[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Figure 1.  The Cross-sectional Percentile Scores on the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire Subscales in Each Profile at 3 Time Points of the Child and Adolescent Cohorts
The Cross-sectional Percentile Scores on the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire Subscales in Each Profile at 3 Time Points of the Child and Adolescent Cohorts

The beige band in each graph denotes the classic abnormal threshold (ie, >90th percentile); off-white band, an elevated but subclinical level (80th-90th percentile).

Figure 2.  Sankey Diagram Displaying the Transition Between the Waves of the Study in Child Sample and Adolescent Sample
Sankey Diagram Displaying the Transition Between the Waves of the Study in Child Sample and Adolescent Sample
Table 1.  Characteristics of the Child and Adolescent Samplesa
Characteristics of the Child and Adolescent Samplesa
Table 2.  Persistence Profile Membership in the Child Sample and the Adolescent Sample
Persistence Profile Membership in the Child Sample and the Adolescent Sample
Supplement.

eMethods.

eAppendix. Supplementary Results

eTable 1. Technical Reports Accessibility for Missing Data Analysis and Reweighting Procedure

eTable 2. Pairwise Associations Between the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaires at Each Wave in 2008 Cohort

eTable 3. Pairwise Associations Between the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaires at Each Wave in 1998 Cohort

eTable 4. Fit Indices, Information Criteria and Entropy for the Latent Profile Analysis and Latent Profile Transition Analysis in the 2008 Cohort at All Ages

eTable 5. Fit Indices, Information Criteria and Entropy for the Latent Profile Analysis and Latent Profile Transition Analysis in the 1998 Cohort at All Ages

eTable 6. Transition Percentages in the 1998 Cohort Between Ages 9 and 13 and Ages 13 and 17

eTable 7. Transition Percentages in 2008 Cohort Between Ages 3 and 5 and Ages 5 and 9

eTable 8. Transition Percentages in Boys and Girls in 1998 Cohort Between Ages 9 and 13 and Ages 13 and 17

eTable 9. Transition Percentages in Boys and Girls in the Infant Cohort Between Ages 3 and 5 and Ages 5 and 9

eTable 10. Persistence Psychopathology From Early Childhood to Late Childhood in Boys and Girls

eTable 11. Persistence Psychopathology From Childhood to Late Adolescence in Boys and Girls

eTable 12. Cohen D Effect-sizes Differences Between the Cohorts and Within Each Profile in the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire Subscale Scores at Age 9

eFigure 1. BIC and Adjusted BIC Values for the Child and Infant Cohort

eFigure 2. Fit Indices and Percentage Change in the Fit Indices by Increasing the Profiles in the Latent Transition Analysis for the Child Cohort and Infant Cohort

eFigure 3. Gender Differences Between the Profiles at Each Age in the Infant and Child Cohorts

eReferences.

1.
Wittchen  HU, Jacobi  F, Rehm  J,  et al.  The size and burden of mental disorders and other disorders of the brain in Europe 2010.   Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2011;21(9):655-679. doi:10.1016/j.euroneuro.2011.07.018PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
Rehm  J, Shield  KD.  Global burden of disease and the impact of mental and addictive disorders.   Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2019;21(2):10. doi:10.1007/s11920-019-0997-0PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
Vigo  D, Thornicroft  G, Atun  R.  Estimating the true global burden of mental illness.   Lancet Psychiatry. 2016;3(2):171-178. doi:10.1016/S2215-0366(15)00505-2PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Collins  PY, Patel  V, Joestl  SS,  et al; Scientific Advisory Board and the Executive Committee of the Grand Challenges on Global Mental Health.  Grand challenges in global mental health.   Nature. 2011;475(7354):27-30. doi:10.1038/475027aPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Arango  C, Díaz-Caneja  CM, McGorry  PD,  et al.  Preventive strategies for mental health.   Lancet Psychiatry. 2018;5(7):591-604. doi:10.1016/S2215-0366(18)30057-9PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Caspi  A, Houts  RM, Ambler  A,  et al.  Longitudinal assessment of mental health disorders and comorbidities across 4 decades among participants in the Dunedin birth cohort study.   JAMA Netw Open. 2020;3(4):e203221. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2020.3221PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
McGrath  JJ, Lim  CCW, Plana-Ripoll  O,  et al.  Comorbidity within mental disorders: a comprehensive analysis based on 145 990 survey respondents from 27 countries.   Epidemiol Psychiatr Sci. 2020;29:e153. doi:10.1017/S2045796020000633PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Plana-Ripoll  O, Pedersen  CB, Holtz  Y,  et al.  Exploring comorbidity within mental disorders among a Danish national population.   JAMA Psychiatry. 2019;76(3):259-270. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2018.3658PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Wigman  JT, van Os  J, Thiery  E,  et al.  Psychiatric diagnosis revisited: towards a system of staging and profiling combining nomothetic and idiographic parameters of momentary mental states.   PLoS One. 2013;8(3):e59559. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0059559PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
McGorry  PD, Purcell  R, Hickie  IB, Yung  AR, Pantelis  C, Jackson  HJ.  Clinical staging: a heuristic model for psychiatry and youth mental health.   Med J Aust. 2007;187(S7):S40-S42. doi:10.5694/j.1326-5377.2007.tb01335.xPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
Nivard  MG, Lubke  GH, Dolan  CV,  et al.  Joint developmental trajectories of internalizing and externalizing disorders between childhood and adolescence.   Dev Psychopathol. 2017;29(3):919-928. doi:10.1017/S0954579416000572PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Olson  SL, Choe  DE, Sameroff  AJ.  Trajectories of child externalizing problems between ages 3 and 10 years: contributions of children’s early effortful control, theory of mind, and parenting experiences.   Dev Psychopathol. 2017;29(4):1333-1351. doi:10.1017/S095457941700030XPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Fanti  KA, Henrich  CC.  Trajectories of pure and co-occurring internalizing and externalizing problems from age 2 to age 12: findings from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Study of Early Child Care.   Dev Psychol. 2010;46(5):1159-1175. doi:10.1037/a0020659PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
Petersen  KJ, Qualter  P, Humphrey  N.  The application of latent class analysis for investigating population child mental health: a systematic review.   Front Psychol. 2019;10:1214. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01214PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
McElroy  E, Shevlin  M, Murphy  J.  Internalizing and externalizing disorders in childhood and adolescence: a latent transition analysis using ALSPAC data.   Compr Psychiatry. 2017;75:75-84. doi:10.1016/j.comppsych.2017.03.003PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Willner  CJ, Gatzke-Kopp  LM, Bray  BC.  The dynamics of internalizing and externalizing comorbidity across the early school years.   Dev Psychopathol. 2016;28(4pt1):1033-1052. doi:10.1017/S0954579416000687PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
Layte  R, McCrory  C. Growing up in Ireland: Maternal health behaviours and child growth in infancy. Infant Cohort Research Report No 4. Economic and Social Research Institute. Published January 15, 2015. Accessed January 12, 2022. https://www.esri.ie/publications/growing-up-in-ireland-maternal-health-behaviours-and-child-growth-in-infancy
18.
Williams  J, Greene  S, Doyle  E,  et al. Growing Up in Ireland: The lives of 9-year-olds. Child Cohort Research Report No 1. Economic and Social Research Institute. Published December 7, 2009. Accessed January 12, 2022. https://www.esri.ie/publications/growing-up-in-ireland-the-lives-of-9-year-olds-of-cohort-08
19.
Goodman  R.  The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: a research note.   J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1997;38(5):581-586. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01545.xPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Goodman  R.  Psychometric properties of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire.   J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2001;40(11):1337-1345. doi:10.1097/00004583-200111000-00015PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
Goodman  A, Goodman  R.  Strengths and difficulties questionnaire as a dimensional measure of child mental health.   J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2009;48(4):400-403. doi:10.1097/CHI.0b013e3181985068PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
Muthén  LK, Muthén  BO. Mplus User's Guide. 7th ed. Published September 2012. Accessed January 12, 2022. https://www.statmodel.com/download/usersguide/Mplus%20user%20guide%20Ver_7_r3_web.pdf
23.
Masyn  KE. Latent class analysis and finite mixture modeling. In: Little  TD, ed.  The Oxford Handbook of Quantitative Methods in Psychology: Statistical analysis. Oxford University Press; 2013: 551–611.
24.
Nylund-Gibson  K, Grimm  R, Quirk  M, Furlong  M.  A latent transition mixture model using the three-step specification.   Struct Equ Modeling. 2014;21(3):439-454. doi:10.1080/10705511.2014.915375Google ScholarCrossref
25.
Basten  MM, Althoff  RR, Tiemeier  H,  et al.  The dysregulation profile in young children: empirically defined classes in the Generation R study.   J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2013;52(8):841-850.e2. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2013.05.007PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
Cannon  M, Coughlan  H, Clarke  M, Harley  M, Kelleher  I.  The mental health of young people in Ireland: a report of the Psychiatric Epidemology Research across the Lifespan (PERL) group.  Royal College of Surgeons in Ireland report. Published December 31, 2012. Accessed January 12, 2022. doi:10.25419/rcsi.10796129.v1
27.
Newman  DL, Moffitt  TE, Caspi  A, Magdol  L, Silva  PA, Stanton  WR.  Psychiatric disorder in a birth cohort of young adults: prevalence, comorbidity, clinical significance, and new case incidence from ages 11 to 21.   J Consult Clin Psychol. 1996;64(3):552-562. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.64.3.552PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
28.
Shah  JL, Scott  J, McGorry  PD,  et al; International Working Group on Transdiagnostic Clinical Staging in Youth Mental Health.  Transdiagnostic clinical staging in youth mental health: a first international consensus statement.   World Psychiatry. 2020;19(2):233-242. doi:10.1002/wps.20745PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
29.
Hodgins  S, Cree  A, Alderton  J, Mak  T.  From conduct disorder to severe mental illness: associations with aggressive behaviour, crime and victimization.   Psychol Med. 2008;38(7):975-987. doi:10.1017/S0033291707002164PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
30.
Harley  M, Murtagh  A, Cannon  M.  Conduct disorder: psychiatry’s greatest opportunity for prevention.   Psychol Med. 2008;38(7):929-931. doi:10.1017/S0033291708002699PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
31.
Rivenbark  JG, Odgers  CL, Caspi  A,  et al.  The high societal costs of childhood conduct problems: evidence from administrative records up to age 38 in a longitudinal birth cohort.   J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2018;59(6):703-710. doi:10.1111/jcpp.12850PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
32.
Tai  YM, Gau  CS, Gau  SS, Chiu  HW.  Prediction of ADHD to anxiety disorders: an 11-year national insurance data analysis in Taiwan.   J Atten Disord. 2013;17(8):660-669. doi:10.1177/1087054712437581PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
33.
Wade  TJ, Cairney  J, Pevalin  DJ.  Emergence of gender differences in depression during adolescence: national panel results from three countries.   J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2002;41(2):190-198. doi:10.1097/00004583-200202000-00013PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
34.
Leadbeater  BJ, Kuperminc  GP, Blatt  SJ, Hertzog  C.  A multivariate model of gender differences in adolescents’ internalizing and externalizing problems.   Dev Psychol. 1999;35(5):1268-1282. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.35.5.1268PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
35.
Van Roy  B, Groholt  B, Heyerdahl  S, Clench-Aas  J.  Understanding discrepancies in parent-child reporting of emotional and behavioural problems: effects of relational and socio-demographic factors.   BMC Psychiatry. 2010;10(1):56. doi:10.1186/1471-244X-10-56PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
Original Investigation
Psychiatry
May 10, 2022

Person-Centered Trajectories of Psychopathology From Early Childhood to Late Adolescence

Author Affiliations
  • 1Department of Psychiatry, Royal College of Surgeons in Ireland, Dublin, Ireland
  • 2Department of Sociology, Trinity College Dublin, Dublin, Ireland
  • 3Child and Adolescent Mental Health Services North Cork, Health Service Executive, Ireland
  • 4Department of Psychology, Trinity College Dublin, Dublin, Ireland
  • 5Department of Psychology, Royal College of Surgeons in Ireland, Dublin, Ireland
  • 6Centre for Academic Mental Health, Bristol Medical School, University of Bristol, Bristol, United Kingdom
  • 7School of Medicine, University College Dublin, Dublin, Ireland
  • 8Lucena Clinic, St John of God Community Mental Health Services, Dublin, Ireland
  • 9Department of Psychiatry, Beaumont Hospital, Dublin, Ireland
JAMA Netw Open. 2022;5(5):e229601. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2022.9601
Key Points

Question  How can the profiles and trajectories of psychopathology from early life (age 3 years) to late adolescence (age 17 years) be mapped with person-centered methods to accommodate problems of comorbidity and movement between different phases of development?

Findings  This cohort study of 2 nationally representative cohorts of Irish children developed 4 distinct developmental profiles for person-centered psychopathological trajectories. Over 44% of children transitioned into 1 of the 3 psychopathological profiles during development, with less than 5% of the sample experiencing persistent psychopathology.

Meaning  These results suggest that psychopathology is common in childhood but mostly limited in duration and that optimizing care in the context of finite resources may require the early identification of those with persistent phenomena.

Abstract

Importance  The understanding of the development of psychopathology has been hampered by a reliance on cross-sectional data and symptom- or disorder-centered methods. Person-centered methods can accommodate both the problems of comorbidity and the movement between different psychopathological states at different phases of development.

Objective  To examine the profiles and map the trajectories of psychopathology from early childhood to late adolescence.

Design, Setting, and Participants  This cohort study used 2 longitudinal nationally representative community-based cohorts from the Growing Up in Ireland study covering developmental periods from early childhood to late adolescence. Data in this investigation came from children and their families who participated in all waves of cohorts recruited in 2008 (children ages 3, 5, and 9 years) and 1998 (adolescents ages 9, 13, and 17 or 18 years). Both samples were weighted to account for representation and attrition. Latent transition analyses were used to map the trajectories of psychopathology. Data were analyzed between October 2020 and September 2021.

Main Outcomes and Measures  Psychopathology was measured using the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire at all ages in both samples.

Results  A total of 13 546 individuals were included in the analyses. In the child cohort, mean (SD) age was 3.0 [0.01] years; 3852 (51.3%) were male participants. In the adolescent cohort, mean age was 9.0 (0.1) years; 3082 (51.0%) were male participants. Four profiles were identified in both cohorts that could be broadly labeled as no psychopathology (incidence range, 60%-70%), high psychopathology (incidence range, 3%-5%), externalizing problems (incidence range, 15%-25%), and internalizing problems (incidence range, 7%-12%). Transition between the profiles was common in both cohorts, with 3649 of 7507 participants (48.6%) in the child cohort and 2661 of 6039 participants (44.1%) in the adolescent cohort moving into 1 of the 3 psychopathology profiles at some point in development. Transition to the high psychopathology profile was most often preceded by externalizing problems. Approximately 3% to 4% of the sample had persistent psychopathology (child cohort, 203 participants [2.7%]; adolescent cohort, 216 participants [3.6%]). All psychopathology profiles were more common in boys in early life but, by late adolescence, girls were more likely to have internalizing problems. In a cross-cohort comparison at age 9, there were differences in the sex distributions of the profiles between the samples.

Conclusions and Relevance  Using person-centered methods, this study demonstrated that from early life young peoples’ experience of psychopathology is dynamic—they can move between different mental health problems; for most children, these problems are transient, but a small proportion (fewer than 5%) have persistent difficulties. In the context of finite resources, optimizing care requires the early identification of those with persistent phenomena.

Introduction

Mental disorders are among the leading causes of lifetime disability1-3 and their prevention has been proposed as a major challenge of this century.4,5 Caspi and colleagues6 have proposed that “our understanding of mental disorders is hampered by a reliance on cross-sectional data.” It has been demonstrated, using cohort samples6 as well as retrospective survey7 and registry data,8 that simultaneous and sequential comorbidities are evident within and across diagnostic clusters, and that individuals move between diagnoses over their lifespan.

The problems of comorbidity are compounded in early life, when psychiatric symptoms are fluid phenomena that can change with development.9,10 Few studies have attempted to map the fluidity of psychopathology across early development and most use symptom-centered approaches, eg, changes in the severity of specific symptoms across time.11-13 A person-centered trajectory approach allows for data to reflect that individuals have multiple different symptoms at each time point and to map changes in these symptoms across time. This aligns closely with both the observed reports of simultaneous and sequential comorbidity6-8 and the dynamic and nondeterministic nature of psychopathology in youth.10 The use of these approaches assumes that there are clusters of psychiatric symptoms in the population but that individuals within these clusters may change with development. A review found 23 eligible studies using latent class analysis, but only 7 investigated a broad range of symptoms occurring simultaneously.14 Latent transition analysis (ie, transition between the classes) has rarely been investigated15,16 and aligns with the dynamic nature of psychopathology in youth.10 Moreover, there are well-established differences in biological sex between the different mental disorders. It is unclear if the differences in biological sex also affect longitudinal trajectories.

Using data from 2 longitudinal cohorts and latent profile transition analysis (LPTA), we aimed to map individuals’ trajectories of psychopathology from early childhood (age 3 years) to late adolescence (age 18 years). First, we examined the symptom clustering and the percentage of the population within these profiles at multiple points across development. Second, we mapped the trajectories between the different profiles. We ran supplementary investigations examining whether there were sex differences in the profiles and generational differences based on a comparison of results between cohorts at age 9 years.

Methods
Participants

We used data from the Growing Up in Ireland study (GUI), which comprises 2 large nationally representative cohorts spanning from infancy to late adolescence (the 1998 cohort and 2008 cohort). The 2008 cohort covers the period of development from ages 9 months to 9 years (7507 participants); for the purpose of this investigation the 2008 cohort is referred to as the child sample. The 1998 cohort covers the period from ages 9 years to 17 or 18 years (6039 participants); for the purpose of this investigation the 2008 cohort is referred to as the adolescent sample. (For further information on the sampling method, representative weighting, attrition, and low to follow-up adjustment see eMethods and eTable 1 in the Supplement). Full information on these cohorts is available in the studies from Layte and McCrory17 and Williams et al.18 Informed consent was obtained from the parent or guardian of the participating child and assent was obtained from the child themselves at each wave of the studies by the Growing Up in Ireland study team. Ethical approval for the GUI study was granted by the research ethics committee of the Health Research Board in Ireland and Royal College of Surgeons in Ireland, and approval for this study was granted by the research ethics committee of the Royal College of Surgeons in Ireland.

Demographics and Psychopathology Measurement

Demographic variables reported to characterize the sample included age, sex, handedness, and nationality (Irish or non-Irish) of the child, the nationality of the primary caregiver, urbanicity (urban or rural), and highest level of education of the primary caregiver (Table 1). Nationality was used instead of race or ethnicity as this information was available for the participating child and the primary care giver in both cohorts.

Psychopathology was measured using the 4 problem subscales of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ): emotional problems, peer problems, conduct problems, and hyperactivity.19-21 It was measured at ages 3, 5, 9 years in the child sample and ages 9, 13, and 17 years in the adolescent sample. In both cohorts and at all ages all data was provided by the primary caregiver (eTables 2 and 3 in the Supplement).

Statistical Analysis

Analyses were carried out in Mplus version 822 and Stata version 15 (StataCorp). Analyses were conducted using latent profile transition analysis following guidelines from Masyn.23 Both cohorts were weighted to ensure representation and to account for attrition (eMethods in the Supplement). Global model fit criteria (bayesian information criteria [BIC], adjusted BIC, and Akaike information criterion) and profile interpretability were used to estimate the optimum number of profiles.24 Time varying and time invariant solutions were compared using log likelihood ratio tests and BIC. Multinomial logistic regression was conducted to examine potential differences in sex distribution between the profiles, transitions, persistence of profiles, and cohorts. These analyses were adjusted for demographic difference between the cohorts, and results are reported as incidence risk ratios (IRR) (eMethods in the Supplement). The threshold of significance was set at α = .05.

Results
Descriptive Statistics

Data were available for 7507 children from the child sample who participated in the 3 waves of the study (ages 3, 5, and 9 years) and for 6039 children from the adolescent sample (ages 9, 13, and 17 or 18 years) (Table 1). In the child cohort, the mean (SD) age was 3.0 [0.01] years and 3852 (51.3%) were male participants. In the adolescent cohort, mean age was 9.0 (0.1) years and 3082 (51.0%) were male participants. The adolescent sample had a significantly higher percentage of non-Irish children (632 participants [10.5%] vs 71 participants [0.9%]). The child sample had a higher percentage of caregivers with higher primary or postgraduate degrees (postgraduate degree, 997 [13.3%] vs 370 [6.1%]) and a higher percentage of caregivers who were non-Irish (1460 [19.5%] vs 898 [14.9%]).

Latent Profile Analysis

We fitted a series of cross-sectional latent profile analysis (LPA) models for each age starting with a 2-profile model. The number of profiles were sequentially increased from 1 to 8. There was continuously improving fit indices with increasing profiles at all ages (eTable 4, eTable 5, eFigure 1 in the Supplement). The diminishing gains in the fit indices indicated that a 4-profile model was the best fit to the data such that 4 distinct symptom clusters existed at each time point, for both cohorts. For completeness, we also compared the 3-profile and 5-profile solutions for the latent profile transition analyses with the 4-profile solution.

Latent Profile Transition Analysis

Based on the information gathered in the LPA we examined the LPTA models. Similarly, while we observed continuously decreasing fit, there was an elbow with the 4-profile model across indices in both cohorts (eMethods and eFigure 2 in the Supplement). We also considered the 3-profile and 5-profile solution relative to the 4-profile solution as well as time varying and time invariant solutions (eAppendix in the Supplement). Briefly, the 4-profile solution had better global model fit (base on Bayesian information criteria metrics and diminishing gains) and clearer profile distinguishing features relative to the 3-profile or 5-profile solution. In both cohorts the time vary model had significantly better model fit (child sample, χ232 = 717.6, P < .001; adolescent sample, χ232 = 201.9, P < .001). The full rationale for the selection of a 4-profile time-varying solution are presented in supplementary material (eAppendix in the Supplement). Thus, the final model in both cohorts (from ages 3 to 9 years and 9 to 17 years) over 3 waves consisted of 4 profiles with 64 potential transition pathways between the profiles. In both cohorts, we examined the mean and standard error of each of the SDQ subscale scores in each of the profiles (Figure 1). There were similar profiles observed in both cohorts. Based on the profiles produced by LPTA the groups were labeled: no psychopathology, externalizing problems, internalizing problems, and high psychopathology.

Profiles
No Psychopathology

This profile was characterized by low scores (ie, 30th to 40th percentile) on all SDQ subscales at each wave in both sample groups. It accounted for approximately 60% to 70% of the participants in each sample at any point in time (eg, 4487 children [59.8%] in child cohort wave 1), with similar percentages for both sexes.

High Psychopathology

This profile was characterized by significant (above 90th percentile) scores across all SDQ problem subscales at almost all time points (Figure 1). This profile accounted for approximately 3% to 5% of the sample at all ages (eg, 346 children [4.6%] in child cohort wave 1). There was a higher proportion of boys in the high psychopathology profile at all ages of the child sample (age 3: IRR, 1.76; 95% CI, 1.41-2.21; P < .001; age 5: IRR, 2.02; 95% CI, 1.63-2.51; P < .001; age 9: IRR, 2.34; 95% CI, 1.79-3.08; P < .001) but only at age 13 years in the adolescent sample (IRR, 1.30; 95% CI, 1.02-1.65; P = .04) relative to the no psychopathology profile.

Externalizing Problems

This profile was characterized by elevated scores (70th to 90th percentile) in the hyperactivity and conduct subscales but average (45th to 72nd percentile) scores in the emotional and peer problems subscales. This profile accounted for approximately 15% to 25% of the population at all ages in both cohorts (eg, 1804 children [24.0%] in child cohort wave 1) and there was a greater proportion of boys in this profile at all ages relative to the no psychopathology profile in both child (age 3: IRR, 1.32; 95% CI, 1.18-1.47; P < .001; age 5: IRR, 1.55; 95% CI, 1.38-1.73; P < .001; age 9: IRR, 1.80; 95% CI, 1.58-2.05; P < .001) and adolescent samples (age 9: IRR, 1.34; 95% CI, 1.18-1.52; P < .001; age 13: IRR, 1.22; 95% CI, 1.08-1.39; P < .001; age 17: IRR, 1.38; 95% CI, 1.21-1.57; P < .001).

Internalizing Problems

This profile was characterized by significant (above 90th percentile) scores in the peer problems subscale and increasing levels of emotional problems across development (ie, 58th percentile at age 3 years to the 80th percentile at 9 years; in the adolescent sample, from the 79th percentile at age 9 years to the 93rd percentile at 17 years). Average (45th to 60th percentile) scores were reported for participants in the hyperactivity and conduct subscales. This profile accounted for approximately 7% to 12% of participants at all ages in both cohorts (eg, 869 children [11.6%] in the child cohort wave 1). In early life, there was a higher proportion of boys in this profile (age 3: IRR, 1.51; 95% CI, 1.30-1.75; P < .001; age 5: IRR, 1.18; 95% CI, 1.01-1.37; P = .04; age 9: IRR, 1.30; 95% CI, 1.12-1.53; P = .001); however, by late adolescence there were more girls in this profile (age 17: IRR, 1.76; 95% CI, 1.47-2.12; P < .001) relative to the no psychopathology profile.

Trajectories Between the Profiles

In the child sample, there was cross-wave shifting between all profiles, particularly in the 3 psychopathology profiles (Figure 2; eTables 3, 4, and 7 in the Supplement). Forty-eight percent of participants fell into 1 of the 3 psychopathology profiles at some point between early childhood and late childhood. Approximately 40% to 50% of those with internalizing problems remitted by the following wave of the study (eg, 375 children [43.1%] between wave 1 and wave 2). Only 12% to 30% of those with the externalizing problems profile remitted by the next wave of the study (230 children [12.7%] between wave 1 and wave 2). Similar to the adolescent sample, very few individuals transitioned directly from no psychopathology to high psychopathology (age 3, 27 [0.6%] and age 5, 14 [0.3%]) or high psychopathology to no psychopathology (age 3, 25 [7.2%] and age 5, 17 [4.5%]). Children who presented with new incidents of high psychopathology were previously most often within the externalizing problems profile (age 5, 129 [61.3%] and age 9, 95 [74.3%]). There were several significant differences in the transition percentages between boys and girls at both waves. The overall pattern suggested that in early life a higher percentage of girls had deescalating trajectories relative to boys (eTable 9 in the Supplement).

Adolescent Sample

There was a lower rate of transition between the profiles relative to the child sample, but transition was still observed in the 3 psychopathology profiles (ie, high psychopathology, externalizing problems, and internalizing problems). A total of 2661 participants (44.1%) fell into 1 of the 3 psychopathology profiles at some point between childhood and late adolescence (Figure 2; eTable 6 in the Supplement). Twenty-five percent to 42% of those with internalizing problems remitted by the following wave of the study (eg, 230 adolescents [41.5%] between wave 1 and wave 2). Only 8% to 15% of those in the externalizing problems profile remitted by the next wave of the study (eg, 100 adolescents [7.9%] between wave 1 and wave 2). Almost no individuals transitioned directly from no psychopathology to high psychopathology (age 9, 0 and age 13, 10 [0.2%]) and very few from high psychopathology to no psychopathology (age 9, 13 [4.5%] and age 13, 21 [7.3%]). Most new incidents of high psychopathology were preceded by externalizing problems (age 13, 129 [91.1%] and age 17, 146 [89.9%]). There were some (limited) differences in the transition percentage between boys and girls (eTable 8 in the Supplement). Most notably, girls were more likely to transition to the internalizing problem profile from the other 3 profiles relative to boys by age 17 years.

Persistence of Profiles in the Child Sample and Adolescent Sample

The trajectories of those with persistent psychopathology profiles (ie, remaining in a specific profile over the 3 waves of each cohort) is of potential clinical importance. The percentage of those with persisting psychopathology profiles varied widely across cohorts (Table 2). In the child sample, only 16% to 23% had a form of psychopathology at age 3 years that persisted through to late childhood (internalizing problem, 140 participants [16.2%]; externalizing problem, 407 participants [22.6%]). In the adolescent sample, 20% to 52% of those with a form of psychopathology at age 9 years persisted through to late adolescence (eg, 160 [29%] adolescents with internalizing problems). Of those with no psychopathology at the first wave, 3378 (85.8%) remained so. Between 3% to 4% of all children were in a profile that persisted and was deemed significant as they had subscale scores greater than the 90th percentile (eg, 56 adolescents [0.9%] with high psychopathology and 160 adolescents [2.7%] with internalizing problems). Finally, in the child sample a higher proportion of boys had persistently high psychopathology (eTable 10 in the Supplement) relative to the persistently no psychopathology profile (IRR, 3.74; 95% CI, 2.08-6.74; P < .001), while in the adolescent sample a higher distribution of boys had persistent externalizing problems (eTable 11 in the Supplement) relative to the persistently no psychopathology profile (IRR, 1.57; 95% CI, 1.33-1.87; P < .001).

Cross-Cohort Comparison

The full results of the cross-cohort comparison are displayed in supplementary materials (eTable 12 in the Supplement). Briefly, we observed little difference between the profiles, but there were cohort by gender interactions indicating that there was a significantly higher proportion of boys in all 3 psychopathology classes in the child sample relative to the adolescent sample.

Discussion

Using 2 nationally representative cohorts that spanned from early childhood to late adolescence and person-centered analysis methods, we identified 4 distinct psychopathology profiles for all ages, namely: no psychopathology (approximately 65%), high psychopathology (approximately 5%), externalizing problems (approximately 20%), and internalizing problems (approximately 10%). These groups broadly mirror other studies using latent class analysis to investigate multiple symptoms of psychopathology.14-16,25

We found that movement between the different profiles was common; between 45% and 50% of young people fell into 1 of the 3 psychopathology categories at some point during development but only 3% to 5% had persistent problems. A similar prevalence of mental disorders is seen in community samples using clinical interviews.26,27 This suggests that, for most young people, mental health problems are common but transient phenomena.

Our results complement recent findings that demonstrate the cross-sectional and sequential comorbidity of mental disorders6-8 and are in line with research emphasizing the fluid nature of psychopathology in youth.28 A small proportion of the samples (3% to 4%) had persistent psychopathology, but most of those with psychopathology had remitting or changing trajectories. The identification of early risk factors identifying those with persistent psychopathology is of paramount importance as they are likely to need the most intervention and resources throughout life. Early intervention in these individuals may improve their outcome.

In line with our expectations, there was more fluidity between profiles in early childhood than adolescence. We observed that a greater proportion of children in the externalizing problems profile remained in this profile relative to the internalizing problems profile. By adolescence the vast majority of those who joined the high psychopathology profile had transitioned from the externalizing problems profile (age 13, 91% and age 17, 90%). This suggests that there was a subsample of those in the externalizing problems profile who were vulnerable to escalating problems and may require monitoring. These findings aligned with observations from similar investigations.15,16 McElroy and colleagues15 found that those with internalizing problems at baseline were more likely to transition to the normative class than those with externalizing problems. They suggest that experiencing early externalizing problems may place an individual at greater risk of cross-domain comorbidity. This is consistent with the high proportion of adults in inpatient psychiatric services29,30 and the high service usage across judicial and health care domains31 of those with a history of conduct disorder. It also aligns with the observation that those with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in early life are more likely to have concurrent and sequential comorbidity than their peers.32 Thus, identifying risk factors for those with externalizing problems and an escalating trajectory, distinguishing them from their peers and intervening early, may assist in reducing the risk of comorbidity and societal health burden and social cost.

In the child sample, boys were more likely to make up a greater proportion of all psychopathology subgroups (eFigure 3 in the Supplement). However, by late adolescence, there were more girls in the internalizing problems profile with more boys in the externalizing problems profile. These trends broadly align with observations from developmental epidemiological samples.33,34 The most notable 10-year cross-cohort difference at age 9 years was the higher percentage of boys in the psychopathology profiles in the child sample relative to the adolescent sample. There are several potential explanations for this observation. This could be a cohort effect explained by unmeasured demographic differences between the samples and recruitment strategies or generational differences in those with psychopathology in mid-to-late childhood. To our knowledge, this trend has not been observed elsewhere and requires further investigation.

An important outstanding question is what other risk factors could accurately project to contribute to these profiles and the transition between them? Future research should examine the risk factors and distal outcomes for these profiles and the trajectory between any 2 profiles. Of particular importance is the identification of risk factors for those with persistent psychopathology vs those who remit. Also important will be the identification of risk factors for those with the externalizing problems profile who go on to have high psychopathology.

Strengths and Limitations

There are a number of strengths to this investigation. First, we used nationally representative data from 2 large longitudinal cohorts spanning across the majority of early life development. In both cohorts the same measures were administered to the respondents allowing for continuity of the findings across cohorts.

This study had several limitations. The SDQ is a widely used measure of psychopathology, but it is limited to the examination of internalizing and externalizing problems. This leaves open the possibility for additional profiles and trajectories if other aspects of psychopathology were measured, for example psychotic experiences and thought disorders.6 Moreover, the SDQ was parent report only, which may differ from self-report, particularly in adolescence.35

For theoretical reasons we opted to allow time-varying profiles. The rationale for this was that psychopathology is likely to change with time. Thus, we expected that the profile would not be the same at each wave of each study. While the configural model selection in both cohorts at all ages suggested a 4-profile solution, there were degrees of variance in the means of each subscale at the different ages. The additional constraint of time invariance had a significantly poorer fit to the data and is theoretically less plausible given the development period covered in the analyses. Thus, while our profiles are similar across the ages they are not to be interpreted as absolutely identical at each age.

Conclusions

There is a complex trajectory of psychopathology across youth. The main findings of this study were: (1) mental health problems in youth appear to fall into 3 distinct categories; (2) up to half of all young people had a mental health problem at least once; (3) 90% of new incidence of high psychopathology were preceded by externalizing problems; and (4) roughly 1 in 20 young people had persistent and clinically relevant mental health problems. It is of paramount importance to identify factors that distinguish those with persisting problems and escalating trajectories so that resources can be appropriately directed.

Back to top
Article Information

Accepted for Publication: March 12, 2022.

Published: May 10, 2022. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2022.9601

Correction: This article was corrected July 26, 2022, to fix errors in the axis label of Figure 2.

Open Access: This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the CC-BY License. © 2022 Healy C et al. JAMA Network Open.

Corresponding Author: Colm Healy, PhD, Department of Psychiatry, Royal College of Surgeons in Ireland, Ardilaun House, St Stephens Green, Dublin 2, Ireland (colmhealy@rcsi.com).

Author Contributions: Dr Healy had full access to all of the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis. Prof Cannon was the senior author of this study.

Concept and design: Healy, Zammit, Cannon.

Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: Healy, Brannigan, Dooley, Staines, Keeley, Whelan, Clarke, Kelleher.

Drafting of the manuscript: Healy, Staines, Keeley, Cannon.

Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Healy, Brannigan, Dooley, Staines, Keeley, Whelan, Clarke, Zammit, Kelleher.

Statistical analysis: Healy, Brannigan, Staines, Whelan.

Obtained funding: Healy, Clarke, Cannon.

Administrative, technical, or material support: Healy, Brannigan, Dooley.

Supervision: Clarke, Kelleher, Cannon.

Conflict of Interest Disclosures: None reported.

Funding/Support: This research was funded by the Health Research Board of Ireland. Dr Healy was funded by the Health Research Board in Ireland (grant No. ILP-PHR-2019-009). Dr Cannon was funded by the European Research Council Consolidator Award (grant No. 724809 iHEAR). Dr Clarke was funded by a Health Research Award from the Health Research Board of Ireland (No. HRA-PHR-2015–1130) and an Irish Research Council award (No. COALESCE/2019/61). The Growing Up in Ireland study was funded by the Government of Ireland.

Role of the Funder/Sponsor: The funders had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

Additional Contributions: We wish to thank all participants and their families and the Growing Up in Ireland Study Team.

References
1.
Wittchen  HU, Jacobi  F, Rehm  J,  et al.  The size and burden of mental disorders and other disorders of the brain in Europe 2010.   Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2011;21(9):655-679. doi:10.1016/j.euroneuro.2011.07.018PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
Rehm  J, Shield  KD.  Global burden of disease and the impact of mental and addictive disorders.   Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2019;21(2):10. doi:10.1007/s11920-019-0997-0PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
Vigo  D, Thornicroft  G, Atun  R.  Estimating the true global burden of mental illness.   Lancet Psychiatry. 2016;3(2):171-178. doi:10.1016/S2215-0366(15)00505-2PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Collins  PY, Patel  V, Joestl  SS,  et al; Scientific Advisory Board and the Executive Committee of the Grand Challenges on Global Mental Health.  Grand challenges in global mental health.   Nature. 2011;475(7354):27-30. doi:10.1038/475027aPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Arango  C, Díaz-Caneja  CM, McGorry  PD,  et al.  Preventive strategies for mental health.   Lancet Psychiatry. 2018;5(7):591-604. doi:10.1016/S2215-0366(18)30057-9PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Caspi  A, Houts  RM, Ambler  A,  et al.  Longitudinal assessment of mental health disorders and comorbidities across 4 decades among participants in the Dunedin birth cohort study.   JAMA Netw Open. 2020;3(4):e203221. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2020.3221PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
McGrath  JJ, Lim  CCW, Plana-Ripoll  O,  et al.  Comorbidity within mental disorders: a comprehensive analysis based on 145 990 survey respondents from 27 countries.   Epidemiol Psychiatr Sci. 2020;29:e153. doi:10.1017/S2045796020000633PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Plana-Ripoll  O, Pedersen  CB, Holtz  Y,  et al.  Exploring comorbidity within mental disorders among a Danish national population.   JAMA Psychiatry. 2019;76(3):259-270. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2018.3658PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Wigman  JT, van Os  J, Thiery  E,  et al.  Psychiatric diagnosis revisited: towards a system of staging and profiling combining nomothetic and idiographic parameters of momentary mental states.   PLoS One. 2013;8(3):e59559. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0059559PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
McGorry  PD, Purcell  R, Hickie  IB, Yung  AR, Pantelis  C, Jackson  HJ.  Clinical staging: a heuristic model for psychiatry and youth mental health.   Med J Aust. 2007;187(S7):S40-S42. doi:10.5694/j.1326-5377.2007.tb01335.xPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
Nivard  MG, Lubke  GH, Dolan  CV,  et al.  Joint developmental trajectories of internalizing and externalizing disorders between childhood and adolescence.   Dev Psychopathol. 2017;29(3):919-928. doi:10.1017/S0954579416000572PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Olson  SL, Choe  DE, Sameroff  AJ.  Trajectories of child externalizing problems between ages 3 and 10 years: contributions of children’s early effortful control, theory of mind, and parenting experiences.   Dev Psychopathol. 2017;29(4):1333-1351. doi:10.1017/S095457941700030XPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Fanti  KA, Henrich  CC.  Trajectories of pure and co-occurring internalizing and externalizing problems from age 2 to age 12: findings from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Study of Early Child Care.   Dev Psychol. 2010;46(5):1159-1175. doi:10.1037/a0020659PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
Petersen  KJ, Qualter  P, Humphrey  N.  The application of latent class analysis for investigating population child mental health: a systematic review.   Front Psychol. 2019;10:1214. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01214PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
McElroy  E, Shevlin  M, Murphy  J.  Internalizing and externalizing disorders in childhood and adolescence: a latent transition analysis using ALSPAC data.   Compr Psychiatry. 2017;75:75-84. doi:10.1016/j.comppsych.2017.03.003PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Willner  CJ, Gatzke-Kopp  LM, Bray  BC.  The dynamics of internalizing and externalizing comorbidity across the early school years.   Dev Psychopathol. 2016;28(4pt1):1033-1052. doi:10.1017/S0954579416000687PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
Layte  R, McCrory  C. Growing up in Ireland: Maternal health behaviours and child growth in infancy. Infant Cohort Research Report No 4. Economic and Social Research Institute. Published January 15, 2015. Accessed January 12, 2022. https://www.esri.ie/publications/growing-up-in-ireland-maternal-health-behaviours-and-child-growth-in-infancy
18.
Williams  J, Greene  S, Doyle  E,  et al. Growing Up in Ireland: The lives of 9-year-olds. Child Cohort Research Report No 1. Economic and Social Research Institute. Published December 7, 2009. Accessed January 12, 2022. https://www.esri.ie/publications/growing-up-in-ireland-the-lives-of-9-year-olds-of-cohort-08
19.
Goodman  R.  The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: a research note.   J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1997;38(5):581-586. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01545.xPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Goodman  R.  Psychometric properties of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire.   J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2001;40(11):1337-1345. doi:10.1097/00004583-200111000-00015PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
Goodman  A, Goodman  R.  Strengths and difficulties questionnaire as a dimensional measure of child mental health.   J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2009;48(4):400-403. doi:10.1097/CHI.0b013e3181985068PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
Muthén  LK, Muthén  BO. Mplus User's Guide. 7th ed. Published September 2012. Accessed January 12, 2022. https://www.statmodel.com/download/usersguide/Mplus%20user%20guide%20Ver_7_r3_web.pdf
23.
Masyn  KE. Latent class analysis and finite mixture modeling. In: Little  TD, ed.  The Oxford Handbook of Quantitative Methods in Psychology: Statistical analysis. Oxford University Press; 2013: 551–611.
24.
Nylund-Gibson  K, Grimm  R, Quirk  M, Furlong  M.  A latent transition mixture model using the three-step specification.   Struct Equ Modeling. 2014;21(3):439-454. doi:10.1080/10705511.2014.915375Google ScholarCrossref
25.
Basten  MM, Althoff  RR, Tiemeier  H,  et al.  The dysregulation profile in young children: empirically defined classes in the Generation R study.   J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2013;52(8):841-850.e2. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2013.05.007PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
Cannon  M, Coughlan  H, Clarke  M, Harley  M, Kelleher  I.  The mental health of young people in Ireland: a report of the Psychiatric Epidemology Research across the Lifespan (PERL) group.  Royal College of Surgeons in Ireland report. Published December 31, 2012. Accessed January 12, 2022. doi:10.25419/rcsi.10796129.v1
27.
Newman  DL, Moffitt  TE, Caspi  A, Magdol  L, Silva  PA, Stanton  WR.  Psychiatric disorder in a birth cohort of young adults: prevalence, comorbidity, clinical significance, and new case incidence from ages 11 to 21.   J Consult Clin Psychol. 1996;64(3):552-562. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.64.3.552PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
28.
Shah  JL, Scott  J, McGorry  PD,  et al; International Working Group on Transdiagnostic Clinical Staging in Youth Mental Health.  Transdiagnostic clinical staging in youth mental health: a first international consensus statement.   World Psychiatry. 2020;19(2):233-242. doi:10.1002/wps.20745PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
29.
Hodgins  S, Cree  A, Alderton  J, Mak  T.  From conduct disorder to severe mental illness: associations with aggressive behaviour, crime and victimization.   Psychol Med. 2008;38(7):975-987. doi:10.1017/S0033291707002164PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
30.
Harley  M, Murtagh  A, Cannon  M.  Conduct disorder: psychiatry’s greatest opportunity for prevention.   Psychol Med. 2008;38(7):929-931. doi:10.1017/S0033291708002699PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
31.
Rivenbark  JG, Odgers  CL, Caspi  A,  et al.  The high societal costs of childhood conduct problems: evidence from administrative records up to age 38 in a longitudinal birth cohort.   J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2018;59(6):703-710. doi:10.1111/jcpp.12850PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
32.
Tai  YM, Gau  CS, Gau  SS, Chiu  HW.  Prediction of ADHD to anxiety disorders: an 11-year national insurance data analysis in Taiwan.   J Atten Disord. 2013;17(8):660-669. doi:10.1177/1087054712437581PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
33.
Wade  TJ, Cairney  J, Pevalin  DJ.  Emergence of gender differences in depression during adolescence: national panel results from three countries.   J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2002;41(2):190-198. doi:10.1097/00004583-200202000-00013PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
34.
Leadbeater  BJ, Kuperminc  GP, Blatt  SJ, Hertzog  C.  A multivariate model of gender differences in adolescents’ internalizing and externalizing problems.   Dev Psychol. 1999;35(5):1268-1282. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.35.5.1268PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
35.
Van Roy  B, Groholt  B, Heyerdahl  S, Clench-Aas  J.  Understanding discrepancies in parent-child reporting of emotional and behavioural problems: effects of relational and socio-demographic factors.   BMC Psychiatry. 2010;10(1):56. doi:10.1186/1471-244X-10-56PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
×