Key PointsQuestion
Which cognitive measure among global cognition, memory, processing speed, and verbal fluency is most useful in assessing risk of future dementia when combined with gait decline?
Findings
In this cohort study of 16 855 relatively healthy older people in Australia and the US, a dual decline in gait and cognitive function compared with nondecliners was significantly associated with increased risk of dementia. This risk was highest in those with both gait and memory decline.
Meaning
These results highlight the importance of gait in dementia risk assessment and suggest that dual decline in gait speed and a memory measure may be the best combination to assess future dementia.
Importance
Dual decline in gait speed and cognition has been found to be associated with increased dementia risk in previous studies. However, it is unclear if risks are conferred by a decline in domain-specific cognition and gait.
Objective
To examine associations between dual decline in gait speed and cognition (ie, global, memory, processing speed, and verbal fluency) with risk of dementia.
Design, Setting, and Participants
This cohort study used data from older adults in Australia and the US who participated in a randomized clinical trial testing low-dose aspirin between 2010 and 2017. Eligible participants in the original trial were aged 70 years or older, or 65 years or older for US participants identifying as African American or Hispanic. Data analysis was performed between October 2020 and November 2021.
Exposures
Gait speed, measured at 0, 2, 4, and 6 years and trial close-out in 2017. Cognitive measures included Modified Mini-Mental State examination (3MS) for global cognition, Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised (HVLT-R) for memory, Symbol Digit Modalities (SDMT) for processing speed, and Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWAT-F) for verbal fluency, assessed at years 0, 1, 3, 5, and close-out. Participants were classified into 4 groups: dual decline in gait and cognition, gait decline only, cognitive decline only, and nondecliners. Cognitive decline was defined as membership of the lowest tertile of annual change. Gait decline was defined as a decline in gait speed of 0.05 m/s or greater per year across the study.
Main Outcomes and Measures
Dementia (using Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders [Fourth Edition] criteria) was adjudicated by an expert panel using cognitive tests, functional status, and clinical records. Cox proportional hazard models were used to estimate risk of dementia adjusting for covariates, with death as competing risk.
Results
Of 19 114 randomized participants, 16 855 (88.2%) had longitudinal gait and cognitive data for inclusion in this study (mean [SD] age, 75.0 [4.4] years; 9435 women [56.0%], 7558 participants [44.8%] with 12 or more years of education). Compared with nondecliners, risk of dementia was highest in the gait plus HVLT-R decliners (hazard ratio [HR], 24.9; 95% CI, 16.5-37.6), followed by the gait plus 3MS (HR, 22.2; 95% CI, 15.0-32.9), gait plus COWAT-F (HR, 4.7; 95% CI, 3.5-6.3), and gait plus SDMT (HR, 4.3; 95% CI, 3.2-5.8) groups. Dual decliners had a higher risk of dementia than those with either gait or cognitive decline alone for 3MS and HVLT-R.
Conclusions and Relevance
Of domains examined, the combination of decline in gait speed with memory had the strongest association with dementia risk. These findings support the inclusion of gait speed in dementia risk screening assessments.
The number of people with dementia is estimated to be 50 million worldwide and projected to grow to 150 million by 2050.1 As much of the neuropathology of dementia is believed to progressively accumulate 20 to 30 years before diagnosis,2 it is important that at-risk individuals are identified so that modifiable risk factors are addressed and available interventions provided.
Changes in motor performance are increasingly recognized as early markers of cognitive decline and dementia. Slow gait speed is associated with both cognitive decline and a greater risk of dementia.3,4 These associations may be because of underlying shared risk factors, such as cardiovascular disease, diabetes,5 and low physical activity,6 or common underlying neural pathways7-9 disrupted by cerebral small vessel disease10 or Alzheimer disease (AD) pathology.11 Over the past 10 years, studies have focused on improving sensitivity of motor biomarkers via combination with cognitive measures. For example, the presence of slow gait and subjective cognitive complaint (Motoric Cognitive Risk Syndrome [MCR]) has been associated with dementia over and above its individual components.12 This raises questions as to whether simultaneous (ie, dual) decline in gait and cognition over time is more strongly associated with future dementia risk than decline in either construct alone. Two previous studies showed stronger associations in those with dual decline compared with those showing no decline. However, these studies either had a small number of participants converting to dementia (22 participants),13 harmonized measures across diverse study conditions,14 and/or used limited cognitive measures (either global cognition13 or immediate memory14), and to our knowledge none have investigated nonamnestic cognitive domains. In most clinical settings it is unusual to apply a range of cognitive measures, especially domain-specific tests such as memory, attention, language, processing speed, and executive function. As it is already known that gait is more strongly correlated with executive function and processing speed,15 we hypothesized that, in addition to dual decliners having greater risk of dementia than nondecliners, the magnitude of elevated risk would be greatest in those with gait decline plus memory decline, as this measure would capture a broader range of cognitive domains and brain pathology. We aimed to examine these hypotheses using cognitive measures from a single, large clinical trial that assessed global cognition, processing speed, memory, and verbal fluency.
Data were collected as part of the ASPREE (ASPirin in Reducing Events in the Elderly) trial (ClinicalTrials.gov identifier NCT01038583; International Standard Randomized Controlled Trial Number ISRCTN83772183). ASPREE was a double-masked, randomized, placebo-controlled trial conducted in Australia and the US of low-dose (100 mg) daily aspirin in 19 114 community-dwelling older people.16 Recruitment spanned 2010 to 2014 and randomized treatment concluded in 2017.17 Inclusion criteria were age 70 years or older (or ages 65 and older for US participants belonging to a minority group), free of cardiovascular disease, dementia, or physical disability (severe difficulty with 1 or more of Katz’s activities of daily living18) and expected to live longer than 5 years. During trial recruitment participants self-identified as Hispanic and/or 1 or more of the following: Aboriginal or Torres Strait Islander; American Indian; Asian; Black or African American; more than 1 race; Native Hawaiian, other Pacific Islander, or Maori; non-Hispanic White; and other. Absence of dementia at baseline was confirmed in writing by participants’ general practitioners and by cognitive screen (score of 78 or higher on the Modified Mini-Mental State examination [3MS]). All participants provided written informed consent, and ethics approval was granted by the ethics committees for Monash University and the Royal Australian College of General Practitioners in Australia and all participating clinic sites in the US.
Gait speed (in m/s) was measured at face-to-face visits at years 0, 2, 4, and 6 and the close-out visit in 2017. Participants completed 2 walks of 3 m at usual pace from standing start, with at least 1 meter at the end of the course to prevent slowing. The mean average of 2 walks was used for analysis.
Cognitive measures included a test of global cognitive function (3MS),19 delayed free recall (Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised [HVLT-R-delay]),20 processing speed (Symbol Digit Modalities [SDMT]),21 and verbal fluency (Controlled Oral Word Association Test–single-letter version [COWAT-F]).22 Each was assessed at years 0, 1, 3, 5, and 2017 close-out.
Suspected cognitive concerns (3MS score below 78 or 10.15 points below predicted score; report of memory concerns to specialist, clinician diagnosis of dementia, prescription of cholinesterase inhibitors [Australia only]) triggered additional cognitive and functional assessment administered following a minimum 6-week delay to exclude delirium. These included the Alzheimer Disease Assessment Scale—Cognitive subscale, Lurian overlapping figures, and the Alzheimer Disease Cooperative Study Activities of Daily Living Scale.23 These data, plus available laboratory tests, brain scans, hospital, and/or specialist clinical case notes were considered by an international dementia end point adjudication committee comprising a panel of neurologists, neuropsychologists, and geriatricians from Australia and the US. The expert committee adjudicated dementia according to Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (Fourth Edition) (DSM-IV) criteria.23 Gait data were not used for end point adjudication.
Annual change in cognition and gait across the study period (prior to diagnosis of dementia, death, or last follow-up) was estimated for participants with longitudinal gait and cognitive data using multilevel linear regression, including random slopes and intercepts.24 Participants missing all data for 1 or more cognitive test were included in analysis of other tests, and participants missing data at particular time points were analyzed using other available time points. Gait decliners were classified using a cut-point of decline in gait speed of 0.05 m/s or more per year.25 Cognitive decliners were those in the lowest tertile of annual change in 3MS, HVLT-R-delay, SDMT,14 or COWAT-F scores. Participants were then classified into 4 phenotypic groups for each cognitive measure (3MS, HVLT-R-delay, SDMT, COWAT-F). For example, for 3MS, the 4 resultant groups were: (1) nondecliners, or those with less than 0.05 m/s annual decline in gait speed and in the highest two-thirds of annual change in 3MS; (2) cognitive decliners, who had less than 0.05 m/s annual decline in gait speed and in the lowest third (at greatest decline) of annual change in 3MS; (3) gait decliners, who had greater than 0.05 m/s annual decline in gait speed (at greatest decline) and in the highest two-thirds of annual change in 3MS; and (4) dual decliners, who had greater than 0.05 m/s annual decline in gait speed and who were also in the lowest third of annual change in 3MS.
We then employed Cox proportional hazards regression to examine whether group membership was associated with incident dementia. Death was modeled as a competing risk (via cause-specific hazard modeling26) and tied failures handled via the Breslow method. Resultant cause-specific hazards reflect the ratio of instantaneous risk of dementia, given participants were both alive and had not reached the dementia end point. Nondecliners formed the reference. As Kaplan-Meier survival estimates are inappropriate in the presence of competing risks,26 we present cumulative incidence curves. We adjusted for demographic characteristics (age, sex, education, and country) but did not adjust for comorbidities, as gait speed is included as an overall marker of the impact of these comorbidities on function. We did not statistically adjust for race or ethnicity in our models as numbers for these groups were low, and we did not hypothesize a unique association of gait and cognitive decline with subsequent dementia for racial or ethnic groupings. Randomization group was not included, as aspirin did not reduce cognitive decline, risk of dementia,23 or other primary end points.27
To evaluate whether changes in gait speed and each cognitive variable added prognostic value beyond baseline measurements, we compared 2 baseline-only models (baseline scores and a combined indicator of lowest cognitive and gait tertile membership) to longitudinal models using likelihood ratio tests. To determine if dual decliners had greater risk of dementia than gait or cognitive decliners, we performed linear contrasts of the relevant coefficients. α = .05 for all tests and statistical analyses were performed using Stata version 16 (Stata Corp).
A total of 16 855 ASPREE participants (88.2%, of 19 114 total randomizations) had longitudinal gait and cognitive data available for analysis (Figure 1). Mean (SD) age was 75.0 (4.4) years, 9435 participants (56.0%) were women and 7558 (44.8%) reported education levels of 12 years or more (Tables 1 and 2). Across the 4 cognitive measures, 2259 participants were excluded from at least 1 model because of the absence of gait and/or cognitive follow-up data prior to dementia diagnosis, death, or withdrawal. Of these, 178 were diagnosed with dementia, 481 died, and 1600 withdrew from in-person study visits prior to collection of relevant follow up data.
Investigating multiple cognitive domains would be unnecessary if decline in 1 domain implied decline in other domains. There were varying degrees of overlap between the 4 different cognitive decliner groups (3MS, HVLT-R, SDMT, COWAT-F) and the 4 groups of dual decliners (gait-3MS, gait-HVLT-R, gait-SDMT, gait-COWAT-F) (eTables 1 and 2 in the Supplement). Overlap was not near-complete for any cognitive decline grouping, with the degree of overlap ranging from 38% (between HVLT-R and SDMT) to 59% (3MS and HVLT-R). The degree of overlap between dual-decline groupings similarly ranged from 38% (HVLT-R and SDMT) to 58% (3MS and HVLT-R).
Associations Between Dual Decline and Risk of Dementia
Gait-3MS dual decliners and gait-only and cognitive-only decliners had significantly higher dementia incidence rates than nondecliners after adjustment for demographic characteristics, baseline 3MS, and gait speed (Table 3). The hazard ratio (HR) was highest for those in the dual decline group, indicating a more than 20-fold increase in risk (HR, 22.2; 95% CI, 15.0-32.9). Upon linear contrast of HRs, dual decliners had a 5-fold greater risk of dementia compared with gait-only decliners (HR, 5.5; 95% CI, 3.8-8.1) and a 3-fold increased risk compared with cognitive-only decliners (HR, 3.1; 95% CI, 2.5-3.9).
Gait-HVLT-R-delay dual decliners and gait-only and cognitive-only decliners had a significantly higher risk of developing dementia compared with nondecliners (Table 3 and Figure 2). The highest HR was in the dual decline group (HR, 24.9; 95% CI, 16.5-37.6). Upon linear contrast review, dual decliners also had a greater risk of dementia compared with gait-only decliners (HR, 6.4; 95% CI, 4.2-9.8) and cognitive-only decliners (HR, 3.3; 95% CI, 2.6-4.1).
Gait-SDMT dual decliners and gait-only decliners had a significantly higher risk of developing dementia compared with nondecliners (Table 3 and Figure 2). The highest hazard ratio (HR) was in those in the dual decline group (HR, 4.3; 95% CI, 3.2-5.8). On linear contrast, dual decliners had a greater risk of dementia compared with cognitive decliners (HR, 3.3; 95% CI, 2.4-4.4), but not gait-only decliners (HR, 1.2; 95% CI, 0.9-1.6).
Gait-COWAT-F dual decliners and gait-only decliners had a significantly higher risk of developing dementia compared with nondecliners (Table 3 and Figure 2). The highest HR was for those in the dual decline group (HR, 4.7; 95% CI, 3.5-6.3). On linear contrast, dual decliners had a greater risk of dementia compared with the cognitive decliners (HR, 3.8; 95% CI, 2.8-5.2), but not gait-only decliners (HR, 1.1; 95% CI, 0.9-1.5).
In Cox models (adjusted for age, education, gender, and country), higher baseline gait speed and cognitive test scores were both associated with lower dementia risk for all 4 models (eTable 4 in the Supplement). Compared with baseline-only models, models including baseline and longitudinal measures demonstrated a statistically significant improvement in fit on likelihood ratio tests (likelihood ratio χ2: 3MS, 337.73; P < .001; SDMT, 136.88; P < .001; HVLT-R-delay, 358.25; P < .001; COWAT, 152.31; P < .001) (Table 3).
Combined Cognition and Gait Measure
A binary indicator denoting simultaneous membership of the lowest cognitive and gait baseline tertiles was associated with higher dementia risk in all models (eTable 4 in the Supplement). Compared with these combined baseline indicator models, models including baseline and longitudinal measures demonstrated a statistically significant improvement in fit on likelihood ratio tests (likelihood ratio χ2: 3MS, 431.85; P < .001; SDMT, 131.95; P < .001; HVLT-R-delay, 457.30; P < .001; COWAT, 124.92; P < .001.).
This study examined associations between dual decline in gait speed and 4 different cognitive measures with incident diagnosis of dementia. The main findings from this study of over 16 000 older people were: (1) dual decline in gait speed and each of the cognitive measures was associated with higher risk of dementia when compared with nondecliners, cognitive only decliners, or gait only decliners (except for SDMT and COWAT-F measurements); (2) for dual decliners, risk of dementia was highest in the gait-HVLT-R-delay group, followed by the gait-3MS, gait-COWAT-F, and gait-SDMT groups; and (3) models that include these longitudinal decline groupings demonstrated superior goodness-of-fit to observed outcomes compared with models including baseline gait and cognitive scores only. These results highlight the importance of gait in dementia risk assessment and suggest that dual decline in gait speed and a memory measure may be the best combination associated with accurate assessment of future dementia risk.
Our findings expand on prior studies by testing 4 different cognitive measures in a single, large, well-characterized cohort. Importantly, we answer the question as to whether dual decline in gait speed and a test of processing speed or verbal fluency exhibits a similar association with progression to dementia as decline in memory (delayed recall) or global cognition. Descriptively, we found that the risk of dementia was highest in the memory dual decliners when compared with the global, verbal fluency, and processing speed dual decliners, which confirmed our hypothesis.
Prior studies have found slower gait speed in non-Alzheimer type dementia when compared with Alzheimer disease28; and others have demonstrated that gait speed is more strongly correlated with executive function and processing speed.15 Therefore, it is possible that gait measures capture decline in nonamnestic domains, which are required (in addition to memory decline) for dementia diagnosis using DSM-IV criteria applied in this study. Association between nonamnestic domains, such as processing speed and verbal fluency, with gait have been explained by the crossover in the underlying networks or pathology.15,29-32 In a prior study,7 gray matter covariance patterns consisting of the brain stem, precuneus, fusiform, motor, supplementary motor, and prefrontal cortex were associated with gait speed, processing speed, and executive function, but not memory. Furthermore, associations have been reported between cerebrovascular markers (white matter hyperintensities, microbleeds, and subcortical infarcts) and both poorer processing speed29,30 and slow gait speed.32,33 While global or overall memory performance shares overlapping networks with motor functions including gait, episodic memory also has distinct underlying networks reliant on normal hippocampal function.34 Poorer performance in tests of episodic memory is also more strongly linked to AD pathology, such as β-amyloid and tau accumulation.35 As the majority of cases of dementia are thought to be due to mixed pathology,36 the addition of gait speed to memory decline would necessarily capture a broader range of distributed brain networks.37
Our findings for global cognition are in agreement with a study of 135 older people with cognitive impairment (mean [SD] baseline Montreal Cognitive Assessment, 24.8 [3.5]), where dual decline in gait and cognition was associated with increased risk of dementia over 2 years compared with nondecliners.13 We expand on these findings with a larger cohort of healthy older people (mean [SD] baseline 3MS, 93.7 [4.5]). Our findings agree with those of Tian et al,14 who in a meta-analysis of 6 studies (8699 participants) found that dual decline in gait and a test of immediate verbal memory was associated with elevated dementia risk compared with usual agers. This meta-analysis harmonized several cognitive tests, criteria for dementia, and gait assessments over differing distances. Our study utilized consistent criteria and standardized tests for all participants. Importantly, we were able to test and confirm the hypothesis raised by Tian et al that memory may better differentiate phenotypic groupings, as decline in processing speed and verbal fluency are more strongly associated with decline in gait speed.15,38
The benefit of measuring both cognition and gait speed in studies of dementia risk has been previously established. A multicountry study found having MCR (a subjective cognitive complaint and poor gait speed) was associated with increased risk of dementia more than each of its components.12 We build on these findings in 2 ways. First, we showed that dual, longitudinal decline in cognition and gait improved modeling of incident dementia above baseline cognition and gait measures. Second, we demonstrated in a large sample that dual decline was associated with significantly higher risk of dementia than either gait-only or cognitive decline phenotypes (except when measured with SDMT and COWAT-F), suggesting that the combined measure has prognostic value. Measurement of gait speed has long been recommended in clinical practice as a marker of overall health and adverse outcomes such as falls, disability, hospitalization, and mortality.39-41 Our findings suggest that serial measurement of gait along with a simple test of memory would be more sensitive to future dementia risk than either measure alone. Such a test appears feasible in primary health clinics, although this requires confirmation in future implementation studies.
This study had several limitations. We reported secondary analysis of trial data; however, aspirin was not found to be associated with cognitive impairment or dementia23 and therefore was unlikely to have influenced our findings. Misclassification bias due to randomization of participants with dementia diagnosis cannot be fully excluded, but was unlikely because participants’ usual health care providers were closely involved during recruitment.17 Gait and cognition were not measured at the same time points, but our linear mixed-model approach accounted for this to some extent. However, a drawback of the mixed-model approach is that measurement error in longitudinal data and the correlation between random slopes and time to dementia diagnosis were unaccounted for in survival modeling.42
Censoring longitudinal data for mixed models at diagnosis date may have biased random slope estimates toward zero (ie, a less-steep decline) for those diagnosed with dementia. However, this would produce bias toward the null in survival models, and survival time and time-at-risk was not affected in our results.
The ASPREE sample is healthier than the general elderly population, and results may not generalize to less-healthy groups. As gait speed follow-up commenced 2 years after randomization, we could include only individuals who survived to year 2 and remained in the trial without dementia diagnosis. This means results were drawn from a healthier subgroup within ASPREE, potentially biasing baseline models, which did not include gait speed and cognitive test data for excluded participants.
Subtyping of dementia end points would have allowed exploration of the utility of dual decline in predicting dementia types but was unavailable. Dementia end point adjudications in ASPREE utilized the so-called “memory-plus” DSM-IV criteria, which may explain the stronger effect sizes observed for the associations with memory decline. Finally, it is to be expected that longitudinal decline in cognitive performance is strongly associated with dementia, as the former is a diagnostic criterion for the latter. By presenting specific comparisons between dual decline and cognitive decline groupings in this study, we have been able to specifically illustrate the additional benefit of a combined gait-cognition measure beyond cognitive testing alone.
Dual decline in gait speed and cognition was associated with an increased risk of dementia in this study, with dual memory decliners showing greatest risk. Our findings provide further evidence for the importance of adding serial gait speed measures to dementia risk screening assessments, providing the opportunity for further comprehensive assessment and early preventative treatments.
Accepted for Publication: March 18, 2022.
Published: May 31, 2022. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2022.14647
Correction: This article was corrected on June 23, 2022, to correct results in the Abstract.
Open Access: This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the CC-BY License. © 2022 Collyer TA et al. JAMA Network Open.
Corresponding Author: Taya A. Collyer, PhD, Monash University, Peninsula Clinical School, Central Clinical School, 2 Hastings Rd, Frankston, Victoria, Australia (taya.collyer@monash.edu).
Author Contributions: Dr Collyer had full access to all the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.
Concept and design: Collyer, Chong, Orchard, Brodtmann, Srikanth, Callisaya.
Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: Collyer, Murray, Woods, Storey, Chong, Ryan, Orchard, Brodtmann, Shah, Callisaya.
Drafting of the manuscript: Collyer, Storey, Chong, Callisaya.
Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Collyer, Murray, Woods, Chong, Ryan, Orchard, Brodtmann, Srikanth, Shah.
Statistical analysis: Collyer.
Obtained funding: Murray, Woods, Shah.
Administrative, technical, or material support: Woods, Chong, Ryan, Orchard, Shah.
Supervision: Murray, Woods, Storey, Brodtmann, Srikanth, Callisaya.
Conflict of Interest Disclosures: Dr Woods reported receiving grants from the National Institutes of Health during the conduct of the study. Dr Orchard reported receiving grants from the Victorian Cancer Agency, Australia during the conduct of the study. Dr Brodtmann reported receiving consultant fees from Biogen and Roche, and grants from the National Health and Medical Research Council outside the submitted work; she also reported service on editorial boards for Neurology and International Journal of Stroke and the scientific advisory board for Biogen Australia Dementia. Dr Shah reported grants from National Institutes of Health during the conduct of the study; he reported grant funding for work as clinical trial subinvestigator from Amylyx Pharmaceuticals, Inc, Athira Pharma, Inc, Eli Lilly & Co Inc, Genentech, Inc, Novartis Pharmaceuticals, Inc, and Roche Holdings AG paid to his institution outside the submitted work; he reported service as a noncompensated Board of Directors member for the Alzheimer's Association, Illinois Chapter, until June 30, 2021. No other disclosures were reported.
Funding/Support: Data collection and management for the ASPREE (Aspirin in Reducing Events in the Elderly) trial was funded by the National Institute on Aging (US) and National Health and Medical Research Council (Australia). AG Bayer provided aspirin and the matching placebo for ASPREE.
Role of the Funder/Sponsor: The funders had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.
Additional Contributions: The authors acknowledge the dedicated and skilled staff in Australia and the US for conduct of the trial. The authors also grateful to the ASPREE participants, who willingly volunteered for this study, and the general practitioners and medical clinics who support participants in the ASPREE study.
8.Jayakody
O, Breslin
M, Beare
R, Srikanth
VK, Blumen
HM, Callisaya
ML. The associations between grey matter volume covariance patterns and gait variability—the Tasmanian Study of Cognition and Gait.
Brain Topogr. 2021;34(4):478-488. doi:
10.1007/s10548-021-00841-5PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 9.Rosano
C, Studenski
SA, Aizenstein
HJ, Boudreau
RM, Longstreth
WT
Jr, Newman
AB. Slower gait, slower information processing and smaller prefrontal area in older adults.
Age Ageing. 2012;41(1):58-64. doi:
10.1093/ageing/afr113PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 13.Montero-Odasso
M, Speechley
M, Muir-Hunter
SW,
et al; Canadian Gait and Cognition Network. Motor and cognitive trajectories before dementia: results from Gait and Brain Study.
J Am Geriatr Soc. 2018;66(9):1676-1683. doi:
10.1111/jgs.15341PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 16.McNeil
JJ, Woods
RL, Nelson
MR,
et al; ASPREE Investigator Group. Baseline characteristics of participants in the ASPREE (ASPirin in Reducing Events in the Elderly) Study.
J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2017;72(11):1586-1593. doi:
10.1093/gerona/glw342PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 17.Lockery
JE, Collyer
TA, Abhayaratna
WP,
et al. Recruiting general practice patients for large clinical trials: lessons from the Aspirin in Reducing Events in the Elderly (ASPREE) study.
Med J Aust. 2019;210(4):168-173. doi:
10.5694/mja2.12060PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 19.Teng
EL, Chui
HC. The Modified Mini-Mental State (3MS) examination.
J Clin Psychiatry. 1987;48(8):314-318.
PubMedGoogle Scholar 21.Smith
A. Symbol Digit Modalities Test. Western Psychological Services; 1973.
22.Spreen
O, Strauss
E. A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests. Administration, Norms, and Commentary. 2nd ed. Oxford University Press; 1998.
38.Callisaya
ML, Blizzard
CL, Wood
AG, Thrift
AG, Wardill
T, Srikanth
VK. Longitudinal relationships between cognitive decline and gait slowing: the Tasmanian Study of Cognition and Gait.
J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2015;70(10):1226-1232. doi:
10.1093/gerona/glv066PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref