Key PointsQuestion
Is there an association between exposure to green space and cognitive function throughout middle age?
Findings
In this cohort study that included 13 594 women, increasing green space was associated with higher scores of overall cognition and psychomotor speed/attention. In contrast, there was no association between green space and learning/working memory.
Meaning
These findings suggest that green space exposure should be investigated as a potential population-level approach to improve cognitive function.
Importance
Green space can decelerate cognitive decline by supporting physical activity, psychological restoration, or reducing exposure to air pollution. However, existing studies on the association of green space with cognitive decline are limited.
Objective
To examine whether residential green space was associated with cognitive function in middle-aged women.
Design, Setting, and Participants
Starting in 1989, the Nurses’ Health Study II enrolled 116 429 female nurses aged 25 to 42 years residing in the US. In 2014 to 2016, 40 082 women were invited to complete an online cognitive battery. This cohort study analyzed women who had data on both green space exposure and cognitive measures. Data analysis was conducted from June to October 2021.
Exposures
Residential exposure to green space was assessed using the Normalized Difference Vegetation Index, a satellite-derived indicator of the quantity of ground vegetation. Landsat satellite data at 270-m and 1230-m buffers around each participants’ residential addresses in 2013 were used.
Main Outcomes and Measures
In 2014 to 2016, cognitive function was measured using a self-administered online battery, the Cogstate Brief Battery, consisting of 4 tasks measuring psychomotor speed, attention, learning, and working memory; 3 composite scores, averaging together all tasks, psychomotor speed/attention, and learning/working memory, were created. In addition, the study evaluated potential mediators, including air pollution, depression, and physical activity.
Results
The analytical sample included 13 594 women, of whom 13 293 (98%) were White. Mean (SD) age was 61.2 (4.6) years. In models adjusted for age at assessment, race, childhood, adulthood, and neighborhood socioeconomic status, green space was associated with higher scores on the global Cogstate composite (mean difference per IQR in green space, 0.05; 95% CI, 0.02 to 0.07), and psychomotor speed/attention (mean difference in score, 0.05 standard units; 95% CI, 0.02 to 0.08). This difference in scores is similar to the difference observed in women 1 year apart in age in the data. By contrast, there was no association between green space and learning/working memory (mean difference, 0.0300; 95% CI, −0.0003 to 0.0500).
Conclusions and Relevance
These findings suggest that increasing residential green space may be associated with modest benefits in cognition in middle-aged women.
The rising prevalence of Alzheimer Disease and related dementias calls for novel prevention strategies. Cognitive function at middle age is associated with subsequent dementia1-3 and is considered a risk factor for decline in physical functioning4 and mortality.5 Recent studies6,7 show that higher residential surrounding green space is associated with improved cognitive function in older adults. However, existing studies on the association of green space and cognitive function are still scarce.
Exposure to green space may be associated with cognitive function through increasing opportunities for physical activity, social engagement, psychological restoration, improving cognitive capacity, and mitigating the negative consequences of noise and air pollution.8-10 Although physical activity has been shown to decrease dementia risk,11 poor social engagement has been associated with increased dementia risk.12 The association between psychological restoration and dementia has been documented across population-based studies.13,14 In this research, the attention restoration theory states that people can recover from stress and mental fatigue and can concentrate better after spending time in or even looking at green space.15 Finally, there is increasing evidence that higher exposure to air pollution and noise are associated with faster cognitive decline16-20 and higher dementia prevalence.21
We used data from a nationwide prospective cohort study of women to investigate the associations between residential green space and cognitive function. Our green space exposure temporally preceded the cognitive function measures, and we evaluated potential mediators that also temporally preceded cognitive function measurement, which provide a step forward to advancing this area of research.22 We assessed effect modification by measures of neighborhood socioeconomic status (SES) and urbanicity.23 We hypothesized that green space exposure was positively associated with cognitive function in a nationwide prospective cohort study of women. Additionally, we hypothesized that this association would be most visible among women with lower neighborhood SES, and that the association would be mediated by depression, air pollution, and physical activity.
Starting in 1989, the Nurses’ Health Study II enrolled 116 429 female nurses aged 25 to 42 years residing in the US.24 Women complete mailed or online questionnaires biennially and follow-up is ongoing.25 Participants’ residential addresses throughout follow-up have been geocoded to obtain latitude and longitude. Between 2014 to 2016, 40 082 women who had completed a supplemental violence questionnaire, and with known email addresses, were invited to complete an online cognitive battery.26 Of those, 14 151 women completed the Cogstate Brief Battery,27 a self-administered computerized cognitive assessment, and 13 994 had complete data for all Cogstate scores. Responders to the invitation to complete the Cognitive Study were comparable to the nonresponders in terms of sociodemographics, depression, health behaviors, and medical conditions potentially relevant to cognitive function.28 The study protocol was approved by the institutional review board of the Brigham and Women’s Hospital. Return of the questionnaires and Cogstate completion constituted consent. We included data from women who had data on both green space and cognitive measures. This study follows the Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) reporting guideline.29
Exposure to green space around participants’ home addresses in 2013 was estimated using the Normalized Difference Vegetation Index (NDVI), a satellite image–based metric (further details in the eAppendix in the Supplement). Briefly, NDVI ranges between −1 and 1, where negative values correspond to water, values around 0 correspond to barren areas of rock or sand, and positive values represent grassland. We examined the average NDVI in 270-m and 1230-m buffers around each geocoded address to evaluate both the immediate area around residences and the walkable area as potential relevant geographic contexts.30 NDVI was assessed as a continuous variable defined by the IQR for analysis. Negative and missing NDVI values (400 missing [2.9%]) were set to 0 using the rationale that green space is only captured on the positive scale.
The Cogstate Battery has 4 tasks: Detection, Identification, One Card Learning, and One Back.27,31 A detailed description of each task can be found in the eAppendix in the Supplement. The validity of Cogstate has been established in previous studies27,32,33 and has been shown to be a sensitive indicator of early cognitive deficits32 and Alzheimer dementia.34,35 On the basis of previous research,28,36 we created 3 composite scores: (1) a psychomotor speed/attention composite (created by averaging z scores of Detection and Identification, with a possible range of −6.9 to 3.4), (2) a learning/working memory composite (averaging z scores from One Card Learning and One Back, with a possible range of −5.9 to 3.8), and (3) an overall cognition composite (averaging z scores from all 4 tasks, with a possible range of −6.4 to 4.0).31 The 3 composite scores were standardized to have a mean of 0 and an SD of 1; all composite scores have had high test-retest reliability and clinical utility in identifying cognitive impairment.34 Higher scores indicate better cognitive performance.
We selected covariates that are associated with green space and have been associated with cognitive function, but that were unlikely to result from either,37 including age at cognitive assessment and self-identified race (White, Black, or Asian). We adjusted for race to capture the outcomes of perceived race, along with various other factors such as quality of schools that are associated with skin color, genetic background, and cultural context.38 Additional covariates assessed in 2005 measuring childhood SES included maternal and paternal education level (percentage with less than a high school education), parents’ occupation (percentage who were unskilled laborers), and parents’ home ownership (yes/no). Covariates measured during participant’s adulthood, closest to the cognitive assessment, included individual and neighborhood SES. Individual SES was measured using 2 variables: husband’s education assessed in 1999 (less than high school, high school, 2 years of college, 4 years of college, or graduate school), and marital status (yes/no) assessed in 2013. Neighborhood SES was assessed by 2 variables: 2010 US Census tract median income and median home value based on the US Census tract of residence in 2013.
On the basis of previous research, US Census tract median income and urbanicity were used as stratifiying variables.23 Urbanicity was measured as the US Census tract population density (number per square kilometers).
Green space has been associated with decreased levels of air pollution,39,40 lower risk of depression, and increased physical activity,40 which, in turn, could improve cognitive function.16,41-43 Thus, we used mediation analysis44-46 to evaluate whether the association between green space and cognition is mediated through these variables of interest (eFigure 1 in the Supplement). We used spatiotemporal smoothing models to estimate exposures to particulate matter with a diameter less than 2.5 μm (eAppendix in the Supplement).47 Depressive symptoms were assessed in 2013 with the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale and modeled as a continuous variable. Finally, physical activity was calculated from self-reported time spent per week (<3, 3 to <9, 9 to <18, 18 to <27, or ≥27 metabolic equivalent hours per week) in 2013.
We used multivariable linear regression to estimate differences in cognitive function across levels of green space exposure. Separate models were conducted for each cognitive composite score. We estimated mean differences (and 95% CIs) in cognitive composites corresponding to a 1-IQR increment in average NDVI in the 270-m buffer for our main results, and in the 1230-m buffer for sensitivity analyses. For each cognitive composite, we initially adjusted for age and race (model 1). Model 2 further adjusted for maternal and paternal education level, parents’ occupation, and parents’ home ownership. Model 3 further adjusted for husband’s education and marital status. Model 4 further adjusted for US Census tract median income and median home value. Natural splines were used to examine nonlinear associations of green space with cognitive function. To assess deviations from linearity for associations with all cognition measurements, we fit generalized additive models for continuous exposures and natural splines with 3 to 4 knots based on Akaike Information Criterion.
We assessed effect modification of associations of green space with cognitive function by Census tract median household income, and urbanicity using stratified analysis, multiplicative interaction terms and likelihood ratio tests to assess significance. We used mediation analysis44-46 to evaluate whether the outcome associated with green space is mediated through air pollution, depression, and physical activity, on each of the cognitive composites (eAppendix in the Supplement). Finally, as a sensitivity analysis, to address the fact that the mediators of interest could also be confounders, we further adjusted model 4 for air pollution, depression, and physical activity. Data analysis was conducted in SAS statistical software version 9.4 (SAS Institute) from June to October 2021. A 2-tailed t-test was used to calculate P values, and < .05 was considered significant.
Of 13 594 total participants (mean [SD] age of 61.2 [4.6] years), virtually all were White (13 293 participants [98%]). Women who lived in areas with higher levels of green space were less likely to be African American, more likely to have parents who had owned their home, and more likely to be married (Table 1). Areas with higher levels of green space were less likely to be densely populated (Table 1).
In models adjusted for age and race, 1 IQR higher green space exposure in a 270-m buffer was associated with higher scores on the psychomotor speed/attention composite (β, 0.04; 95% CI, 0.02 to 0.07) (Table 2) and on overall cognition (β, 0.04; 95% CI, 0.01 to 0.06) (Table 2). Further adjustment for individual childhood and adulthood SES (β, 0.05; 95% CI, 0.02 to 0.08) and neighborhood SES (β, 0.05; 95% CI, 0.02 to 0.07) did not attenuate these associations, and the results remained virtually unchanged. Similarly, in sensitivity analyses using the 1230-m buffer, in fully adjusted models, 1 IQR higher of green space exposure was associated with higher scores on the psychomotor speed/attention composite (β, 0.04; 95% CI, 0.02 to 0.07) (eTable 1 in the Supplement) and on overall cognition (β, 0.04; 95% CI, 0.02 to 0.06) (eTable 1 in the Supplement). In the main analyses within a 270-m buffer and in sensistivity analyses within a 1230-m buffer, green space exposure was not associated with higher scores on the learning/working memory composite (fully adjusted within 270-m buffer: β, 0.03; 95% CI, −0.000 to 0.05; 1230-m buffer: β, 0.02; 95% CI, −0.01 to 0.04). The estimate of the association between green space and processing speed and attention was equivalent to the estimate of being 1.2 years younger in our data.
We observed positive nonlinear associations between green space and the psychomotor speed/attention composite as well as the global score, whereas a U-shaped association was observed for the learning/working memory composite, after accounting for further confounders (eFigure 2 in the Supplement). Increasing green space was associated with higher scores on the psychomotor speed/attention composite, although the evidence of consistent improvement at levels of green space above 0.8 NDVI was not clear (eFigure 2 in the Supplement). Although the test for deviations from linearity for green space was significant for both psychomotor speed/attention composite and the global Cogstate score, the associations were visually monotonic and, thus, we report the IQR results as the main findings. Moreover, the distribution of the residuals of the main models did not suggest deviation from normality.
We did not observe an association between green space and the learning/working memory composite among the different quartiles of Census tract median income and there was no statistical evidence of a difference in associations (P for interaction = .65). In contrast, we observed associations between green space and the psychomotor speed/attention composite (β, 0.08; 95% CI, 0.03-0.13) and the global score (β, 0.06; 95% CI, 0.01-0.11) among participants with higher Census tract median income (Table 3). We also observed variability in the psychomotor speed/attention composite across levels of population density, although there was no evidence of a significant difference (P for interaction = .51).
Estimates of the proportion of the association between green space and cognitive function that might be mediated by other factors (assuming that underlying assumptions of the mediation analyses hold) were significant for depression (Table 4). Depression was estimated to explain 3.95% (95% CI, 0.35%-7.55%) of the association between green space and psychomotor speed/attention and 6.30% (95% CI, 0.77%-11.81%) of the association between green space and overall cognition. The results did not suggest mediation of the association between green space and cognition by any other of the investigated mediators.
In sensitivity analyses, where we address the fact that the mediators of interest could also be confounders, we observed that adjustment for depression, air pollution, and physical activity in the main models did not change the association between green space and cognition (eTable 2 in the Supplement).
This nationwide cohort study of adult women investigated the association of residential surrounding green space in 270-m and 1230-m buffers with cognitive function. Our study found that higher levels of residential green space were associated with higher scores on processing speed and attention and on overall cognition, after adjusting for age, race, individual childhood and adulthood SES, and neighborhood SES. The estimate of the association between green space and processing speed and attention was equivalent to the estimate of being 1.2 years younger, in our data. We also found that higher levels of residential green space were not associated with learning/working memory battery scores. Our estimates were robust to a wide range of sensitivity analyses.
Our results were similar to those of a 10-year longitudinal study6 of British civil servants from the Whitehall cohort that found that an IQR increase in NDVI was associated with a difference in the global cognition z score (0.02; 95% CI, 0.003-0.037) in the 500-m buffer. Our results were also consistent with a recent analysis48 of US Medicare beneficiaries aged less than 65 years that suggested that higher green space was associated with reduced risk of Alzheimer Disease, after adjustment for individual and neighborhood sociodemographics.
The associations with psychomotor speed/attention were greater among participants living in neighborhoods with higher SES. This contrasted with the Whitehall study,6 which found no clear trends in the associations with cognitive function across strata of neighborhood SES. This difference might be due to different distributions and variation of neighborhood SES among the US and the English populations. A recent article49 reported that the economic inequalities in the UK are substantially lower compared with inequality in the USA. Our results also contrast with those by Cherrie et al,7 who found an association between green space and cognition among lower SES groups. However, the latter study used parental occupational social class as a measure of SES from childhood, whereas we used neighborhood SES. All our participants were nurses at baseline so there was likely less variation in SES, which might explain the difference in our results.
The null asociations between green space and learning and working memory are in accordance with a recent study50 that examined the association between green space and brain-based magnetic resonance imaging measures in older adults. The authors50 found that living in neighborhoods with more green space was not associated with left or right hippocampal volume, which is responsible for memory and learning. In line with previous research, this study suggests differing associations between green space and cognition based on the cognitive domain examined.51,52
Previous research on green space and cognition has reported greater associations for women compared with men,6,7 which may explain the robust associations found in our study, despite the fact that the women in our study were younger than those in previous studies, which might be expected to reduce our ability to detect associations. However, this may also signal the public health importance of green space, if associations were detected in these young-older women. Furthermore, subtle alterations in continuous measures of cognition have important clinical implications at the population level.53
We evaluated several potential mediatiors on the association between green space and cognitive function. Higher green space exposure has been associated with lower exposure to air pollution, lower risk of depression, and increased opportunities for physical activity,40,54 which, in turn, could improve cognition. However, in our study we did not find evidence that the association between green space and cognitive function could be explained by air pollution or physical activity. Our results on physical activity and air pollution are in agreement with a previous study6 that evaluated these as potential mediators between green space and cognitive decline. Our mediation analysis did suggest that green space may be associated with cognitive function through depression. There is a foundation for this mechanism in the literature. Higher exposure to green space has been consistently linked to lower levels of depression.55,56 In addition, depression has been documented to be an important factor associated with risk for dementia.41,57 These findings highlight the relevance of green space as a potential factor to reduce depressive episodes and thus subsequent dementia.
Limitations and Strengths
Potential limitations of this study include the outcome measurement at a single time point, which did not allow us to examine causal associations or temporality of associations. However, the Cogstate battery was administered at least 1 year after the green space exposure measure. Further longitudinal studies that examine associations between exposure to green space and cognitive decline would be informative. Second, our study population was predominantly White women with high education levels. This limits examination of racial disparities, effect modification by SES, or generalizability to populations with substantially different distributions of green space. In addition, the most appropriate buffer within which to measure green space is not clear.58 We examined buffers around the immediate area of residences (270 m) and walkable area (1230 m) as potential relevant geographic contexts. In both analyses, we observed similar results, but it is unclear whether these associations would remain if other buffer sizes were examined or if we were able to incorporate information about where participants spent their time when not at home.59 Third, NDVI does not measure the quality of green space or green space features as individuals experience them. Future studies should look into green space quality and ground-level metrics that can capture how individuals experience their environment, including street-view imagery or smartphone Global Positioning System data. Fourth, the assumption of temporal ordering of exposure, mediators, and outcome may not be satisfied, nor would the no unmeasured confounding assumption, particularly for the exposure-mediator as they were both measured at the same time.
Our study also has important strengths. We accounted for multiple individual childhood and adulthood socioeconomic factors and neighborhood SES as potential confounders. In addition, we evaluated several domains of cognition through the Cogstate battery, which is an objective measure of cognitive function and has been shown to be a sensitive indicator of early cognitive deficits.32 This is among the first examinations to study the association between green space exposure and cognitive function across the entire US. Finally, measurements of important environmental exposures, mental health, and behaviors enabled us to examine potential mechanisms through which green space is associated with cognitive function.
We observed that higher residential surrounding green space was associated with better processing speed and attention, as well as overall cognition among participants in the Nurses’ Health Study II. The worldwide aging population and the rapid increase of dementia calls for novel prevention strategies. Our results suggest that green space exposure should be investigated as a potential population-level approach to improve cognitive function.
Accepted for Publication: March 10, 2022.
Published: April 27, 2022. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2022.9306
Open Access: This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the CC-BY License. © 2022 Jimenez MP et al. JAMA Network Open.
Corresponding Author: Marcia P. Jimenez, PhD, MSc, MA, Department of Epidemiology, Boston University School of Public Health, 715 Albany St, Ste 420 E, Boston, MA 02118 (jimenezm@bu.edu).
Author Contributions: Drs Jimenez and James had full access to all of the data in the study and take responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.
Concept and design: Jimenez, Elliott, DeVille, Laden, Grodstein, James.
Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: Jimenez, Elliott, Laden, Hart, Weuve, Grodstein, James.
Drafting of the manuscript: Jimenez, James.
Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: All authors.
Statistical analysis: Jimenez, Elliott, Weuve.
Obtained funding: Grodstein.
Administrative, technical, or material support: Elliott, Laden, Hart, Grodstein, James.
Supervision: Laden, Grodstein, James.
Conflict of Interest Disclosures: Dr Weuve reported receiving personal fees from Alzheimer’s Association and personal fees from Health Effects Institute outside the submitted work. No other disclosures were reported.
Funding/Support: Research reported in this publication was supported by National Institutes of Health grants 1K99AG066949-02 (to Dr Jimenez), R01AG067497 and R01AG065359 (both to Dr Weuve) and R00CA201542 (to Dr James).
Role of the Funder/Sponsor: The funder had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.
Disclaimer: The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the sponsoring agency.
6.de Keijzer
C, Tonne
C, Basagaña
X,
et al. Residential surrounding greenness and cognitive decline: a 10-year follow-up of the Whitehall II cohort.
Environ Health Perspect. 2018;126(7):077003. doi:
10.1289/EHP2875PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 12.Penninkilampi
R, Casey
AN, Singh
MF, Brodaty
H. The association between social engagement, loneliness, and risk of dementia: a systematic review and meta-analysis.
J Alzheimers Dis. 2018;66(4):1619-1633. doi:
10.3233/JAD-180439PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 18.Weuve
J, Bennett
EE, Ranker
L,
et al. Exposure to air pollution in relation to risk of dementia and related outcomes: an updated systematic review of the epidemiological literature.
Environ Health Perspect. 2021;129(9):96001. doi:
10.1289/EHP8716PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 21.Weuve
J, D’Souza
J, Beck
T,
et al. Long-term community noise exposure in relation to dementia, cognition, and cognitive decline in older adults.
Alzheimers Dement. 2021;17(3):525-533. doi:
10.1002/alz.12191PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 27.Fredrickson
J, Maruff
P, Woodward
M,
et al. Evaluation of the usability of a brief computerized cognitive screening test in older people for epidemiological studies.
Neuroepidemiology. 2010;34(2):65-75. doi:
10.1159/000264823PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 28.Sumner
JA, Hagan
K, Grodstein
F, Roberts
AL, Harel
B, Koenen
KC. Posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms and cognitive function in a large cohort of middle-aged women.
Depress Anxiety. 2017;34(4):356-366. doi:
10.1002/da.22600PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 29.von Elm
E, Altman
DG, Egger
M, Pocock
SJ, Gøtzsche
PC, Vandenbroucke
JP; STROBE Initiative. The Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) statement: guidelines for reporting observational studies.
Bull World Health Organ. 2007;85(11):867-872. doi:
10.2471/BLT.07.045120PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 31.Koyama
AK, Hagan
KA, Okereke
OI, Weisskopf
MG, Rosner
B, Grodstein
F. Evaluation of a self-administered computerized cognitive battery in an older population.
Neuroepidemiology. 2015;45(4):264-272. doi:
10.1159/000439592PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 32.Maruff
P, Thomas
E, Cysique
L,
et al. Validity of the CogState brief battery: relationship to standardized tests and sensitivity to cognitive impairment in mild traumatic brain injury, schizophrenia, and AIDS dementia complex.
Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2009;24(2):165-178. doi:
10.1093/arclin/acp010PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 33.Maruff
P, Collie
A, Darby
D, Weaver-Cargin
J, Masters
C, Currie
J. Subtle memory decline over 12 months in mild cognitive impairment.
Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2004;18(3-4):342-348. doi:
10.1159/000080229PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 34.Maruff
P, Lim
YY, Darby
D,
et al; AIBL Research Group. Clinical utility of the cogstate brief battery in identifying cognitive impairment in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease.
BMC Psychol. 2013;1(1):30. doi:
10.1186/2050-7283-1-30PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 35.Lim
YY, Ellis
KA, Harrington
K,
et al; The AIBL Research Group. Use of the CogState Brief Battery in the assessment of Alzheimer’s disease related cognitive impairment in the Australian Imaging, Biomarkers and Lifestyle (AIBL) study.
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2012;34(4):345-358. doi:
10.1080/13803395.2011.643227PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 37.Hernán
MA, Hernández-Díaz
S, Werler
MM, Mitchell
AA. Causal knowledge as a prerequisite for confounding evaluation: an application to birth defects epidemiology.
Am J Epidemiol. 2002;155(2):176-184. doi:
10.1093/aje/155.2.176PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 43.Brasure
M, Desai
P, Davila
H,
et al. Physical activity interventions in preventing cognitive decline and Alzheimer-type dementia: a systematic review.
Ann Intern Med. 2018;168(1):30-38. doi:
10.7326/M17-1528PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 47.Yanosky
JD, Paciorek
CJ, Laden
F,
et al. Spatio-temporal modeling of particulate air pollution in the conterminous United States using geographic and meteorological predictors.
Environ Health. 2014;13(1):63. doi:
10.1186/1476-069X-13-63PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 48.Aitken
WW, Lombard
J, Wang
K,
et al. Relationship of neighborhood greenness to Alzheimer’s disease and non-Alzheimer’s dementia among 249 405 U.S. Medicare beneficiaries.
J Alzheimers Dis. 2021;81(2):597-606. doi:
10.3233/JAD-201179PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 50.Besser
LM, Lovasi
GS, Michael
YL,
et al. Associations between neighborhood greenspace and brain imaging measures in non-demented older adults: the Cardiovascular Health Study.
Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2021;56(9):1575-1585. doi:
10.1007/s00127-020-02000-wPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 55.Beyer
KM, Kaltenbach
A, Szabo
A, Bogar
S, Nieto
FJ, Malecki
KM. Exposure to neighborhood green space and mental health: evidence from the survey of the health of Wisconsin.
Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2014;11(3):3453-3472. doi:
10.3390/ijerph110303453PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref