[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Figure 1.  Study Flow Diagram Illustrating Participant Device Data Collection, Missingness, and Apps Selected for Coding
Study Flow Diagram Illustrating Participant Device Data Collection, Missingness, and Apps Selected for Coding
Figure 2.  Total Manipulative Design Scores Across Different App Categories Played by Preschool-Aged Children With Their Own Mobile Devices
Total Manipulative Design Scores Across Different App Categories Played by Preschool-Aged Children With Their Own Mobile Devices

Medians (with IQRs represented in error bars) are shown for app categories with 9 or more apps; individual data points are shown for smaller categories. VR indicates virtual reality; YT, YouTube.

Table 1.  Sociodemographic Characteristics of Preschool-Aged Children With Their Own Mobile Device Participating in the Preschooler Tablet Study
Sociodemographic Characteristics of Preschool-Aged Children With Their Own Mobile Device Participating in the Preschooler Tablet Study
Table 2.  Manipulative Design Features to Prolong Gameplay or Encourage Purchases in Apps Played by Preschool-Aged Children
Manipulative Design Features to Prolong Gameplay or Encourage Purchases in Apps Played by Preschool-Aged Children
Table 3.  Manipulative Design Features to Encourage Interaction With Advertisements in Apps Played by Preschool-Aged Children
Manipulative Design Features to Encourage Interaction With Advertisements in Apps Played by Preschool-Aged Children
1.
Zagal  JP, Björk  S, Lewis  C. Dark patterns in the design of games.  Foundations of Digital Games. Published online 2013. Accessed January 5, 2022. https://www.semanticscholar.org/paper/Dark-patterns-in-the-design-of-games-Zagal-Bj%C3%B6rk/19a241378b06d868eb5f6b76027172c3aaca86f4
2.
Gray  CM, Kou  Y, Battles  B, Hoggatt  J, Toombs  AL. The dark (patterns) side of UX design. CHI 2018 conference paper. 2018:1-14. Accessed January 5, 2022. https://dl.acm.org/doi/pdf/10.1145/3173574.3174108
3.
Mathur  A, Acar  G, Friedman  MJ,  et al.  Dark patterns at scale: findings from a crawl of 11K shopping websites.   Proceedings of the ACM on Human-Computer Interaction. 2019;3(CSCW):1-32. doi:10.1145/3359183Google ScholarCrossref
4.
Narayanan  A, Mathur  A, Chetty  M, Kshirsagar  M.  Dark patterns: past, present, and future: the evolution of tricky user interfaces.   Queue. 2020;18(2):67-92. doi:10.1145/3400899.3400901Google ScholarCrossref
5.
Bösch  C, Erb  B, Kargl  F, Kopp  H, Pfattheicher  S.  Tales from the dark side: privacy dark strategies and privacy dark patterns.   Proc Priv Enhancing Technol. 2016;2016(4):237-254. doi:10.1515/popets-2016-0038Google ScholarCrossref
6.
Meyer  M, Adkins  V, Yuan  N, Weeks  HM, Chang  Y-J, Radesky  J.  Advertising in young children’s apps: a content analysis.   J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2019;40(1):32-39. doi:10.1097/DBP.0000000000000622PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Zhao  F, Egelman  S, Weeks  HM, Kaciroti  N, Miller  AL, Radesky  JS.  Data collection practices of mobile applications played by preschool-aged children.   JAMA Pediatr. 2020;174(12):e203345-e203345. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2020.3345PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Fitton  D, Read  JC.  Creating a framework to support the critical consideration of dark design aspects in free-to-play apps.  Proceedings of the 18th ACM International Conference on Interaction Design and Children. Published online June 2019:407-418. doi:10.1145/3311927.3323136
9.
Meyer  M, Zosh  JM, McLaren  C,  et al.  How educational are ‘educational’ apps for young children? app store content analysis using the Four Pillars of Learning framework.   J Child Media. 2021;15(4):526-548. doi:10.1080/17482798.2021.1882516PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Rideout  V.  The Common Sense Census: Media Use by Kids Age Zero to Eight. Common Sense Media; 2020.
11.
Ramsetty  A, Adams  C.  Impact of the digital divide in the age of COVID-19.   J Am Med Inform Assoc. 2020;27(7):1147-1148. doi:10.1093/jamia/ocaa078PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Radesky  JS, Weeks  HM, Ball  R,  et al.  Young children’s use of smartphones and tablets.   Pediatrics. 2020;146(1):e20193518. doi:10.1542/peds.2019-3518PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Harris  PA, Taylor  R, Minor  BL,  et al; REDCap Consortium.  The REDCap consortium: building an international community of software platform partners.   J Biomed Inform. 2019;95:103208. doi:10.1016/j.jbi.2019.103208PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
Harris  PA, Taylor  R, Thielke  R, Payne  J, Gonzalez  N, Conde  JG.  Research electronic data capture (REDCap)—a metadata-driven methodology and workflow process for providing translational research informatics support.   J Biomed Inform. 2009;42(2):377-381. doi:10.1016/j.jbi.2008.08.010PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Mathur  A, Kshirsagar  M, Mayer  J. What makes a dark pattern...dark? design attributes, normative considerations, and measurement methods.  arXiv. Preprint posted online January 13, 2021. doi:10.1145/3411764.3445610
16.
Schüll  ND.  Addiction by Design. Princeton University Press; 2012. doi:10.1515/9781400834655
17.
Eyal  N.  Hooked: How to Build Habit-Forming Products. Penguin; 2014.
18.
Hoffner  C.  Children’s wishful identification and parasocial interaction with favorite television characters.   J Broadcast Electron Media. 1996;40(3):389-402. doi:10.1080/08838159609364360Google ScholarCrossref
19.
Kocher  MG, Sutter  M.  Time is money—time pressure, incentives, and the quality of decision-making.   J Econ Behav Organ. 2006;61(3):375-392. doi:10.1016/j.jebo.2004.11.013Google ScholarCrossref
20.
Elliott  AC, Hynan  LSA.  A SAS macro implementation of a multiple comparison post hoc test for a Kruskal-Wallis analysis.   Comput Methods Programs Biomed. 2011;102(1):75-80. doi:10.1016/j.cmpb.2010.11.002PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
Radesky  J, Hiniker  A.  From moral panic to systemic change: making child-centered design the default.   Int J Child Comput Interact. 2022;31:100351. doi:10.1016/j.ijcci.2021.100351PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
Yoon  G, Li  C, Ji  Y, North  M, Hong  C, Liu  J.  Attracting comments: digital engagement metrics on Facebook and financial performance.   J Advert. 2018;47(1):24-37. doi:10.1080/00913367.2017.1405753Google ScholarCrossref
23.
Bernier  A, Carlson  SM, Whipple  N.  From external regulation to self-regulation: early parenting precursors of young children’s executive functioning.   Child Dev. 2010;81(1):326-339. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01397.xPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Carlson  SM, Mandell  DJ, Williams  L.  Executive function and theory of mind: stability and prediction from ages 2 to 3.   Dev Psychol. 2004;40(6):1105-1122. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.40.6.1105PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
Radesky  J, Chassiakos  YLR, Ameenuddin  N, Navsaria  D; Council on Communication and Media.  Digital advertising to children.   Pediatrics. 2020;146(1):e20201681. doi:10.1542/peds.2020-1681PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
Federal Trade Commission. Apple Inc will provide full consumer refunds of at least $32.5 million to settle FTC complaint it charged for kids’ in-app purchases without parental consent. Federal Trade Commission press release. January 15, 2014. Accessed January 5, 2022. https://www.ftc.gov/news-events/press-releases/2014/01/apple-inc-will-provide-full-consumer-refunds-least-325-million
27.
Sun  K, Sugatan  C, Afnan  T,  et al. “They see you’re a girl if you pick a pink robot with a skirt”: a qualitative study of how children conceptualize data processing and digital privacy risks. Proceedings of the 2021 CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems. May 2021:1-34. doi:10.1145/3411764.3445333
Original Investigation
Pediatrics
June 17, 2022

Prevalence and Characteristics of Manipulative Design in Mobile Applications Used by Children

Author Affiliations
  • 1Department of Pediatrics, University of Michigan Medical School, Ann Arbor
  • 2University of Washington Information School, Seattle
  • 3University of Michigan School of Public Health, Ann Arbor
  • 4Department of Communication and Media Studies, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor
  • 5Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor
JAMA Netw Open. 2022;5(6):e2217641. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2022.17641
Key Points

Question  What types of manipulative design features exist in childrens’ mobile apps, and do inequities exist in young childrens exposure to manipulative design?

Findings  In this cross-sectional study of apps used by 160 children aged 3 to 5 years, the majority of apps were associated with manipulative design features that included parasocial relationship pressure, fabricated time pressure, navigation constraints, and use of attractive lures to encourage longer gameplay or more purchases, in addition to advertisement-based pressure; only 20% of apps had no manipulative design features. Children from lower socioeconomic strata played apps with more manipulative design.

Meaning  These results suggest that interactive designs that serve the interests of technology companies over the interests of children are common and deserve further study and regulation.

Abstract

Importance  Manipulative design features (known as dark patterns) are common in video games and adult-directed technologies, but their prevalence in children’s interactive media has not been described.

Objectives  To develop a reliable coding scheme for gathering data on manipulative digital designs, describe their prevalence within apps used by a community-based sample of young children, and test hypotheses about associations of manipulative design features with socioeconomic status (SES).

Design, Setting, and Participants  This cross-sectional study of a convenience sample of parents of children aged 3 to 5 years was conducted online. Eligible parents were legal guardians of a 3-to-5-year-old child, lived with their child at least 5 days per week, understood English, and were part of a family that owned at least 1 Android or iOS tablet or smartphone. For each participant, the 3 apps used for the longest duration by children with their own mobile devices were downloaded, played, and coded. Data were analyzed between April and August 2021.

Exposures  Child SES, operationalized as parent educational attainment and household income-to-needs ratio.

Main Outcomes and Measures  Researchers assigned each child a prevalence score for manipulative design features (overall, gameplay pressure, purchase pressure, and advertisement viewing pressure) within the apps children played.

Results  Of 160 children in the sample, mean (SD) age was 4.0 (0.6) years; 120 children (75.0%) were non-Hispanic White, and 96 (60.0%) had a parent with a college degree or more. Manipulative designs promoted prolonged gameplay or purchases through 4 user experience typologies: parasocial relationship pressure occurred in 33 (24.8%) and 25 (18.8%) apps with characters; time pressure in 23 (17.3%) and 14 (10.5%) apps; navigation constraints in 61 (45.9%) and 49 (36.8%) apps; and attractive lures in 60 (45.1%) and 61 (45.9%) apps, respectively. Children from households whose parents had lower education levels had higher manipulative design prevalence scores than children whose parents had graduated from college (median [IQR] 3.7 [2.5-5.0] vs 3.0 [2.0-4.0]; P = .02), gameplay-prolonging design (2.3 [1.6-3.0] vs 2.0 [1.5-2.8]; P = .047), and purchase pressure (1.0 [0.5-1.5] vs 0.6 [0-1.3]; P = .02). Purchase pressure prevalence scores were higher for children from households with lower income (R = −0.18; P = .02).

Conclusions and Relevance  Design features that encourage monetization of children’s digital experiences were common in this sample and disproportionately occurred in apps used by children with lower SES.

Introduction

Young children are avid users of touchscreen devices, which allow contingent interactivity between users and applications (commonly referred to as apps). Despite its many benefits, interactivity has also enabled a host of what design experts call “dark patterns,” interactive designs that benefit the interests of the technology designer (eg, company, website, video game) at the expense of the user.1,2 Dark patterns were first described within video games; examples included features that demanded players disrupt their daily activities to return to the game or tedious tasks that could only be avoided by making purchases (termed “grinding”).1 Dark patterns are pervasive and have since been identified in many contexts outside of gaming. For example, they are used to exert purchasing pressure on users of e-commerce websites,3 encourage excessive labor from workers on gig economy platforms,4 and exploit people’s social belonging to increase information disclosure.5

A small body of prior work suggests dark patterns are present in interactive apps for children as well. For example, childrens’ apps often monetize gameplay by showing advertisements, encouraging in-app purchases,6 or collecting data.7 To our knowledge, only one study has attempted to characterize the dark patterns children encounter. Fitton and Read8 developed a framework validated through interviews with teenage girls, which included temporal patterns like grinding, monetary patterns like paying for enhancements, and sneaky advertisements that pop up unprompted. However, that analysis did not examine the prevalence of these patterns in childrens’ digital products.

To address these gaps in understanding about age-inappropriate digital design, we leveraged data from a cohort study of young children whose mobile device use was objectively measured. We focused on young children because of their less-developed understanding of persuasive intent (ie, in advertising),9 which may render them more susceptible to manipulative designs. This study aimed to (1) develop a reproducible framework for characterizing dark patterns in children’s apps (we refer to these as “manipulative design features” to avoid associating the racialized term “dark” with unethical behavior); (2) describe the frequency of manipulative design in a sample of apps used by young children; (3) examine differences in manipulative design prevalence by app characteristics (ie, by category and free vs paid); and (4) test the hypothesis that children from lower socioeconomic status households, who experience access barriers to high-quality media,10,11 would be exposed to apps with more manipulative design features.

Methods
Population and Study Design

We analyzed data from the Preschooler Tablet Study, a longitudinal single-center study assessing child development and mobile device use in 3-to-5-year-old children. Data were collected in 3 waves: baseline (phase 1), 3 months (phase 2), and 6 months (phase 3); parents received up to $150 for completion of all phases. The study was approved by the University of Michigan institutional review board, and written informed consent was obtained from parents or legal guardians.

The study population and recruitment methods have been described in detail previously.12 This report follows the Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) reporting guideline for observational studies and a study flow diagram shows sample sizes at each phase (Figure 1). A community-based convenience sample of English-speaking parents of 3-to-5-year-old children was recruited; parents or legal guardians were eligible if they lived with the child at least 5 days per week and owned an Android or iOS tablet or smartphone. After providing online consent for themselves and their child, parents were emailed instructions for mobile device sampling and REDCap13,14 surveys in which they reported sociodemographic characteristics, including household income and size (from which income-to-needs ratio [ITN] was calculated), and the participating parent’s highest educational attainment. ITN was calculated by dividing annual household income by the US Department of Health and Human Services poverty guideline for the corresponding year and household size. An ITN of 1.0 represents a family living at the federal poverty level; 2.0 represents 200% of the poverty level. In our sample, ITN ranged from 0.15 to 7.39. Parents reported their child’s race and ethnicity; response options were collapsed into non-Hispanic White and minoritized racial and ethnic groups (including Asian or Pacific Islander, Black and African American, Hispanic [any race], multiple races [non-Hispanic], and Native American or Alaska Native) because of small cell sizes for some categories (ie, to increase statistical power) and because of prior research showing differences in media use by child race and ethnicity (Table 1).10

Mobile Device Sampling

Mobile device sampling occurred at each phase. This method uses data collected by mobile devices to describe childrens’ app usage (for Android devices, the Chronicle app [Methodic] provides timestamped app usage output; for Apple devices, parents collected screenshots of app usage from the phone’s Settings usage archive).12 From Chronicle and screenshot output, we generated a list of apps each participant used and the average daily duration of use for each app at all 3 phases. Mobile device data was analyzed using data.table and tidyverse in R versions 3.5.2 through 4.1.1 (R Project for Statistical Computing).

App Sample for Coding

To create a feasible sample of apps to code, we limited our sample to children who had their own mobile device (165 children), as childrens’ app usage duration could not be calculated on shared devices (ie, parents or siblings might also use high-duration apps such as YouTube, thus skewing daily estimates) (Figure 1). Then, we identified the 3 top-duration apps used by children with their own devices, presuming that manipulative design features would be easier to identify in apps that maintain childrens’ attention for longer. We selected top-duration apps from phase 1 of the study when possible and selected additional top-duration apps from phases 2 or 3 as needed (eAppendix 1 in the Supplement). Children with no available or codable apps were excluded, yielding a sample of 160 children and 133 unique apps; 86 children (53.8%) had 3 codable apps, 46 (28.8%) had 2, and 28 (17.5%) had 1.

Coding Scheme Development

To develop a manipulative design coding scheme, we used an approach that combined user experience (UX) design concepts,15 gambling,16 and persuasive design frameworks.17 Authors and coders played apps and noted patterns of monetization, behavior of parasocial characters (given their influence on child behavior18), reinforcement techniques, and navigability and/or presence of stoppage cues. We refined the coding scheme iteratively through review of codes and app designs with coauthors (A.H., S.C., A.G.).

The resulting framework (Tables 2 and 3) described UX typologies (parasocial pressure, time pressure, navigation constraints, and lures) that appeared designed to achieve monetization goals (ie, prolong gameplay, influence purchase decisions), as well as 3 designs intended to increase advertisement viewing. We defined UX features as manipulative if they used age-inappropriate interaction or reduced user autonomy in service of the monetization goals (eAppendix 2 in the Supplement). Two adult coders trained to reliability (κ = 0.78-0.90) downloaded and played each app for 10 to 15 minutes, completing several levels and visiting every available page (eg, settings, store, level map) to assign apps a binary score for each of 11 possible codes (1 if features present, 0 if not). Coding uncertainties were resolved by consensus (eAppendix 1 in the Supplement).

Statistical Analysis

Across coded apps, we calculated the frequency of each manipulative design feature and created 4 summary scores: (1) gameplay-prolonging features (score range, 0-4); (2) purchase pressure (0-4); (3) advertising pressure (0-3); and (4) sum of all manipulative design features (0-11). For each child, we calculated manipulative design prevalence scores by averaging each summary score across their top apps. Because manipulative design prevalence scores were positively skewed, we conducted Wilcoxon rank-sum tests or Spearman correlations to compare summary scores in free vs paid apps and between app categories (described in prior work12), as well as to examine associations between manipulative design prevalence scores and childrens’ socioeconomic characteristics (ITN and parent education). App-level and child-level univariate and bivariate statistics were conducted in SAS version 9.4 (SAS Institute, Inc). Statistical significance was set at P < .05 and tests were 2-tailed.

Results

Of 160 children in the sample, mean (SD) age was 4.0 (0.6) years; 120 children (75.0%) were non-Hispanic White, and 96 (60.0%) had a parent with a college degree or more (Table 1). The eTable in the Supplement shows the names of apps, categories, and manipulative design codes.

Manipulative UX Typologies
Parasocial Relationship Pressure: App Characters or Influencers Behave in Ways That Pressure Users to Prolong Gameplay or Make Purchases

In total, 33 apps (24.8% overall, 29.0% of apps with characters) used parasocial characters to prolong gameplay, either by pressuring the user to keep playing or by expressing disapproval if they stopped. For example, in My Talking Tom 2, Talking Tom made statements like, “Do you want to give up?” when the player opted not to move to the next level or “You’re making me want to go to sleep” when the player was idle. Pressure could occur during gameplay (eg, urging player to save friends from a violent farmer in Green Grandpa Alien), as a notification to return to the game (eg, Blockcraft 3D notifies players that “people are protesting your absence!”), or an enticement to return at a later time (eg, a DragonML character saying, “Come back tomorrow to get THIS dragon! Then visit Dragolandia each day for other valuable rewards!”).

Separately, 25 apps (18.8% overall, 21.9% of apps with characters) used parasocial relationships to promote purchases. This pressure sometimes came from a narrator (eg, the narrator of ABC Animals says, “You can play with these cute animals for a tiny fee! Ask your parents!”) or a character (eg, the buddy character in Kick the Buddy says things like, “Don’t just stand there, buy something!” when the player is on the store page). Video characters on YouTube Kids also encouraged purchases of their branded products. Todo Math shows a character with a large tear in their eye as the child is prompted to sign up for a free trial of the paid version.

Time-based Pressure: Visual Indicators Conveying Scarcity of Time

Apps displayed countdown clocks and other visual indications of time running out, which is known to interfere with decision-making.19 Time pressure was used to prolong gameplay in 23 apps (17.3%) and to promote purchases in 14 apps (10.5%), usually at decision points between levels (eg, Miraculous stating, “Save me!” with 4 seconds remaining). Time-based pressure to encourage purchases typically appeared as messaging indicating artificial scarcity (eg, “Limited time only!”) or a countdown indicator that a discount or item would disappear if the user did not act (such as the “Flash deals” advertised in Subway Surfer).

Navigation Constraints: Obstacles Blocking the User’s Ability to Maneuver Within the Interface

Features like tunneling (providing no options for where to go next), pop-ups, or auto-advancing were used to prolong gameplay in 61 apps (45.9%) and to promote purchases in 49 apps (36.8%). This included making it difficult to reverse or opt out of a level back to the homepage or auto-advancing without providing a clear pause or disengagement options between levels (eg, Race Master only provides an option for players to proceed to the next level after finishing a level). In other apps, like Robocar Poli Habit—Kids Game Package, the player is not provided with an “X” button but must instead click the “Settings” button, which eventually navigates to the home page.

Navigation constraints to promote purchases appeared in several forms. The most common was asymmetric design that highlighted purchase options and made opt-outs hard to see (eg, lighter “X” and confusingly worded “Restore purchase” buttons in Kick the Buddy). Other apps made the in-app store the landing page (eg, Miraculous) or had store pages that featured only paid items first, making it cumbersome to find free items. Navigation constraints that promoted purchases also included touchscreen hit pads that promoted purchases (eg, Sonic Classic app had an inaccurately wide “REMOVE ADS” banner) or prompts to purchase items between each level (eg, Super Jabber Jump 2).

Lures: Bringing Attention to an Attractive Object—Including Stickers, Trophies, and Leaderboards—Just as the Player is Trying to Make a Decision

Lures were presented at decision points to prolong gameplay in 60 apps (45.1%) and encourage purchases in 61 apps (45.9%). Lures were coded when they enticed players to engage repeatedly with the app (eg, daily rewards in Green Grandpa Alien) or offered as achievements users could earn for repeated play (eg, DisneyNow displays types of virtual items players can earn for gameplay targets), sometimes repetitive in nature (eg, Scribblenauts Remix offers a gold crown for a repetitive gameplay). Lure purchase pressure usually involved sparkles, pulsing buttons, exclamation points, brightly colored banners that obscure other items, or other attention-grabbing cues on pages where money would be spent (eg, Kick The Buddy store page uses visual lures to encourage higher purchase levels).

Advertising-related manipulative designs took 3 forms. First, roadblock advertisements (referred to as ads) stayed up for over 20 seconds and prompted the player to interact before allowing the ad to be closed (eg, the ad for Mr. Bullet prompted the user to swipe and thereby make Santa shoot people). At times, the “X” to close out the ad was replaced with an “>>” icon, which when clicked would take the user to the app store. These appeared in 29 apps (21.8%). Second, in strategically timed ads (24 apps [18.1%]), ads would pop up when the player chose not to make a purchase or tried to navigate back to the home screen. Third were ads with reinforcement (22 apps [16.5%]), in which in-game rewards were given in exchange for ad viewing, usually providing free lives, new gameplay items, or updated features.

App-Level Analyses

Apps contained a mean (SD) 3.0 (2.7) manipulative design features (median [IQR], 2 [1-5]; range, 0-10); 26 apps (19.6%) contained none. The most frequent monetization goal was prolonging gameplay, while lures and navigation constraints were the most common UX typologies (Tables 2 and 3). When comparing the 109 free apps analyzed with the 24 paid ones, free apps showed more total manipulative design features (median [IQR] 3.0 [1.0-5.0] vs 1.0 [0-2.0]; P < .001), in addition to gameplay (1.0 [0-2.0] vs 0.5 [0-1.5], P = .04), purchase (1.0 [0-2.0] vs 0 [0-0.5]; P < .001), and advertising (0 [0-1.0] vs 0 [0]; P < .001) features.

Total manipulative design scores differed significantly between app categories (considering only categories with more than 2 apps, P < .001). The highest total manipulative design scores were present in general audience apps, significantly higher than e-books, PBS KIDS apps, educational apps, and early childhood games in Dunn post hoc comparisons (Figure 2).20

Child-Level Analyses

Almost all children (158 [98.8%]) had at least 1 manipulative design feature in their top-duration apps. Compared with children whose parents had graduated from college (96 children), children from lower-education households (64 children) had higher total manipulative design prevalence scores (median [IQR] 3.0 [2-4] vs 3.7 [2.5-5]; P = .02), gameplay-prolonging scores (2.0 [1.5-2.8] vs 2.3 [1.6-3]; P = .047), and purchase pressure scores (0.6 [0-1.3] vs 1.0 [0.5-1.5]; P = .02). Purchase pressure scores were higher for children in lower-income households (R = −0.18; P = .02), but other manipulative design prevalence scores did not vary with ITN (data not shown).

Discussion

In this app-content analysis nested within a community-based cohort study, we defined the concept of manipulative design features relative to childrens’ interactive experiences. Manipulative designs were common and clustered into 4 UX typologies—parasocial relationship pressure, time pressure, navigation constraints, and lures—with goals of encouraging gameplay, purchases, and advertisement viewing. These findings contribute to the literature by documenting the high frequency of manipulative designs in apps played by children.

Manipulative design features to prolong gameplay or reengage with the app were the most common design feature in this sample. Apps’ success is typically evaluated based on metrics such as duration and frequency of use,21,22 which is likely why app developers use design tricks that advance these goals. The designs we describe in this study reduced user autonomy to pause or end gameplay; applied pressure from favorite characters, to which young children may be particularly susceptible18; or provided virtual rewards that appeal to childrens’ immature impulse inhibition.23,24 Important next steps will be to investigate how child users respond to these design cues in real-time, whether and how childrens’ ability to transition away from devices changes when engagement-prolonging cues are present, and whether manipulative design leads to displacement of nondigital daily activities.

Manipulative design features to promote purchases occurred most often through navigation constraints and lures. Young children, whose attention may be more attracted to novelty, may be especially susceptible to attention-grabbing features. In addition, children may be less likely than adults to realize that time pressure is fabricated for persuasive purposes.25 The US Federal Trade Commission regulates “deceptive financial practice” in general, but more needs to be known about how children interpret interactive nudges to make purchases with real or virtual currency. For these reasons, and reports of children spending thousands of dollars on in-app purchases,26 ethical standards are needed regarding purchase-related design features in apps likely to be used by children.

Navigation constraints were another common UX typology used to tunnel players into prolonged gameplay or purchases, but have received less attention in the literature.1,2,4 Players may not be aware of how these subtle ways of obscuring navigation options shape their gameplay or decision-making—particularly child users, who need more explicit and transparent design cues to support digital critical thinking skills.27

Advertising is highly prevalent in all free digital products, but we identified several features that were more coercive than simple pop-up ads. In 21.8% of apps in this sample, we encountered ads that prompted the player to engage with interactive elements, sometimes with violent content, and with “X” hitpads that navigated the user to the app store. These are novel features compared with our prior analysis of in-app advertising practices,6 and they suggest that more regulation of mobile ad networks is needed.

We found that manipulative design features were most common in apps labeled as for “general audiences.” Interestingly, this category of apps had one of the longest daily usage durations in our prior analysis of mobile device data from this cohort,12 which raises the possibility that manipulative features are extending children’s time on these apps. In contrast, apps made by the nonprofit group PBS KIDS had zero manipulative design features (other than use of autoplay), which suggests that these apps are more likely to provide positive digital experiences.

Finally, children from low-SES families were more likely to encounter manipulative design features. Although these results are exploratory, they build upon existing evidence of inequities in child access to quality media10 by demonstrating an uneven prevalence of manipulative designs intended to monetize childrens’ digital experiences. Replication in other samples and wider age ranges are important future research directions.

Limitations

Limitations of our study included use of a convenience sample of children with their own mobile devices, whose media use practices may not reflect that of the general population. Our results with respect to socioeconomic status may therefore not be generalizable. We only coded participants’ longest-duration apps, so these results may overestimate the prevalence of manipulative design in childrens’ apps overall.

Conclusions

This study contributes to child-centered design policy discussions, such as the Age-Appropriate Design Code in the United Kingdom, Digital Services Act in the European Union, and legislation under consideration in the US. Because many digital products are designed by teams without backgrounds in child development, design decisions may be motivated more by monetization and less by how a child experiences their product. There is therefore a pressing need for government, regulatory, or industry actions to ensure that childrens’ needs are considered before digital products are released to market.

Back to top
Article Information

Accepted for Publication: April 29, 2022.

Published: June 17, 2022. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2022.17641

Open Access: This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the CC-BY License. © 2022 Radesky J et al. JAMA Network Open.

Corresponding Author: Jenny Radesky, MD, Department of Pediatrics, University of Michigan Medical School, 300 N Ingalls St, Ste 1107, Ann Arbor, MI 48109 (jradesky@med.umich.edu).

Author Contributions: Drs Radesky and Weeks had full access to all the data in the study and take responsibility for the integrity of the data and accuracy of the data analysis.

Concept and design: Radesky, Hiniker, McLaren, Akgun, Schaller, Gearhardt.

Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: Radesky, Hiniker, McLaren, Akgun, Weeks, Campbell.

Drafting of the manuscript: Radesky, McLaren.

Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Hiniker, Akgun, Schaller, Weeks, Campbell, Gearhardt.

Statistical analysis: Akgun, Weeks, Campbell.

Obtained funding: Radesky, Gearhardt.

Administrative, technical, or material support: Akgun, Schaller.

Supervision: Radesky, Schaller.

Conflict of Interest Disclosures: Dr Radesky reported receiving grant funding from Common Sense Media and consulting fees from Melissa & Doug Toys outside the submitted work and receiving compensation for service on Noggin’s scientific advisory board. The Noggin app included in this study was coded by trained coders who did not know of Dr Radesky’s role with the company and did not seek input from Dr Radesky about final codes. Noggin had no role in the design, analysis, or interpretation of this study. No other disclosures were reported.

Funding/Support: This research was funded by the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Development (NICHD grant No. 1R21HD094051) for the Preschooler Tablet Study. Dr Radesky’s Pathway to Independence Award, as well as REDCap and recruitment support, was provided through the Michigan Institute for Clinical & Health Research (CTSA grant No. UL1TR002240).

Role of the Funder/Sponsor: The funders had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

Additional Contributions: We thank Josh Golin, BA (Fairplay), Ariel Fox Johnson, JD (Common Sense Media during award period, now with Zoom), Amina Fazullah, JD (Common Sense Media), and Angela Campbell, JD (Georgetown Law School and Fairplay), for volunteering their time as a steering committee for this grant and providing feedback on coding scheme development. They received no compensation.

References:
1.
Zagal  JP, Björk  S, Lewis  C. Dark patterns in the design of games.  Foundations of Digital Games. Published online 2013. Accessed January 5, 2022. https://www.semanticscholar.org/paper/Dark-patterns-in-the-design-of-games-Zagal-Bj%C3%B6rk/19a241378b06d868eb5f6b76027172c3aaca86f4
2.
Gray  CM, Kou  Y, Battles  B, Hoggatt  J, Toombs  AL. The dark (patterns) side of UX design. CHI 2018 conference paper. 2018:1-14. Accessed January 5, 2022. https://dl.acm.org/doi/pdf/10.1145/3173574.3174108
3.
Mathur  A, Acar  G, Friedman  MJ,  et al.  Dark patterns at scale: findings from a crawl of 11K shopping websites.   Proceedings of the ACM on Human-Computer Interaction. 2019;3(CSCW):1-32. doi:10.1145/3359183Google ScholarCrossref
4.
Narayanan  A, Mathur  A, Chetty  M, Kshirsagar  M.  Dark patterns: past, present, and future: the evolution of tricky user interfaces.   Queue. 2020;18(2):67-92. doi:10.1145/3400899.3400901Google ScholarCrossref
5.
Bösch  C, Erb  B, Kargl  F, Kopp  H, Pfattheicher  S.  Tales from the dark side: privacy dark strategies and privacy dark patterns.   Proc Priv Enhancing Technol. 2016;2016(4):237-254. doi:10.1515/popets-2016-0038Google ScholarCrossref
6.
Meyer  M, Adkins  V, Yuan  N, Weeks  HM, Chang  Y-J, Radesky  J.  Advertising in young children’s apps: a content analysis.   J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2019;40(1):32-39. doi:10.1097/DBP.0000000000000622PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Zhao  F, Egelman  S, Weeks  HM, Kaciroti  N, Miller  AL, Radesky  JS.  Data collection practices of mobile applications played by preschool-aged children.   JAMA Pediatr. 2020;174(12):e203345-e203345. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2020.3345PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Fitton  D, Read  JC.  Creating a framework to support the critical consideration of dark design aspects in free-to-play apps.  Proceedings of the 18th ACM International Conference on Interaction Design and Children. Published online June 2019:407-418. doi:10.1145/3311927.3323136
9.
Meyer  M, Zosh  JM, McLaren  C,  et al.  How educational are ‘educational’ apps for young children? app store content analysis using the Four Pillars of Learning framework.   J Child Media. 2021;15(4):526-548. doi:10.1080/17482798.2021.1882516PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Rideout  V.  The Common Sense Census: Media Use by Kids Age Zero to Eight. Common Sense Media; 2020.
11.
Ramsetty  A, Adams  C.  Impact of the digital divide in the age of COVID-19.   J Am Med Inform Assoc. 2020;27(7):1147-1148. doi:10.1093/jamia/ocaa078PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Radesky  JS, Weeks  HM, Ball  R,  et al.  Young children’s use of smartphones and tablets.   Pediatrics. 2020;146(1):e20193518. doi:10.1542/peds.2019-3518PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Harris  PA, Taylor  R, Minor  BL,  et al; REDCap Consortium.  The REDCap consortium: building an international community of software platform partners.   J Biomed Inform. 2019;95:103208. doi:10.1016/j.jbi.2019.103208PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
Harris  PA, Taylor  R, Thielke  R, Payne  J, Gonzalez  N, Conde  JG.  Research electronic data capture (REDCap)—a metadata-driven methodology and workflow process for providing translational research informatics support.   J Biomed Inform. 2009;42(2):377-381. doi:10.1016/j.jbi.2008.08.010PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Mathur  A, Kshirsagar  M, Mayer  J. What makes a dark pattern...dark? design attributes, normative considerations, and measurement methods.  arXiv. Preprint posted online January 13, 2021. doi:10.1145/3411764.3445610
16.
Schüll  ND.  Addiction by Design. Princeton University Press; 2012. doi:10.1515/9781400834655
17.
Eyal  N.  Hooked: How to Build Habit-Forming Products. Penguin; 2014.
18.
Hoffner  C.  Children’s wishful identification and parasocial interaction with favorite television characters.   J Broadcast Electron Media. 1996;40(3):389-402. doi:10.1080/08838159609364360Google ScholarCrossref
19.
Kocher  MG, Sutter  M.  Time is money—time pressure, incentives, and the quality of decision-making.   J Econ Behav Organ. 2006;61(3):375-392. doi:10.1016/j.jebo.2004.11.013Google ScholarCrossref
20.
Elliott  AC, Hynan  LSA.  A SAS macro implementation of a multiple comparison post hoc test for a Kruskal-Wallis analysis.   Comput Methods Programs Biomed. 2011;102(1):75-80. doi:10.1016/j.cmpb.2010.11.002PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
Radesky  J, Hiniker  A.  From moral panic to systemic change: making child-centered design the default.   Int J Child Comput Interact. 2022;31:100351. doi:10.1016/j.ijcci.2021.100351PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
Yoon  G, Li  C, Ji  Y, North  M, Hong  C, Liu  J.  Attracting comments: digital engagement metrics on Facebook and financial performance.   J Advert. 2018;47(1):24-37. doi:10.1080/00913367.2017.1405753Google ScholarCrossref
23.
Bernier  A, Carlson  SM, Whipple  N.  From external regulation to self-regulation: early parenting precursors of young children’s executive functioning.   Child Dev. 2010;81(1):326-339. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01397.xPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Carlson  SM, Mandell  DJ, Williams  L.  Executive function and theory of mind: stability and prediction from ages 2 to 3.   Dev Psychol. 2004;40(6):1105-1122. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.40.6.1105PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
Radesky  J, Chassiakos  YLR, Ameenuddin  N, Navsaria  D; Council on Communication and Media.  Digital advertising to children.   Pediatrics. 2020;146(1):e20201681. doi:10.1542/peds.2020-1681PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
Federal Trade Commission. Apple Inc will provide full consumer refunds of at least $32.5 million to settle FTC complaint it charged for kids’ in-app purchases without parental consent. Federal Trade Commission press release. January 15, 2014. Accessed January 5, 2022. https://www.ftc.gov/news-events/press-releases/2014/01/apple-inc-will-provide-full-consumer-refunds-least-325-million
27.
Sun  K, Sugatan  C, Afnan  T,  et al. “They see you’re a girl if you pick a pink robot with a skirt”: a qualitative study of how children conceptualize data processing and digital privacy risks. Proceedings of the 2021 CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems. May 2021:1-34. doi:10.1145/3411764.3445333
×