den Heuvel  OA, Berendse  HW, van der Werf  YD, Vriend  C.  The effects of cognitive training on brain network activity and connectivity in aging and neurodegenerative diseases: a systematic review.   Neuropsychol Rev. 2020;30(2):267-286. doi:10.1007/s11065-020-09440-wPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
60.
Marek  S, Dosenbach  NUF.  The frontoparietal network: function, electrophysiology, and importance of individual precision mapping.   Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2018;20(2):133-140. doi:10.31887/DCNS.2018.20.2/smarekPubMedGoogle Scholar
61.
Spreng  RN, Stevens  WD, Chamberlain  JP, Gilmore  AW, Schacter  DL.  Default network activity, coupled with the frontoparietal control network, supports goal-directed cognition.   Neuroimage. 2010;53(1):303-317. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.016PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
62.
Gates  NJ, Sachdev  P.  Is cognitive training an effective treatment for preclinical and early Alzheimer’s disease?   J Alzheimers Dis. 2014;42(suppl 4):S551-S559. doi:10.3233/JAD-141302PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
63.
Zelinski  EM.  Far transfer in cognitive training of older adults.   Restor Neurol Neurosci. 2009;27(5):455-471. doi:10.3233/RNN-2009-0495PubMedGoogle Scholar
64.
Wolinsky  FD, Vander Weg  MW, Martin  R,  et al.  The effect of speed-of-processing training on depressive symptoms in ACTIVE.   J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2009;64(4):468-472. doi:10.1093/gerona/gln044PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
65.
Karssemeijer  EGA, Aaronson  JA, Bossers  WJ, Smits  T, Olde Rikkert  MGM, Kessels  RPC.  Positive effects of combined cognitive and physical exercise training on cognitive function in older adults with mild cognitive impairment or dementia: a meta-analysis.   Ageing Res Rev. 2017;40:75-83. doi:10.1016/j.arr.2017.09.003PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
66.
Holtzer  R, Shuman  M, Mahoney  JR, Lipton  R, Verghese  J.  Cognitive fatigue defined in the context of attention networks.   Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. 2011;18(1):108-128. doi:10.1080/13825585.2010.517826PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
67.
Farhadian  S, Patel  P, Spudich  S.  Neurological complications of HIV infection.   Curr Infect Dis Rep. 2017;19(12):50. doi:10.1007/s11908-017-0606-5PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
68.
Schulz  KF, Chalmers  I, Hayes  RJ, Altman  DG.  Empirical evidence of bias. dimensions of methodological quality associated with estimates of treatment effects in controlled trials.   JAMA. 1995;273(5):408-412. doi:10.1001/jama.1995.03520290060030PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
Original Investigation
Infectious Diseases
March 3, 2022

Evaluation of Computerized Cognitive Training and Cognitive and Daily Function in Patients Living With HIV: A Meta-analysis

Author Affiliations
  • 1Beijing Key Laboratory for HIV/AIDS Research, Clinical and Research Center for Infectious Diseases, Beijing Youan Hospital, Capital Medical University, Beijing, China
  • 2Department of Social and Behavioural Sciences, City University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong Special Administrative Region, China
  • 3Department of Radiology, Beijing Youan Hospital, Capital Medical University, Beijing, China
JAMA Netw Open. 2022;5(3):e220970. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2022.0970
Key Points

Question  Are computerized cognitive training (CCT) programs associated with improvements in cognitive and daily function among people living with HIV?

Findings  In this meta-analysis of 12 randomized clinical trials involving 596 participants living with HIV, CCT was associated with improved function in 6 of the 8 domains assessed, including abstraction and executive function, attention and working memory, memory, speed of information processing, motor skills, and daily function.

Meaning  This meta-analysis found that CCT programs were associated with improvements in cognitive and daily function among people living with HIV.

Abstract

Importance  In the era of antiretroviral therapy (ART), the incidence of HIV-associated neurocognitive disorder (HAND) has not yet been controlled. With the exception of ART, there is no beneficial pharmacologic treatment. However, some studies have reported that computerized cognitive training (CCT) programs may improve cognitive function among people living with HIV.

Objective  To examine the association between CCT programs and 8 domains measuring cognitive function (7 domains) and daily function (1 domain) among people living with HIV.

Data Sources  Records from the Cochrane Library, PsycINFO, PubMed, and Web of Science were searched from database inception to December 15, 2020. Supplementary searches to identify missing studies were conducted in Google Scholar using updated search terms from database inception to November 18, 2021.

Study Selection  Studies that compared changes before and after a CCT intervention among people living with HIV were included. Search terms were a combination of words associated with HIV (eg, people living with HIV, HIV, and/or AIDS) and cognitive training (eg, cognitive intervention, nonpharmacology intervention, computer game, video game, computerized training, cognitive exercise, cognitive stimulation, and/or cognitive enhancement). Studies were included if they (1) used CCT as the primary intervention or combined CCT with other types of interventions; (2) used placebo, passive control conditions, traditional cognitive training, or single training tasks as control conditions; (3) reported changes between baseline and posttraining; (4) included participants 18 years or older; and (5) were randomized clinical trials (RCTs). Studies were excluded if they (1) were not associated with HIV, (2) were research protocols or feedback reports, (3) were case reports, or (4) did not report findings for domains of interest.

Data Extraction and Synthesis  Two reviewers independently extracted data. This study followed the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analyses (PRISMA) reporting guideline. Random-effects models were used to quantitatively synthesize the existing data.

Main Outcomes and Measures  Primary outcomes were the meta-synthesized changes in each domain after CCT.

Results  Among 1245 records identified, 1043 were screened after removal of duplicates. Of those, 1019 records were excluded based on titles and abstracts, and 24 full-text articles were assessed for eligibility. After exclusions, 12 eligible RCTs were selected for inclusion in the meta-analysis. These RCTs involved 596 total participants, with 320 individuals in the CCT group (mean age, 47.5-59.7 years; 0%-94% female; 8.3-14.2 years of education) and 276 individuals in the control group (mean age, 44.2-60.0 years; 19%-90% female; 9.0-14.9 years of education). The average HIV inhibition ratio (the proportion of participants who achieved virological suppression) ranged from 30% to 100%, and the CD4+ T-cell count ranged from 471 to 833 cells/μL. The time since training ranged from 3 to 24 weeks. After receipt of CCT, function significantly improved in 6 of the 8 domains: abstraction and executive function (standardized mean difference [SMD], 0.58; 95% CI, 0.26-0.91; P < .001), attention and working memory (SMD, 0.62; 95% CI, 0.33-0.91; P < .001), memory (SMD, 0.59; 95% CI, 0.20-0.97; P = .003), motor skills (SMD, 0.50; 95% CI, 0.24-0.77; P < .001), speed of information processing (SMD, 0.65; 95% CI, 0.37-0.94; P < .001), and daily function (SMD, 0.44; 95% CI, 0.02-0.86; P = .04). Sensory and perceptual skills (SMD, 0.06; 95% CI, −0.36 to 0.48; P = .78) and verbal and language skills (SMD, 0.46; 95% CI, −0.07 to 0.99; P = .09) did not significantly improve after CCT.

Conclusions and Relevance  This meta-analysis of RCTs found that CCT programs were associated with improvements in cognitive and daily function among people living with HIV. Future studies are needed to design optimal specific training programs and use implementation science to enable the transformation of CCT from a scientific research tool to a real-world clinical intervention.

Introduction

People living with HIV experience cognitive deterioration, also known as HIV-associated neurocognitive disorder (HAND), despite the widespread use of antiretroviral therapy (ART).1-4 Based on commonly used criteria,5 HAND is diagnosed by assessing 7 cognitive domains, including speed of information processing, sensory and perceptual (sensory/perceptual) skills, memory (learning and recall), attention and working memory (attention/working memory), motor skills, verbal and language (verbal/language) skills, and abstraction and executive function (abstraction/executive function).6,7 A diagnosis of HAND can have substantial consequences for an individual’s daily function.8,9 In addition, HAND has been associated with reduced life satisfaction10-12 and social isolation,13 producing public health and economic burdens worldwide.14,15 With the exception of ART, there is no specific pharmacologic treatment for HAND.16,17 However, studies have reported potential benefits from nonpharmacologic interventions that may ameliorate cognitive decline and reduce the odds of developing HAND among people living with HIV.18,19 These interventions have attracted the attention of many researchers. Chan et al18 focused on different types of cognitive and neurologic rehabilitation strategies among people living with HIV. Their results highlighted the importance of ART and suggested that nonpharmacologic strategies might improve cognitive function, either as stand-alone interventions or as part of a multidisciplinary approach.

Because of its safety, relevance, low cost, scalability, and convenience, computerized cognitive training (CCT) has been one of the most commonly used nonpharmacologic interventions.20-22 Computerized cognitive training programs aim to incorporate guided drills and practice for single or multiple cognitive domains through specific standardized procedures, differentiating CCT from other cognitive remediation approaches.23 The theoretical premise behind CCT is that it can stimulate neuroplasticity.24 From a neuropsychological perspective,25 CCT has the potential to shape brain structure and reorganize function among cognitively healthy older adults26-28 and individuals with Alzheimer disease,29,30 Parkinson disease,31,32 attention deficits,33 and acquired brain injury.34 Furthermore, physiological parameters, including brain metabolism35 and inflammatory,36 hormonal,37 and sleep-related factors,38 may also be transformed through brain plasticity.25 In addition, many studies involving people living with HIV have reported that CCT may reduce the risk of cognitive deterioration in several domains,39 such as working memory,40-43 speed of information processing,40,44 executive function and attention,45-47 and memory (learning and recall).42 However, the benefits reported in original articles using the same CCT program (Posit Science software) have been inconsistent. Pope et al48 found that this software could improve abstraction/executive function, whereas Fazeli et al49 reported that the software could not only enhance abstraction/executive function but also improve attention, working memory, and speed of information processing among people living with HIV.

Because of the mixed results reported in original studies, Vance et al39 investigated the findings in a systematic review. After identifying 13 items that fit their selection criteria, they found that most of the CCT programs were associated with improvements in cognitive function that translated into better daily function, improved mood, more substantial locus of control, and enhanced quality of life. Nevertheless, Vance et al39 did not calculate the effect size for each subdomain or perform sensitivity or moderator analyses. In addition, to our knowledge, no previous meta-analysis has confirmed the association of CCT with daily function and with each cognitive domain categorized by the Frascati criteria.5 The potential factors associated with CCT outcomes for each cognitive domain among people living with HIV have also not been confirmed.

Given these knowledge gaps, we conducted a meta-analysis to assess the associations of CCT programs with cognitive and daily function among people living with HIV. The present study aimed to (1) assess the extent of improvement in each domain after CCT among people living with HIV and (2) explore the consistency of the domain results among all of the potential factors. Furthermore, the study aimed to provide suggestions for future implementation of CCT interventions among people living with HIV.

Methods

This study was registered in the International Prospective Register of Systematic Reviews (PROSPERO; registration No.: CRD42020210805). The study followed the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analyses (PRISMA) reporting guideline for meta-analyses.50

Search Strategy

We performed a record search of electronic databases, including the Cochrane Library, PsycINFO, PubMed, and Web of Science, with no limitations on publication type, from database inception to December 15, 2020. We also conducted a supplementary search using additional search terms from a previous meta-analysis.51 The search terms were a combination of words associated with HIV (eg, people living with HIV, HIV, and/or AIDS) and cognitive training (eg, cognitive training, cognitive intervention, cognitive rehabilitation, nonpharmacology intervention, mnemonic training, processing speed training, working memory training, N-back training, attention training, reasoning training, computer game, video game, computerized training, computerized intervention, cognitive exercise, brain exercise, cognitive stimulation, and/or cognitive enhancement). Additional searches to identify missing studies were also conducted from database inception to November 18, 2021, using Google Scholar and the reference lists of reviews and included studies. These supplementary searches did not identify any missing studies or studies published after the conclusion of the primary search (December 15, 2020). A full description of the initial and supplementary search strategies is available in eTable 1 and eTable 2 in the Supplement.

Selection Criteria

Studies were eligible for inclusion if they (1) used CCT as the primary intervention or combined CCT with other types of interventions; (2) used a placebo, passive control conditions, traditional cognitive training, or single training tasks as control conditions; (3) reported changes between baseline and posttraining; (4) included participants 18 years or older and (5) were randomized clinical trials (RCTs). Studies were excluded if they (1) were not associated with HIV, (2) were research protocols or feedback reports, (3) were case reports, or (4) did not report findings for domains of interest. Duplicate studies were removed using EndNote X9 software (Clarivate), and 2 reviewers (J.W. and J.H.) separately selected search results based on titles and abstracts. Assessment of full-text articles to determine eligibility of the remaining studies was conducted by the same 2 reviewers. Disagreements about eligibility between reviewers were resolved through discussion with 2 investigators (B.S. and T.Z.). A detailed description of reasons for exclusion is available in eTable 3 in the Supplement, and the study selection process is shown in Figure 1.

Data Extraction

Relevant data were independently extracted and cross-checked by 2 researchers (J.W. and J.H.) using an Excel spreadsheet (Microsoft Corporation). The summary statistics collected for each outcome included number of participants, means, and SDs. Training outcomes included daily function (1 domain) and cognitive tasks (7 domains, which included speed of information processing, sensory/perceptual skills, memory [learning and recall], attention/working memory, motor skills, verbal/language skills, and abstraction/executive function). The 7 cognitive domains were categorized using Frascati criteria5 and selected based on criteria from a previous study52 and work by Lezak et al.53 Details about the categorization process are shown in eTable 4 in the Supplement. Training dose was defined by the total number of training sessions, duration of each session, total training hours, training frequency, and time since training. Other information extracted from each RCT included the name of the first author, study location, year of publication, sample size, sex distribution, mean age and educational level of participants, current CD4+ T-cell counts, and current HIV inhibition ratio (the proportion of participants who achieved virological suppression).

Statistical Analysis

Data analysis was performed using Comprehensive Meta-Analysis software, version 3 (Biostat, Inc), and the funnel plot was constructed using Review Manager software, version 5.4 (Cochrane Training). Because of the inherent heterogeneity across studies, we used random-effects models to estimate pooled effect sizes. Standardized mean differences (SMDs) were calculated as the mean change from pretraining to posttraining in the intervention group minus the mean change from pretraining to posttraining in the control group divided by the combined pretest SD (adjusted for bias). The inverse variance method was used to connect the SMDs of each study. We used Q tests (ie, χ2 tests) and the I2 statistic to assess the statistical and proportional significance of heterogeneity. We also used the Egger regression intercept test to estimate publication bias. The threshold for statistical significance was 2-tailed P < .05.

Study quality was evaluated using the Cochrane risk of bias tool, which divided risk of various biases into 3 grades: low, high, and unclear (Figure 2; eFigure in the Supplement). We used GRADEpro software, version 3.6 (McMaster University and Evidence Prime, Inc), to assess the methodological quality of included evidence (eTable 5 in the Supplement).

Sensitivity analyses were performed for significant results. The moderators included participant age (<18 years vs ≥18 years), proportion of women (<50% vs ≥50%), years of education (≤12 years vs >12 years), current CD4+ T-cell counts (<500 cells/μL vs ≥500 cells/μL), current HIV inhibition ratio (<100% vs 100%), total sessions (<22 vs ≥22), session duration (<60 minutes vs ≥60 minutes), session frequency (<3 sessions per week vs ≥3 sessions per week), total training time (<10 hours vs ≥10 hours), and time since training (<10 weeks vs ≥10 weeks). Study participants were also divided into those with normal vs impaired cognitive status.

Results
Search Results

Because the methods and results of supplementary research covered the initial research results, we used the flowchart from our supplementary research to describe study selection in this article (Figure 1). We included a total of 1245 records. After removing all duplicates (n = 202), 1043 records were screened. Of those, 1019 records were excluded based on titles and abstracts, and 24 full-text articles were assessed for eligibility. After exclusions, 12 eligible RCTs40-42,44-49,54-56 were selected for inclusion in the meta-analysis.

Study Characteristics

Studies included data from the US,40,41,44,46-49,54-56 Uganda,42 and Italy,45 comprising 596 total participants (320 participants in the CCT group and 276 participants in the control group). Participant ages ranged from a mean of 47.5 years45 to 59.7 years42 in the CCT group and 44.2 years46 to 60.0 years42 in the control group. The proportion of women ranged from 0%54 to 94%40 in the CCT group and 19%45 to 90%40 in the control group. Years of education ranged from 8.3 years42 to 14.2 years40 in the CCT group and 9.0 years45 to 14.9 years40 in the control group. Only 9 studies40,42,44-49,54 reported etiologic data from CCT groups; in those studies, CD4+ T-cell counts ranged from 471 cells/μL44 to 833 cells/μL,49 and the HIV inhibition ratio ranged from 30%44 to 100%.45 Additional characteristics of the included RCTs and participants are shown in the Table.

The total number of training sessions ranged from 654 to 48,47 and session duration ranged from 20 minutes54 to 90 minutes.56 Training frequency ranged from 2 sessions per week41,42,46 to 4 sessions per week,48 total number of training hours ranged from 1.754 to 20.0,55,56 and time since training ranged from 3 weeks54 to 24 weeks.45

Although all 12 studies40-42,44-49,54-56 used CCT programs, the intervention tools were different. Cody et al,41 Fazeli et al,49 and Pope et al48 used the same cognitive training program (BrainHQ.com; Posit Science) along with transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). Ownby et al54 also used tDCS combined with a video game (GT Racing 2; Gameloft). Towe et al46,47 used an active cognitive training tool (Lumosity web-based cognitive games; Lumos Labs, Inc). Chang et al40 used an adaptive working memory training platform (Cogmed; Neural Assembly), Ezeamama et al42 used computerized cognitive rehabilitation therapy software (Captain’s Log MindPower Builder; Brain Train, Inc), and Livelli et al45 combined paper and pencil and computer-based exercises. Vance et al44,55,56 used a computer program (InSight; Posit Science) in their 2012 study44 and an individualized targeted cognitive training framework in their 2021 studies.55,56

The control conditions were divided into 3 types: placebo (6 studies41,42,45,48,49,54 used sham tDCS or standard of care), no contact (3 studies44,55,56), and other (2 studies46,47 used nonactive cognitive training and 1 study40 used nonadaptive working memory training [Cogmed; Neural Assembly]). Six studies44,45,48,49,54,56 reported outcomes for the abstraction/executive function domain, 9 studies40,41,44-47,49,54,56 for the attention/working memory domain, 5 studies41,42,49,54,56 for the memory domain, 5 studies42,45,49,54,56 for the motor skills domain, 5 studies41,42,44,47,54 for the speed of information processing domain, 3 studies44,45,55 for the daily function domain, 1 study56 for the sensory/perceptual skills domain, and 4 studies42,45,47,49 for the verbal/language skills domain.

Meta-analysis of Cognitive and Daily Function Domains

Computerized cognitive training was significantly associated with improvements in 6 of the 8 domains: abstraction/executive function, attention/working memory, memory, motor skills, speed of information processing, and daily function. The detailed results of the meta-analysis for each domain are shown in Figure 3 and Figure 4.

Abstraction/Executive Function

Among 6 studies44,45,48,49,54,56 including 240 participants, the SMD for abstraction/executive function was 0.58 (95% CI, 0.26-0.91; P < .001). No significant heterogeneity (Q = 7.11; I2 = 30%; P = .21) or publication bias (intercept, −0.70; 95% CI, −5.92 to 4.52; P = .73) was found.

Attention/Working Memory

Among 9 studies40,41,44-47,49,54,56 involving 376 participants, the SMD for attention/working memory was 0.62 (95% CI, 0.33-0.91; P < .001). Moderate heterogeneity was detected (Q = 14.04; I2 = 43%; P = .08). No significant publication bias was found (intercept, 1.98; 95% CI, −1.69 to 5.65; P = .24).

Memory

Among 5 studies41,42,49,54,56 comprising 245 participants, the SMD for memory was 0.59 (95% CI, 0.20-0.97; P < .001). Moderate heterogeneity was detected (Q = 7.64; I2 = 48%; P = .11). No significant publication bias was found (intercept, 1.13; 95% CI, −5.29 to 7.55; P = .61).

Motor Skills

Among 5 studies42,45,49,54,56 involving 229 participants, the SMD for motor skills was 0.50 (95% CI, 0.24-0.77; P < .001). No significant heterogeneity (Q = 3.82; I2 = 0%; P = .43) or publication bias (intercept, 1.29; 95% CI, −3.71 to 6.30; P = .47) was found.

Speed of Information Processing

Among studies41,42,44,47,54 including 246 participants, the SMD for speed of information processing was 0.65 (95% CI, 0.37-0.94; P < .001). No significant heterogeneity (Q = 4.60; I2 = 13%; P = .33) or publication bias (intercept, −0.22; 95% CI, −5.72 to 5.29; P = .91) was detected.

Daily Function

Among 3 studies44,45,55 comprising 187 participants, the SMD for daily function was 0.44 (95% CI, 0.02-0.86; P = .04). No significant heterogeneity (Q = 3.60; I2 = 45%; P = .16) or publication bias (intercept, 3.87; 95% CI, −12.58 to 20.31; P = .21) was detected.

Sensory/Perceptual Skills

One study56 comprising 88 participants examined sensory/perceptual skills. The SMD was 0.06 (95% CI, −0.36 to 0.48; P = .78).

Verbal/Language Skills

Among 4 studies42,45,47,49 involving 204 participants the SMD for verbal/language skills was 0.46 (95% CI, −0.07 to 0.99; P = .09). Moderate heterogeneity was detected (Q = 9.72; I2 = 69%; P = .02). However, no significant publication bias was found (intercept, 5.04; 95% CI, −9.55 to 19.63; P = .28).

Sensitivity Analysis

We conducted sensitivity analyses among all of the factors extracted from RCTs for each domain that had statistically significant results in the meta-analysis. Only 5 factors produced substantial changes in outcomes: age (memory domain: Q = 0.75; P = .02), session hours (attention/working memory domain: Q = 9.41; P < .001), time since training (abstraction/executive function domain: Q = 5.33; P = .02), CD4+ T-cell count (attention/working memory domain: Q = 4.78; P = .03), and HIV inhibition ratio (attention/working memory domain: Q = 4.52; P = .03; memory domain: Q = 4.76; P = .03). Detailed results from sensitivity analyses are available in eTable 6 in the Supplement.

Study Quality and Risk of Bias

Eleven studies40-42,44-49,54,55 had moderate-quality evidence, and 1 study56 had very low-quality evidence (eTable 5 in the Supplement). Six domains (abstraction/executive function, attention/working memory, memory, motor skills, speed of information processing, and daily function) had moderate-quality evidence, for which the main reasons were moderate to high risk of bias, small samples, inclusion of few studies, and large 95% CIs. Two domains (sensory/perceptual and verbal/language skills) had very low-quality evidence.

Most studies had a low risk of bias for random sequence generation (11 studies40,41,44-49,54-56), selective reporting (10 studies41,42,44-46,48,49,54-56), incomplete outcome data (8 studies40,42,45-47,49,55,56), and other types of bias (10 studies40,42,44,45,47-49,54-56) (Figure 2; eFigure in the Supplement). Six studies41,47,48,54-56 (50.0%) reported allocation concealment as a risk of bias. Eight studies40,42,44-47,55,56 (66.7%) did not blind those who implemented or assessed the interventions and/or those who reported relevant information.

Discussion

To our knowledge, this meta-analysis of 12 RCTs40-42,44-49,54-56 is the first to assess the association of CCT programs with cognitive and daily function among people living with HIV. Significant improvement was found in daily function and most cognitive domains, with the exception of sensory/perceptual and verbal/language skills, after CCT. Our results revealed no publication bias. The findings of the sensitivity analyses revealed that all pooled effects, with the exception of the memory and verbal/language skills domains, were statistically significant.

Moderate effect sizes for the speed of information processing and the attention/working memory domains suggested the potential for future improvement. Consistent with findings of previous meta-analyses of CCT interventions,23,37,51,57 these 2 domains were also more likely to improve after CCT among individuals with mild cognitive impairment23,37 and older adults with healthy cognitive status.51,57 Because improvements after cognitive training typically reflect training content,57,58 this result may be a result of sufficient training on these 2 subdomains within studies. Previous studies have also found that functional connectivity in the frontal-parietal brain network, mainly involved in the speed of information processing and the attention/working memory domains, increases after training.59-61 With regard to the sensory/perceptual and verbal/language skills domains, the null effect may be a result of the small number of studies and the presence of measurement bias because associations between these 2 domains and CCT have been found in other populations.51,57 Future studies may consider dedicating more time to targeting these 2 domains. Multiple studies involving cognitively healthy adults57 and individuals with Alzheimer disease62 have reported that training benefits extended beyond the cognitive domains assessed after training to include other cognitive domains, and these benefits have transferred to daily function, psychological health, and other higher-order competencies.63,64 We also found an association between CCT and daily function among people living with HIV. Despite the limitations of these training tools,63 preliminary data suggest that CCT can, in principle, improve a broader range of essential functions, including cognitive and daily function.57

With regard to the sensitivity analysis, fewer results were statistically significant, and analysis of the same factor applied to different domains yielded substantially different results. We did not find any statistical difference in the characteristics of study participants. Our results partially replicated those reported in a meta-analysis of CCT programs among patients with Alzheimer disease.65 Karssemeijer et al65 found only a slight difference in the benefits of CCT among older adults with mild cognitive impairment vs dementia. Hill et al23 reported that CCT was beneficial for global cognition in the memory and learning domains among people with mild cognitive impairment. In comparison, the evidence for benefit among people with dementia was weak.23 Possible reasons might include (1) the cognitive enhancement mechanism after CCT may have differed between the 2 groups, and (2) differences in the training methods and measurement tools used may have produced different results.

Regarding CCT dose, our analyses suggested that better results occurred after longer CCT sessions (ie, >60 minutes), possibly because synaptic plasticity is more likely to occur after 30 to 60 minutes of stimulation.57,66 A meta-analysis involving studies of patients with Alzheimer disease reached similar conclusions.57 Therefore, longer training sessions might be the recommended approach for people living with HIV. In contrast, many commercial products designed for at-home training use protocols consisting of shorter sessions, which may not be adequate. The findings of the current meta-analysis also provided a better understanding of viral load and current CD4+ T-cell counts among people living with HIV. Those with undetectable viral loads and current CD4+ T-cell counts of more than 500 cells/μL performed better than those with detectable viral loads and lower CD4+ T-cell counts, particularly in the attention/working memory domain. A previous study also found that people living with HIV who had current CD4+ T-cell counts lower than 500 cells/μL were more likely to have HAND.4 The pathogenesis of HAND may explain this phenomenon. After entering the central nervous system, HIV can stimulate chronic neuroinflammation, which interacts with viral proteins and produces cognitive impairment.67 Therefore, people with lower viral loads and higher CD4+ T-cell counts, which indicate better immune status, may experience more benefit from CCT.

Only 50% of the included RCTs41,47,48,54-56 reported allocation concealment as a risk of bias, producing inflated effect sizes and potential selection bias.68 Eight of 12 RCTs40,42,44-47,55,56 (66.7%) did not blind those who implemented or assessed the interventions and/or those who reported relevant information. These limitations may have created implementation and assessor biases, producing increases in false-positive results. With regard to the quality of the evidence, 2 domains (sensory/perceptual and verbal/language skills) were considered to have very low-quality evidence, and the remaining 6 domains (abstraction/executive function, attention/working memory, memory, motor skills, speed of information processing, and daily function) had moderate-quality evidence, for which the main reasons were moderate to high risk of bias, small samples, inclusion of few RCTs, and wide 95% CIs. This result suggested that our results need to be further explored. We found no significant publication bias across outcomes, suggesting our outcomes were statistically significant.

Limitations

This study has limitations. First, the number of studies included in the meta-analysis was small, producing heterogeneity and low evidence quality. Therefore, the optimal intervention design for eliciting beneficial outcomes remains unclear. Second, the included RCTs also lacked measurement of blood, cerebrospinal fluid, and brain imaging biomarkers after CCT. Including such measurements could have helped to clarify the potential mechanisms of the benefits observed after CCT. Third, the RCTs did not measure concurrent treatment (eg, ART, tDCS, physical exercise, or mindfulness) during the CCT intervention. Analysis of synergistic factors and outcomes can help physicians develop more beneficial plans for patients.

Conclusions

The findings of this meta-analysis of pooled data from RCTs suggested that CCT programs were associated with significant improvements in 6 cognitive and daily function domains (including abstraction/executive function, attention/working memory, memory, motor skills, speed of information processing, and daily function) among people living with HIV. Future studies are needed to clarify whether there is a difference in training benefits between CCT programs and to examine the synergistic factors and outcomes of different auxiliary interventions (eg, tDCS or exercise). In addition, more studies are needed to confirm the impact of potential factors and to assess training protocols among a large population of individuals living with HIV who are at risk of developing HAND. Studies in the field of implementation science are especially needed to address the challenge of removing barriers and bringing CCT from scientific research into clinical practice and implementing CCT programs in the real world.

Back to top
Article Information

Accepted for Publication: January 14, 2022.

Published: March 3, 2022. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2022.0970

Open Access: This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the CC-BY License. © 2022 Wei J et al. JAMA Network Open.

Corresponding Authors: Tong Zhang, MD, PhD, Beijing Key Laboratory for HIV/AIDS Research, Clinical and Research Center for Infectious Diseases, Beijing Youan Hospital, Capital Medical University, No. 8 Xitoutiao, Youanmenwai St, Feng Tai District, Beijing, China (zt_doc@ccmu.edu.cn); Bin Su, PhD, Beijing Key Laboratory for HIV/AIDS Research, Clinical and Research Center for Infectious Diseases, Beijing Youan Hospital, Capital Medical University, No. 8 Xitoutiao, Youanmenwai St, Feng Tai District, Beijing, China (binsu@ccmu.edu.cn).

Author Contributions: Drs Zhang and Su had full access to all of the data in the study and take responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis. Ms Wei and Mr Hou contributed equally to this article as co–first authors.

Concept and design: Hou, Sun, Su, Zhang.

Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: Wei, Hou, Mu, Li, Wu, Zhang.

Drafting of the manuscript: Wei, Hou, Mu, Li, Su, Zhang.

Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Hou, Sun, Wu, Su, Zhang.

Statistical analysis: Wei, Hou, Mu, Li.

Obtained funding: Wu, Su, Zhang.

Administrative, technical, or material support: Sun, Su, Zhang.

Supervision: Su, Zhang.

Conflict of Interest Disclosures: None reported.

Funding/Support: This work was supported by grant 81761128001 from the National Natural Science Foundation of China–National Institutes of Health Biomedical Collaborative Research Program (Dr Wu); grants 81772165 (Dr Su), 81974303 (Dr Su), and 82072271 (Dr Zhang) from the National Natural Science Foundation of China; grant DFL20191701 from the Climbing the Peak (Dengfeng) Talent Training Program of the Beijing Hospitals Authority (Dr Zhang); grant BHTPP2020 from the Beijing Health Technologies Promotion Program (Dr Zhang); and grant BZ0089 from the Beijing Key Laboratory for HIV/AIDS Research.

Role of the Funder/Sponsor: The funding organizations had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

References
1.
Bonnet  F, Amieva  H, Marquant  F,  et al; S CO3 Aquitaine Cohort.  Cognitive disorders in HIV-infected patients: are they HIV-related?   AIDS. 2013;27(3):391-400. doi:10.1097/QAD.0b013e32835b1019PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
Snider  WD, Simpson  DM, Nielsen  S, Gold  JW, Metroka  CE, Posner  JB.  Neurological complications of acquired immune deficiency syndrome: analysis of 50 patients.   Ann Neurol. 1983;14(4):403-418. doi:10.1002/ana.410140404PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
McArthur  JC, Hoover  DR, Bacellar  H,  et al.  Dementia in AIDS patients: incidence and risk factors. multicenter AIDS cohort study.   Neurology. 1993;43(11):2245-2252. doi:10.1212/WNL.43.11.2245PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Wei  J, Hou  J, Su  B,  et al. The prevalence of Frascati-criteria–based HIV-associated neurocognitive disorder (HAND) in HIV-infected adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Front Neurol. 2020;11:581346. doi:10.3389/fneur.2020.581346
5.
Antinori  A, Arendt  G, Becker  JT,  et al.  Updated research nosology for HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders.   Neurology. 2007;69(18):1789-1799. doi:10.1212/01.WNL.0000287431.88658.8bPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Miller  TR, Weiss  JJ, Bräu  N, Dieterich  DT, Stivala  A, Rivera-Mindt  M.  Greater decline in memory and global neurocognitive function in HIV/hepatitis C co-infected than in hepatitis C mono-infected patients treated with pegylated interferon and ribavirin.   J Neurovirol. 2017;23(2):260-272. doi:10.1007/s13365-016-0494-8PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Kanmogne  GD, Fonsah  JY, Umlauf  A,  et al.  Attention/working memory, learning and memory in adult Cameroonians: normative data, effects of HIV infection and viral genotype.   J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2020;26(6):607-623. doi:10.1017/S1355617720000120PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Marquine  MJ, Flores  I, Kamat  R,  et al.  A composite of multisystem injury and neurocognitive impairment in HIV infection: association with everyday functioning.   J Neurovirol. 2018;24(5):549-556. doi:10.1007/s13365-018-0643-3PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Laverick  R, Haddow  L, Daskalopoulou  M,  et al; Cognitive Impairment in People with HIV in the European Region (CIPHER) Study Group.  Self-reported decline in everyday function, cognitive symptoms, and cognitive function in people with HIV.   J Acquir Immune Defic Syndr. 2017;76(3):e74-e83. doi:10.1097/QAI.0000000000001468PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Milanini  B, Catella  S, Perkovich  B,  et al.  Psychiatric symptom burden in older people living with HIV with and without cognitive impairment: the UCSF HIV over 60 cohort study.   AIDS Care. 2017;29(9):1178-1185. doi:10.1080/09540121.2017.1281877PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
Barclay  TR, Hinkin  CH, Castellon  SA,  et al.  Age-associated predictors of medication adherence in HIV-positive adults: health beliefs, self-efficacy, and neurocognitive status.   Health Psychol. 2007;26(1):40-49. doi:10.1037/0278-6133.26.1.40PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Kamat  R, Morgan  E, Marcotte  TD,  et al; HNRP Group.  Implications of apathy and depression for everyday functioning in HIV/AIDS in Brazil.   J Affect Disord. 2013;150(3):1069-1075. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2012.11.040PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Antoni  MH.  Stress management and psychoneuroimmunology in HIV infection.   CNS Spectr. 2003;8(1):40-51. doi:10.1017/S1092852900023440PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
Tesch-Romer  C, Wahl  HW. Toward a more comprehensive concept of successful aging: disability and care needs. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2017;72(2):310-318. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbw162
15.
Wang  Y, Liu  M, Lu  Q,  et al.  Global prevalence and burden of HIV-associated neurocognitive disorder: a meta-analysis.   Neurology. 2020;95(19):e2610-e2621. doi:10.1212/WNL.0000000000010752PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Robertson  KR, Miyahara  S, Lee  A,  et al; AIDS Clinical Trials Group (ACTG) 5303 Team.  Neurocognition with maraviroc compared with tenofovir in HIV.   AIDS. 2016;30(15):2315-2321. doi:10.1097/QAD.0000000000001189PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
Saylor  D, Dickens  AM, Sacktor  N,  et al.  HIV-associated neurocognitive disorder—pathogenesis and prospects for treatment.   Nat Rev Neurol. 2016;12(4):234-248. doi:10.1038/nrneurol.2016.27PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
Chan  T, Marta  M, Hawkins  C, Rackstraw  S.  Cognitive and neurologic rehabilitation strategies for central nervous system HIV infection.   Curr HIV/AIDS Rep. 2020;17(5):514-521. doi:10.1007/s11904-020-00515-0PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
19.
Kaur  N, Dendukuri  N, Fellows  LK, Brouillette  MJ, Mayo  N.  Association between cognitive reserve and cognitive performance in people with HIV: a systematic review and meta-analysis.   AIDS Care. 2020;32(1):1-11. doi:10.1080/09540121.2019.1612017PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Gates  NJ, Rutjes  AW, Di Nisio  M,  et al.  Computerised cognitive training for maintaining cognitive function in cognitively healthy people in midlife.   Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2019;3(3):CD012278. doi:10.1002/14651858.CD012278.pub2PubMedGoogle Scholar
21.
Martin  DM, Kumar  D, Wong  A, Loo  CK.  A comparison of computerized versus pen-and-paper cognitive tests for monitoring electroconvulsive therapy–related cognitive side effects.   J ECT. 2020;36(4):260-264. doi:10.1097/YCT.0000000000000687PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
Dardiotis  E, Nousia  A, Siokas  V,  et al.  Efficacy of computer-based cognitive training in neuropsychological performance of patients with multiple sclerosis: a systematic review and meta-analysis.   Mult Scler Relat Disord. 2018;20:58-66. doi:10.1016/j.msard.2017.12.017PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
23.
Hill  NTM, Mowszowski  L, Naismith  SL, Chadwick  VL, Valenzuela  M, Lampit  A.  Computerized cognitive training in older adults with mild cognitive impairment or dementia: a systematic review and meta-analysis.   Am J Psychiatry. 2017;174(4):329-340. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2016.16030360PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Clare  L, Woods  RT, Moniz Cook  ED, Orrell  M, Spector  A.  Cognitive rehabilitation and cognitive training for early-stage Alzheimer’s disease and vascular dementia.   Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2003;(4):CD003260. doi:10.1002/14651858.CD003260PubMedGoogle Scholar
25.
Bredesen  DE.  Reversal of cognitive decline: a novel therapeutic program.   Aging (Albany NY). 2014;6(9):707-717. doi:10.18632/aging.100690PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
Bahar-Fuchs  A, Barendse  MEA, Bloom  R,  et al. Computerized cognitive training for older adults at higher dementia risk due to diabetes: findings from a randomized controlled trial. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2020;75(4):747-754. doi:10.1093/gerona/glz073
27.
Gates  NJ, Rutjes  AW, Di Nisio  M,  et al.  Computerised cognitive training for 12 or more weeks for maintaining cognitive function in cognitively healthy people in late life.   Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2020;2(2):CD012277. doi:10.1002/14651858.CD012277.pub3PubMedGoogle Scholar
28.
Lenze  EJ, Stevens  A, Waring  JD,  et al.  Augmenting computerized cognitive training with vortioxetine for age-related cognitive decline: a randomized controlled trial.   Am J Psychiatry. 2020;177(6):548-555. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2019.19050561PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
29.
Cavallo  M, Hunter  EM, van der Hiele  K, Angilletta  C.  Computerized structured cognitive training in patients affected by early-stage Alzheimer’s disease is feasible and effective: a randomized controlled study.   Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2016;31(8):868-876. doi:10.1093/arclin/acw072PubMedGoogle Scholar
30.
D’Antonio J, Simon-Pearson L, Goldberg T, et al. Cognitive training and neuroplasticity in mild cognitive impairment (COG-IT): protocol for a two-site, blinded, randomised, controlled treatment trial. BMJ Open. 2019;9(8):e028536. doi:10.1136/bmjopen-2018-028536
31.
Smith  SJ, McMillan  I, Leroi  I,  et al.  Feasibility and acceptability of computerised cognitive training of everyday cognition in Parkinson’s disease.   Parkinsons Dis. 2019;2019:5258493. doi:10.1155/2019/5258493PubMedGoogle Scholar
32.
Zimmermann  R, Gschwandtner  U, Benz  N,  et al.  Cognitive training in Parkinson disease: cognition-specific vs nonspecific computer training.   Neurology. 2014;82(14):1219-1226. doi:10.1212/WNL.0000000000000287PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
33.
Simone  M, Viterbo  RG, Margari  L, Iaffaldano  P.  Computer-assisted rehabilitation of attention in pediatric multiple sclerosis and ADHD patients: a pilot trial.   BMC Neurol. 2018;18(1):82. doi:10.1186/s12883-018-1087-3PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
34.
Bogdanova  Y, Yee  MK, Ho  VT, Cicerone  KD.  Computerized cognitive rehabilitation of attention and executive function in acquired brain injury: a systematic review.   J Head Trauma Rehabil. 2016;31(6):419-433. doi:10.1097/HTR.0000000000000203PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
35.
Cherif  A, Roelands  B, Meeusen  R, Chamari  K.  Effects of intermittent fasting, caloric restriction, and Ramadan intermittent fasting on cognitive performance at rest and during exercise in adults.   Sports Med. 2016;46(1):35-47. doi:10.1007/s40279-015-0408-6PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
36.
Darweesh  SKL, Wolters  FJ, Ikram  MA, de Wolf  F, Bos  D, Hofman  A.  Inflammatory markers and the risk of dementia and Alzheimer’s disease: a meta-analysis.   Alzheimers Dement. 2018;14(11):1450-1459. doi:10.1016/j.jalz.2018.02.014PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
37.
Zhang  H, Huntley  J, Bhome  R,  et al.  Effect of computerised cognitive training on cognitive outcomes in mild cognitive impairment: a systematic review and meta-analysis.   BMJ Open. 2019;9(8):e027062. doi:10.1136/bmjopen-2018-027062PubMedGoogle Scholar
38.
Lutsey  PL, Misialek  JR, Mosley  TH,  et al.  Sleep characteristics and risk of dementia and Alzheimer’s disease: the Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities study.   Alzheimers Dement. 2018;14(2):157-166. doi:10.1016/j.jalz.2017.06.2269PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
39.
Vance  DE, Fazeli  PL, Cheatwood  J, Nicholson  WC, Morrison  SA, Moneyham  LD.  Computerized cognitive training for the neurocognitive complications of HIV infection: a systematic review.   J Assoc Nurses AIDS Care. 2019;30(1):51-72. doi:10.1097/JNC.0000000000000030PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
40.
Chang  L, Lohaugen  GC, Andres  T,  et al.  Adaptive working memory training improved brain function in human immunodeficiency virus–seropositive patients.   Ann Neurol. 2017;81(1):17-34. doi:10.1002/ana.24805PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
41.
Cody  SL, Fazeli  PL, Crowe  M,  et al.  Effects of speed of processing training and transcranial direct current stimulation on global sleep quality and speed of processing in older adults with and without HIV: a pilot study.   Appl Neuropsychol Adult. 2020;27(3):267-278. doi:10.1080/23279095.2018.1534736PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
42.
Ezeamama  AE, Sikorskii  A, Sankar  PR,  et al.  Computerized cognitive rehabilitation training for Ugandan seniors living with HIV: a validation study.   J Clin Med. 2020;9(7):2137. doi:10.3390/jcm9072137PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
43.
Cody  SL, Fazeli  PL, Vance  DE.  Feasibility of a home-based speed of processing training program in middle-aged and older adults with HIV.   J Neurosci Nurs. 2015;47(4):247-254. doi:10.1097/JNN.0000000000000147PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
44.
Vance  DE, Fazeli  PL, Ross  LA, Wadley  VG, Ball  KK.  Speed of processing training with middle-age and older adults with HIV: a pilot study.   J Assoc Nurses AIDS Care. 2012;23(6):500-510. doi:10.1016/j.jana.2012.01.005PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
45.
Livelli  A, Orofino  GC, Calcagno  A,  et al.  Evaluation of a cognitive rehabilitation protocol in HIV patients with associated neurocognitive disorders: efficacy and stability over time.   Front Behav Neurosci. 2015;9:306. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00306PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
46.
Towe  SL, Patel  P, Meade  CS.  The acceptability and potential utility of cognitive training to improve working memory in persons living with HIV: a preliminary randomized trial.   J Assoc Nurses AIDS Care. 2017;28(4):633-643. doi:10.1016/j.jana.2017.03.007PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
47.
Towe  SL, Hartsock  JT, Xu  Y, Meade  CS. Web-based cognitive training to improve working memory in persons with co-occurring HIV infection and cocaine use disorder: outcomes from a randomized controlled trial. AIDS Behav. 2021;25(5):1542-1551. doi:10.1007/s10461-020-02993-0
48.
Pope  CN, Stavrinos  D, Vance  DE,  et al. A pilot investigation on the effects of combination transcranial direct current stimulation and speed of processing cognitive remediation therapy on simulated driving behavior in older adults with HIV. Transport Res Part F Traffic Psychol Behav. 2018;58:1061-1073. doi:10.1016/j.trf.2018.08.002
49.
Fazeli  PL, Woods  AJ, Pope  CN, Vance  DE, Ball  KK.  Effect of transcranial direct current stimulation combined with cognitive training on cognitive functioning in older adults with HIV: a pilot study.   Appl Neuropsychol Adult. 2019;26(1):36-47. doi:10.1080/23279095.2017.1357037PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
50.
Moher  D, Liberati  A, Tetzlaff  J, Altman  DG; PRISMA Group.  Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement.   PLoS Med. 2009;6(7):e1000097. doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097PubMedGoogle Scholar
51.
Bonnechere  B, Langley  C, Sahakian  BJ.  The use of commercial computerised cognitive games in older adults: a meta-analysis.   Sci Rep. 2020;10(1):15276. doi:10.1038/s41598-020-72281-3PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
52.
Deng  L, Zhang  X, Gao  Y,  et al.  Association of HIV infection and cognitive impairment in older adults: a meta-analysis.   Ageing Res Rev. 2021;68:101310. doi:10.1016/j.arr.2021.101310PubMedGoogle Scholar
53.
Lezak  MD, Howieson  DB, Loring  DW. Neuropsychological Assessment. 4th ed. Oxford University Press; 2004.
54.
Ownby  RL, Rivera  J, Mejia  E, Acevedo  A.  Proceedings #5. a pilot study of cognitive training with or without tDCS for cognitive deficits in older persons with HIV.   Brain Stimul. 2017;10(4):e50-e52. doi:10.1016/j.brs.2017.04.098Google ScholarCrossref
55.
Vance  D, Fazeli  P, Azuero  A,  et al.  Can individualized-targeted computerized cognitive training improve everyday functioning in adults with HIV-associated neurocognitive disorder?   Appl Neuropsychol Adult. 2021:1-12. doi:10.1080/23279095.2021.1906678PubMedGoogle Scholar
56.
Vance  DE, Fazeli  PL, Azuero  A, Wadley  VG, Raper  JL, Ball  KK.  Can individualized-targeted computerized cognitive training benefit adults with HIV-associated neurocognitive disorder? the Training on Purpose Study (TOPS).   AIDS Behav. 2021;25(12):3898-3908. doi:10.1007/s10461-021-03230-yPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
57.
Lampit  A, Hallock  H, Valenzuela  M.  Computerized cognitive training in cognitively healthy older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis of effect modifiers.   PLoS Med. 2014;11(11):e1001756. doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.1001756PubMedGoogle Scholar
58.
Ball  K, Berch  DB, Helmers  KF,  et al; Advanced Cognitive Training for Independent and Vital Elderly Study Group.  Effects of cognitive training interventions with older adults: a randomized controlled trial.   JAMA. 2002;288(18):2271-2281. doi:10.1001/jama.288.18.2271PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
59.
van Balkom  TD, van den Heuvel  OA, Berendse  HW, van der Werf  YD, Vriend  C.  The effects of cognitive training on brain network activity and connectivity in aging and neurodegenerative diseases: a systematic review.   Neuropsychol Rev. 2020;30(2):267-286. doi:10.1007/s11065-020-09440-wPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
60.
Marek  S, Dosenbach  NUF.  The frontoparietal network: function, electrophysiology, and importance of individual precision mapping.   Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2018;20(2):133-140. doi:10.31887/DCNS.2018.20.2/smarekPubMedGoogle Scholar
61.
Spreng  RN, Stevens  WD, Chamberlain  JP, Gilmore  AW, Schacter  DL.  Default network activity, coupled with the frontoparietal control network, supports goal-directed cognition.   Neuroimage. 2010;53(1):303-317. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.016PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
62.
Gates  NJ, Sachdev  P.  Is cognitive training an effective treatment for preclinical and early Alzheimer’s disease?   J Alzheimers Dis. 2014;42(suppl 4):S551-S559. doi:10.3233/JAD-141302PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
63.
Zelinski  EM.  Far transfer in cognitive training of older adults.   Restor Neurol Neurosci. 2009;27(5):455-471. doi:10.3233/RNN-2009-0495PubMedGoogle Scholar
64.
Wolinsky  FD, Vander Weg  MW, Martin  R,  et al.  The effect of speed-of-processing training on depressive symptoms in ACTIVE.   J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2009;64(4):468-472. doi:10.1093/gerona/gln044PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
65.
Karssemeijer  EGA, Aaronson  JA, Bossers  WJ, Smits  T, Olde Rikkert  MGM, Kessels  RPC.  Positive effects of combined cognitive and physical exercise training on cognitive function in older adults with mild cognitive impairment or dementia: a meta-analysis.   Ageing Res Rev. 2017;40:75-83. doi:10.1016/j.arr.2017.09.003PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
66.
Holtzer  R, Shuman  M, Mahoney  JR, Lipton  R, Verghese  J.  Cognitive fatigue defined in the context of attention networks.   Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. 2011;18(1):108-128. doi:10.1080/13825585.2010.517826PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
67.
Farhadian  S, Patel  P, Spudich  S.  Neurological complications of HIV infection.   Curr Infect Dis Rep. 2017;19(12):50. doi:10.1007/s11908-017-0606-5PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
68.
Schulz  KF, Chalmers  I, Hayes  RJ, Altman  DG.  Empirical evidence of bias. dimensions of methodological quality associated with estimates of treatment effects in controlled trials.   JAMA. 1995;273(5):408-412. doi:10.1001/jama.1995.03520290060030PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
×