[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Figure 1.  Association of Sleep Duration With Amyloid-β Positron Emission Tomography (PET) Standardized Uptake Value Ratio (SUVR)
Association of Sleep Duration With Amyloid-β Positron Emission Tomography (PET) Standardized Uptake Value Ratio (SUVR)

Adjusted mean (SE) within each sleep duration group (≤5, 6, 7, 8, and ≥9 hours) was calculated for a mean age (71.3 [4.7] years), mean years of education (16.6 [2.8] years), and non-Hispanic White male participants with an APOE ε33 allele genotype.

Figure 2.  Association of Sleep Duration With Cognitive Function
Association of Sleep Duration With Cognitive Function

Adjusted mean (SE) within each sleep duration group (≤5, 6, 7, 8, and ≥9 hours) was calculated for a mean age (71.3 [4.7] years), mean years of education (16.6 [2.8] years), and non-Hispanic White male participants with an APOE ε33 allele genotype. CFI indicates Cognitive Function Index (score range: 0-15, with higher scores indicating worse reported cognitive function); DSST, Digit Symbol Substitution Test (score range: 0-91, with higher scores indicating correct responses in 90 seconds); and MMSE, Mini-Mental State Examination (score range: 0-30, with higher scores indicating better cognitive performance).

Figure 3.  Association of Sleep Duration With Lifestyle Outcomes
Association of Sleep Duration With Lifestyle Outcomes

Adjusted mean (SE) within each sleep duration group (≤5, 6, 7, 8, and ≥9 hours) was calculated for a mean age (71.3 [4.7] years), mean years of education (16.6 [2.8] years), and non-Hispanic White male participants with an apolipoprotein E ε33 allele genotype. BMI indicates body mass index (calculated as weight in kilograms divided by height in meters squared); GDS, Geriatric Depression Scale (score range: 0-15, with higher scores indicating higher depressive symptoms).

Table 1.  Demographic Characteristics of Sleep Duration Groupsa
Demographic Characteristics of Sleep Duration Groupsa
Table 2.  Summary of Sleep Duration Associations With Amyloid-β Positron Emission Tomography (PET), Cognitive Performance, and Lifestyle Outcomesa
Summary of Sleep Duration Associations With Amyloid-β Positron Emission Tomography (PET), Cognitive Performance, and Lifestyle Outcomesa
1.
Mander  BA, Winer  JR, Walker  MP.  Sleep and human aging.   Neuron. 2017;94(1):19-36. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2017.02.004 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
Ohayon  MM, Carskadon  MA, Guilleminault  C, Vitiello  MV.  Meta-analysis of quantitative sleep parameters from childhood to old age in healthy individuals: developing normative sleep values across the human lifespan.   Sleep. 2004;27(7):1255-1273. doi:10.1093/sleep/27.7.1255 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
Kocevska  D, Lysen  TS, Dotinga  A,  et al.  Sleep characteristics across the lifespan in 1.1 million people from the Netherlands, United Kingdom and United States: a systematic review and meta-analysis.   Nat Hum Behav. 2021;5(1):113-122. doi:10.1038/s41562-020-00965-x PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Franzen  PL, Buysse  DJ.  Sleep disturbances and depression: risk relationships for subsequent depression and therapeutic implications.   Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2008;10(4):473-481. doi:10.31887/DCNS.2008.10.4/plfranzen PubMedGoogle Scholar
5.
Potvin  O, Lorrain  D, Belleville  G, Grenier  S, Préville  M.  Subjective sleep characteristics associated with anxiety and depression in older adults: a population-based study.   Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2014;29(12):1262-1270. doi:10.1002/gps.4106 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Devore  EE, Grodstein  F, Duffy  JF, Stampfer  MJ, Czeisler  CA, Schernhammer  ES.  Sleep duration in midlife and later life in relation to cognition.   J Am Geriatr Soc. 2014;62(6):1073-1081. doi:10.1111/jgs.12790 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Virta  JJ, Heikkilä  K, Perola  M,  et al.  Midlife sleep characteristics associated with late life cognitive function.   Sleep. 2013;36(10):1533-1541, 1541A. doi:10.5665/sleep.3052PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Lim  ASP, Yu  L, Kowgier  M, Schneider  JA, Buchman  AS, Bennett  DA.  Modification of the relationship of the apolipoprotein E ε4 allele to the risk of Alzheimer disease and neurofibrillary tangle density by sleep.   JAMA Neurol. 2013;70(12):1544-1551. doi:10.1001/jamaneurol.2013.4215 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Lysen  TS, Luik  AI, Ikram  MK, Tiemeier  H, Ikram  MA.  Actigraphy-estimated sleep and 24-hour activity rhythms and the risk of dementia.   Alzheimers Dement. 2020;16(9):1259-1267. doi:10.1002/alz.12122 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Cappuccio  FP, Cooper  D, D’Elia  L, Strazzullo  P, Miller  MA.  Sleep duration predicts cardiovascular outcomes: a systematic review and meta-analysis of prospective studies.   Eur Heart J. 2011;32(12):1484-1492. doi:10.1093/eurheartj/ehr007 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
Cappuccio  FP, D’Elia  L, Strazzullo  P, Miller  MA.  Quantity and quality of sleep and incidence of type 2 diabetes: a systematic review and meta-analysis.   Diabetes Care. 2010;33(2):414-420. doi:10.2337/dc09-1124 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Hirshkowitz  M, Whiton  K, Albert  SM,  et al.  National Sleep Foundation’s updated sleep duration recommendations: final report.   Sleep Health. 2015;1(4):233-243. doi:10.1016/j.sleh.2015.10.004 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Lo  JC, Groeger  JA, Cheng  GH, Dijk  D-J, Chee  MWL.  Self-reported sleep duration and cognitive performance in older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis.   Sleep Med. 2016;17:87-98. doi:10.1016/j.sleep.2015.08.021 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
Ma  Y, Liang  L, Zheng  F, Shi  L, Zhong  B, Xie  W.  Association between sleep duration and cognitive decline.   JAMA Netw Open. 2020;3(9):e2013573. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2020.13573 PubMedGoogle Scholar
15.
Spira  AP, Gamaldo  AA, An  Y,  et al.  Self-reported sleep and β-amyloid deposition in community-dwelling older adults.   JAMA Neurol. 2013;70(12):1537-1543. doi:10.1001/jamaneurol.2013.4258 PubMedGoogle Scholar
16.
Jack  CR  Jr, Bennett  DA, Blennow  K,  et al.  NIA-AA research framework: toward a biological definition of Alzheimer’s disease.   Alzheimers Dement. 2018;14(4):535-562. doi:10.1016/j.jalz.2018.02.018 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
Jagust  W.  Imaging the evolution and pathophysiology of Alzheimer disease.   Nat Rev Neurosci. 2018;19(11):687-700. doi:10.1038/s41583-018-0067-3 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
Sperling  RA, Rentz  DM, Johnson  KA,  et al.  The A4 study: stopping AD before symptoms begin?   Sci Transl Med. 2014;6(228):228fs13. doi:10.1126/scitranslmed.3007941 PubMedGoogle Scholar
19.
Sperling  RA, Donohue  MC, Raman  R,  et al; A4 Study Team.  Association of factors with elevated amyloid burden in clinically normal older individuals.   JAMA Neurol. 2020;77(6):735-745. doi:10.1001/jamaneurol.2020.0387 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Deters  KD, Napolioni  V, Sperling  RA,  et al.  Amyloid PET imaging in self-identified non-Hispanic Black participants of the anti-amyloid in asymptomatic Alzheimer’s disease (A4) study.   Neurology. 2021;96(11):e1491-e1500. doi:10.1212/WNL.0000000000011599 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
Kingsbury  JH, Buxton  OM, Emmons  KM.  Sleep and its relationship to racial and ethnic disparities in cardiovascular disease.   Curr Cardiovasc Risk Rep. 2013;7(5). doi:10.1007/s12170-013-0330-0 PubMedGoogle Scholar
22.
Carnethon  MR, De Chavez  PJ, Zee  PC,  et al.  Disparities in sleep characteristics by race/ethnicity in a population-based sample: Chicago Area Sleep Study.   Sleep Med. 2016;18:50-55. doi:10.1016/j.sleep.2015.07.005 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
23.
Donohue  MC, Sperling  RA, Salmon  DP,  et al; Australian Imaging, Biomarkers, and Lifestyle Flagship Study of Ageing; Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative; Alzheimer’s Disease Cooperative Study.  The preclinical Alzheimer cognitive composite: measuring amyloid-related decline.   JAMA Neurol. 2014;71(8):961-970. doi:10.1001/jamaneurol.2014.803 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Mormino  EC, Papp  KV, Rentz  DM,  et al.  Early and late change on the preclinical Alzheimer’s cognitive composite in clinically normal older individuals with elevated amyloid β.   Alzheimers Dement. 2017;13(9):1004-1012. doi:10.1016/j.jalz.2017.01.018 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
Grober  E, Sanders  AE, Hall  C, Lipton  RB.  Free and cued selective reminding identifies very mild dementia in primary care.   Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2010;24(3):284-290. doi:10.1097/WAD.0b013e3181cfc78b PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
Amariglio  RE, Donohue  MC, Marshall  GA,  et al; Alzheimer’s Disease Cooperative Study.  Tracking early decline in cognitive function in older individuals at risk for Alzheimer disease dementia: the Alzheimer’s Disease Cooperative Study Cognitive Function Instrument.   JAMA Neurol. 2015;72(4):446-454. doi:10.1001/jamaneurol.2014.3375 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
27.
Kronholm  E, Sallinen  M, Suutama  T, Sulkava  R, Era  P, Partonen  T.  Self-reported sleep duration and cognitive functioning in the general population.   J Sleep Res. 2009;18(4):436-446. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2869.2009.00765.x PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
28.
Papp  KV, Rentz  DM, Mormino  EC,  et al.  Cued memory decline in biomarker-defined preclinical Alzheimer disease.   Neurology. 2017;88(15):1431-1438. doi:10.1212/WNL.0000000000003812 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
29.
Kang  J-E, Lim  MM, Bateman  RJ,  et al.  Amyloid-beta dynamics are regulated by orexin and the sleep-wake cycle.   Science. 2009;326(5955):1005-1007. doi:10.1126/science.1180962 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
30.
Roh  JH, Huang  Y, Bero  AW,  et al.  Disruption of the sleep-wake cycle and diurnal fluctuation of β-amyloid in mice with Alzheimer’s disease pathology.   Sci Transl Med. 2012;4(150):150ra122. doi:10.1126/scitranslmed.3004291 PubMedGoogle Scholar
31.
Lucey  BP, Hicks  TJ, McLeland  JS,  et al.  Effect of sleep on overnight cerebrospinal fluid amyloid β kinetics.   Ann Neurol. 2018;83(1):197-204. doi:10.1002/ana.25117 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
32.
Xie  L, Kang  H, Xu  Q,  et al.  Sleep drives metabolite clearance from the adult brain.   Science. 2013;342(6156):373-377. doi:10.1126/science.1241224 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
33.
Hablitz  LM, Plá  V, Giannetto  M,  et al.  Circadian control of brain glymphatic and lymphatic fluid flow.   Nat Commun. 2020;11(1):4411. doi:10.1038/s41467-020-18115-2 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
34.
Fultz  NE, Bonmassar  G, Setsompop  K,  et al.  Coupled electrophysiological, hemodynamic, and cerebrospinal fluid oscillations in human sleep.   Science. 2019;366(6465):628-631. doi:10.1126/science.aax5440 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
35.
Branger  P, Arenaza-Urquijo  EM, Tomadesso  C,  et al.  Relationships between sleep quality and brain volume, metabolism, and amyloid deposition in late adulthood.   Neurobiol Aging. 2016;41:107-114. doi:10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2016.02.009 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
36.
Sprecher  KE, Koscik  RL, Carlsson  CM,  et al.  Poor sleep is associated with CSF biomarkers of amyloid pathology in cognitively normal adults.   Neurology. 2017;89(5):445-453. doi:10.1212/WNL.0000000000004171 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
37.
Ju  YE-S, McLeland  JS, Toedebusch  CD,  et al.  Sleep quality and preclinical Alzheimer disease.   JAMA Neurol. 2013;70(5):587-593. doi:10.1001/jamaneurol.2013.2334 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
38.
Ettore  E, Bakardjian  H, Solé  M,  et al.  Relationships between objectives sleep parameters and brain amyloid load in subjects at risk for Alzheimer’s disease: the INSIGHT-preAD Study.   Sleep. 2019;42(9):zsz137. doi:10.1093/sleep/zsz137 PubMedGoogle Scholar
39.
Mander  BA, Marks  SM, Vogel  JW,  et al.  β-amyloid disrupts human NREM slow waves and related hippocampus-dependent memory consolidation.   Nat Neurosci. 2015;18(7):1051-1057. doi:10.1038/nn.4035 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
40.
Varga  AW, Wohlleber  ME, Giménez  S,  et al.  Reduced slow-wave sleep is associated with high cerebrospinal fluid Aβ42 levels in cognitively normal elderly.   Sleep. 2016;39(11):2041-2048. doi:10.5665/sleep.6240 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
41.
Winer  JR, Mander  BA, Kumar  S,  et al.  Sleep disturbance forecasts β-amyloid accumulation across subsequent years.   Curr Biol. 2020;30(21):4291-4298.e3. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2020.08.017 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
42.
Mander  BA, Winer  JR, Jagust  WJ, Walker  MP.  Sleep: a novel mechanistic pathway, biomarker, and treatment target in the pathology of Alzheimer’s disease?   Trends Neurosci. 2016;39(8):552-566. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2016.05.002 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
43.
Wang  C, Holtzman  DM.  Bidirectional relationship between sleep and Alzheimer’s disease: role of amyloid, tau, and other factors.   Neuropsychopharmacology. 2020;45(1):104-120. doi:10.1038/s41386-019-0478-5 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
44.
Grandner  MA, Drummond  SPA.  Who are the long sleepers? Towards an understanding of the mortality relationship.   Sleep Med Rev. 2007;11(5):341-360. doi:10.1016/j.smrv.2007.03.010 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
45.
Benito-León  J, Louis  ED, Bermejo-Pareja  F.  Cognitive decline in short and long sleepers: a prospective population-based study (NEDICES).   J Psychiatr Res. 2013;47(12):1998-2003. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychires.2013.09.007 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
46.
Yaffe  K, Falvey  CM, Hoang  T.  Connections between sleep and cognition in older adults.   Lancet Neurol. 2014;13(10):1017-1028. doi:10.1016/S1474-4422(14)70172-3 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
47.
Lemos  R, Duro  D, Simões  MR, Santana  I.  The free and cued selective reminding test distinguishes frontotemporal dementia from Alzheimer’s disease.   Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2014;29(7):670-679. doi:10.1093/arclin/acu031 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
48.
Nicolazzo  J, Xu  K, Lavale  A,  et al.  Sleep symptomatology is associated with greater subjective cognitive concerns: findings from the community-based Healthy Brain Project.   Sleep. 2021;zsab097. doi:10.1093/sleep/zsab097 PubMedGoogle Scholar
49.
Leng  Y, Wainwright  NWJ, Cappuccio  FP,  et al.  Self-reported sleep patterns in a British population cohort.   Sleep Med. 2014;15(3):295-302. doi:10.1016/j.sleep.2013.10.015 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
50.
George  KM, Peterson  RL, Gilsanz  P,  et al.  Racial/ethnic differences in sleep quality among older adults: Kaiser Healthy Aging and Diverse Life Experiences (KHANDLE) study.   Ethn Dis. 2020;30(3):469-478. doi:10.18865/ed.30.3.469 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
51.
Grandner  MA, Williams  NJ, Knutson  KL, Roberts  D, Jean-Louis  G.  Sleep disparity, race/ethnicity, and socioeconomic position.   Sleep Med. 2016;18:7-18. doi:10.1016/j.sleep.2015.01.020 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
52.
Stamatakis  KA, Kaplan  GA, Roberts  RE.  Short sleep duration across income, education, and race/ethnic groups: population prevalence and growing disparities during 34 years of follow-up.   Ann Epidemiol. 2007;17(12):948-955. doi:10.1016/j.annepidem.2007.07.096 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
53.
Whinnery  J, Jackson  N, Rattanaumpawan  P, Grandner  MA.  Short and long sleep duration associated with race/ethnicity, sociodemographics, and socioeconomic position.   Sleep. 2014;37(3):601-611. doi:10.5665/sleep.3508 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
54.
Slopen  N, Williams  DR.  Discrimination, other psychosocial stressors, and self-reported sleep duration and difficulties.   Sleep. 2014;37(1):147-156. doi:10.5665/sleep.3326 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
55.
Thomas  KS, Bardwell  WA, Ancoli-Israel  S, Dimsdale  JE.  The toll of ethnic discrimination on sleep architecture and fatigue.   Health Psychol. 2006;25(5):635-642. doi:10.1037/0278-6133.25.5.635 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
56.
Beatty  DL, Hall  MH, Kamarck  TA,  et al.  Unfair treatment is associated with poor sleep in African American and Caucasian adults: Pittsburgh SleepSCORE project.   Health Psychol. 2011;30(3):351-359. doi:10.1037/a0022976 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
57.
Altman  NG, Izci-Balserak  B, Schopfer  E,  et al.  Sleep duration versus sleep insufficiency as predictors of cardiometabolic health outcomes.   Sleep Med. 2012;13(10):1261-1270. doi:10.1016/j.sleep.2012.08.005 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
58.
Knutson  KL.  Sleep duration and cardiometabolic risk: a review of the epidemiologic evidence.   Best Pract Res Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2010;24(5):731-743. doi:10.1016/j.beem.2010.07.001 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
59.
Hsu  DC, Mormino  EC, Schultz  AP,  et al; Harvard Aging Brain Study.  Lower late-life body-mass index is associated with higher cortical amyloid burden in clinically normal elderly.   J Alzheimers Dis. 2016;53(3):1097-1105. doi:10.3233/JAD-150987 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
60.
Leng  Y, Redline  S, Stone  KL, Ancoli-Israel  S, Yaffe  K.  Objective napping, cognitive decline, and risk of cognitive impairment in older men.   Alzheimers Dement. 2019;15(8):1039-1047. doi:10.1016/j.jalz.2019.04.009 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
61.
Yaffe  K, Laffan  AM, Harrison  SL,  et al.  Sleep-disordered breathing, hypoxia, and risk of mild cognitive impairment and dementia in older women.   JAMA. 2011;306(6):613-619. doi:10.1001/jama.2011.1115 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
62.
Osorio  RS, Gumb  T, Pirraglia  E,  et al; Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative.  Sleep-disordered breathing advances cognitive decline in the elderly.   Neurology. 2015;84(19):1964-1971. doi:10.1212/WNL.0000000000001566 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
63.
Ju  Y-ES, Finn  MB, Sutphen  CL,  et al.  Obstructive sleep apnea decreases central nervous system-derived proteins in the cerebrospinal fluid.   Ann Neurol. 2016;80(1):154-159. doi:10.1002/ana.24672 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
64.
Sharma  RA, Varga  AW, Bubu  OM,  et al.  Obstructive sleep apnea severity affects amyloid burden in cognitively normal elderly. A longitudinal study.   Am J Respir Crit Care Med. 2018;197(7):933-943. doi:10.1164/rccm.201704-0704OC PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
65.
André  C, Rehel  S, Kuhn  E,  et al; Medit-Ageing Research Group.  Association of sleep-disordered breathing with Alzheimer disease biomarkers in community-dwelling older adults: a secondary analysis of a randomized clinical trial.   JAMA Neurol. 2020;77(6):716-724. doi:10.1001/jamaneurol.2020.0311 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
66.
Bubu  OM, Pirraglia  E, Andrade  AG,  et al; Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative.  Obstructive sleep apnea and longitudinal Alzheimer’s disease biomarker changes.   Sleep. 2019;42(6):zsz048. doi:10.1093/sleep/zsz048 PubMedGoogle Scholar
67.
Duarte  RLM, Mendes  BA, Oliveira-E-Sá  TS, Magalhães-da-Silveira  FJ, Gozal  D.  Perception of sleep duration in adult patients with suspected obstructive sleep apnea.   PLoS One. 2020;15(8):e0238083. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0238083 PubMedGoogle Scholar
68.
Lauderdale  DS, Knutson  KL, Yan  LL, Liu  K, Rathouz  PJ.  Self-reported and measured sleep duration: how similar are they?   Epidemiology. 2008;19(6):838-845. doi:10.1097/EDE.0b013e318187a7b0 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
69.
Landry  GJ, Best  JR, Liu-Ambrose  T.  Measuring sleep quality in older adults: a comparison using subjective and objective methods.   Front Aging Neurosci. 2015;7:166. doi:10.3389/fnagi.2015.00166 PubMedGoogle Scholar
70.
Matthews  KA, Patel  SR, Pantesco  EJ,  et al.  Similarities and differences in estimates of sleep duration by polysomnography, actigraphy, diary, and self-reported habitual sleep in a community sample.   Sleep Health. 2018;4(1):96-103. doi:10.1016/j.sleh.2017.10.011 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
71.
Kaplan  KA, Hardas  PP, Redline  S, Zeitzer  JM; Sleep Heart Health Study Research Group.  Correlates of sleep quality in midlife and beyond: a machine learning analysis.   Sleep Med. 2017;34:162-167. doi:10.1016/j.sleep.2017.03.004 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
72.
Buysse  DJ.  Sleep health: can we define it? Does it matter?   Sleep. 2014;37(1):9-17. doi:10.5665/sleep.3298 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
1 Comment for this article
EXPAND ALL
RE: Association of Short and Long Sleep Duration With Amyloid-β Burden and Cognition in Aging
Tomoyuki Kawada, MD | Nippon Medical School
Winer et al. conducted a cross-sectional study to investigate the association between self-reported sleep duration and brain amyloid beta (Aβ) burden (1). Self-reported shorter sleep duration was linearly associated with higher Aβ burden and associated with reduced cognition that was remarkable in memory domains. In contrast, there was no significant difference in brain Aβ between long and normal sleep duration groups. But long sleep duration was associated with worse performance across multiple cognitive domains. The authors emphasized the importance of maintaining adequate sleep, and I have a comment about their study, presenting two meta-analyses.
Hudon et al. conducted a meta-analysis,
presenting that depression, short and long sleep duration were significantly associated with behavioral/psychological symptoms and cognitive decline (2). Fan et al. also conducted a meta-analysis of prospective studies (3). Pooled hazard ratios (95% confidence intervals) of long sleep duration for all-cause dementia and Alzheimer disease (AD) were 1.77 (1.32-2.37) and 1.63 (1.24-2.13), respectively. In contrast, there was no significant association between short sleep duration, all-cause dementia and AD, which was inconsistent with data by Winer et al. Although neural biomarkers were not used in meta-analyses, increased brain Aβ burden by short sleep duration could not be explained by past epidemiologic data.
In any case, more prospective data are needed to identify the association between sleep duration and subsequent brain Aβ burden.

References
1. Winer JR, Deters KD, Kennedy G, et al. Association of Short and Long Sleep Duration With Amyloid-β Burden and Cognition in Aging. JAMA Neurol 2021;78(10):1187-96.
2. Hudon C, Escudier F, De Roy J, et al. Behavioral and Psychological Symptoms that Predict Cognitive Decline or Impairment in Cognitively Normal Middle-Aged or Older Adults: a Meta-Analysis. Neuropsychol Rev 2020;30(4):558-79.
3. Fan L, Xu W, Cai Y, et al. Sleep Duration and the Risk of Dementia: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis of Prospective Cohort Studies. J Am Med Dir Assoc 2019;20(12):1480-7.e5.

CONFLICT OF INTEREST: None Reported
READ MORE
Original Investigation
August 30, 2021

Association of Short and Long Sleep Duration With Amyloid-β Burden and Cognition in Aging

Author Affiliations
  • 1Department of Neurology and Neurological Sciences, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford, California
  • 2Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University, Stanford, California
  • 3Sierra-Pacific Mental Illness Research, Education, and Clinical Center, Veterans Affairs Palo Alto Health Care System, Palo Alto, California
JAMA Neurol. 2021;78(10):1187-1196. doi:10.1001/jamaneurol.2021.2876
Key Points

Question  What role does self-reported sleep duration play in brain amyloid-β accumulation, cognitive performance, and lifestyle factors in the context of healthy aging?

Findings  In this cross-sectional study of 4417 older adults with normal cognition, a higher amyloid-β burden was associated with short sleep duration. Sleeping duration of 6 hours or less or 9 hours or more was associated with distinct deficits in cognitive performance as well as greater depressive symptoms, body mass index, and daytime napping.

Meaning  Short and long sleep durations were associated with multiple adverse health outcomes, highlighting the importance of healthy sleep in aging.

Abstract

Importance  Disrupted sleep is common in aging and is associated with cognition. Age-related changes to sleep are associated with multiple causes, including early Alzheimer disease pathology (amyloid β [Aβ]), depression, and cardiovascular disease.

Objective  To investigate the associations between self-reported sleep duration and brain Aβ burden as well as the demographic, cognitive, and lifestyle variables in adults with normal cognition.

Design, Setting, and Participants  This cross-sectional study obtained data from participants in the Anti-Amyloid Treatment in Asymptomatic Alzheimer’s Disease (A4) study, which is being conducted in 67 sites in the United States, Canada, Australia, and Japan. The sample for this analysis consisted of individuals aged 65 to 85 years who underwent an Aβ positron emission tomography (PET) scan, had complete apolipoprotein E (APOE) genotype data, and were identified as clinically normal (per a Clinical Dementia Rating score of 0) and cognitively unimpaired (per a Mini-Mental State Examination score of 25 to 30 and Logical Memory Delayed Recall test score of 6 to 18). Data were analyzed from April 3, 2020, to June 20, 2021.

Main Outcomes and Measures  The outcome was self-reported nightly sleep duration (grouped by short sleep duration: ≤6 hours, normal sleep duration: 7-8 hours, and long sleep duration: ≥9 hours) compared with demographic characteristics, Aβ burden (as measured with a fluorine 18–labeled-florbetapir PET scan), objective and subjective cognitive function measures, and lifestyle variables.

Results  The 4417 participants in the study included 2618 women (59%) and had a mean (SD) age of 71.3 (4.7) years. Self-reported shorter sleep duration was linearly associated with higher Aβ burden (β [SE] = –0.01 [0.00]; P = .005), and short sleep duration was associated with reduced cognition that was mostly in memory domains. No difference in Aβ was found between long and normal sleep duration groups (β [SE] = 0.00 [0.01]; P = .99). However, compared with normal sleep duration, both short and long sleep durations were associated with higher body mass index (short vs normal sleep duration: β [SE] = 0.48 [0.17], P = .01; long vs normal sleep duration: β [SE] = 0.97 [0.31], P = .002), depressive symptoms (short vs normal sleep duration: β [SE] = 0.31 [0.05], P < .001; long vs normal sleep duration: β [SE] = 0.39 [0.09], P < .001), and daytime napping (short vs normal sleep duration: β [SE] = 2.66 [0.77], P = .001; long vs normal sleep duration: β [SE] = 3.62 [1.38], P = .01). Long sleep duration was associated with worse performance across multiple cognitive domains.

Conclusions and Relevance  In this cross-sectional study, both short and long sleep durations were associated with worse outcomes for older adults, such as greater Aβ burden, greater depressive symptoms, higher body mass index, and cognitive decline, emphasizing the importance of maintaining adequate sleep.

Introduction

Aging is associated with robust changes in sleep, resulting from shifts in lifestyle and underlying neurophysiological mechanisms.1 These changes are not uniform but instead heterogeneous, with variability in sleep architecture among older adults observed even in the absence of disease.2,3 Sleep health has important consequences for aging trajectories, and sleep disruption has been associated with increased risk of depression,4,5 cognitive decline,6,7 Alzheimer disease (AD),8,9 and cardiovascular and metabolic outcomes.10,11 Because poor sleep represents a potentially modifiable risk factor, it is critical to identify cognitive and biological correlates of sleep disturbance in the context of healthy aging.

Nocturnal sleep duration has been a focus of public health recommendations, with 7 to 8 hours of sleep typically advised for older adults.12 Focusing on cognitive decline, both short sleep (≤6 h/night) and long sleep (≥9 h/night) durations have been associated with worse outcomes.7,13,14 Although short sleep duration has been associated with greater brain amyloid-β (Aβ) burden in healthy older adults,15 which is believed to reflect a preclinical stage of AD,16,17 the modest sample size of previous Aβ positron emission tomography (PET) imaging studies and the rarity of long sleep duration have precluded a thorough investigation of the distinct factors in short and long sleep duration while modeling the outcome of elevated Aβ.

We investigated the associations between self-reported sleep duration and brain Aβ burden as well as the demographic, cognitive, and lifestyle variables in a cohort of 4417 older adults with normal cognition. This sample size allowed the opportunity to examine both the linear (ie, is self-reported sleep continuously associated with aging phenotypes?) and nonlinear (ie, how do short and long sleep duration groups compared with the normal [7-8 hours] sleep duration reference group?) associations of the outcomes in a large cohort of older individuals with Aβ status. We hypothesized that multiple independent variables would play a role in sleep duration, such that greater Aβ burden would be associated with short sleep duration, but that worse age-related cognitive and lifestyle outcomes would be associated with both short and long sleep durations.

Methods

From April 3, 2020, to June 20, 2021, we analyzed data from participants in the Anti-Amyloid Treatment in Asymptomatic Alzheimer’s Disease (A4) study, which was conducted at 67 sites across the United States, Canada, Australia, and Japan. Institutional review board approval was obtained at each site of the A4 Study. All participants provided written informed consent before participation.

A total of 6763 individuals with normal cognition between age 65 and 85 years were initially screened for the A4 Study.18,19 Participants with a Clinical Dementia Rating global score of 0, Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) score of 25 to 30, and Logical Memory Delayed Recall test score of 6 to 18 were eligible for fluorine 18–labeled-florbetapir PET imaging. A total of 4486 participants underwent a PET scan, and of this sample, 45 individuals were excluded from the present cross-sectional study for missing apolipoprotein E (APOE [OMIM 107741]) genotype data. An additional 24 individuals were excluded for having cognitive test scores that did not meet the screening criteria on rescreening, resulting in a final analysis sample of 4417 participants. An overview of the inclusion process in this study is presented in eFigure 1 in the Supplement.

Self-identified race/ethnicity information was collected during screening. Because both Aβ burden20 and sleep duration21,22 have been shown to differ across racial/ethnic groups, self-identified race/ethnicity was used as a covariate in the analyses. Race/ethnicity was reclassified into 4 categories: Latino or Hispanic White (n = 112), non-Hispanic Asian (n = 168), non-Hispanic Black or African American (n = 155), and non-Hispanic White (n = 3881). A fifth category, other (including Hispanic American Indian or Alaskan Native, Hispanic Asian, Hispanic Native Hawaiian or Pacific Islander, and Hispanic Black or African American; non-Hispanic American Indian or Alaskan Native and non-Hispanic Native Hawaiian or Pacific Islander; and anyone who identified as unknown, did not report race/ethnicity, or identified as more than 1 race/ethnicity19), was used for all remaining participants (n = 101).

To probe different cognitive domains, we examined the performance on the individual components of the Preclinical Alzheimer Cognitive Composite23,24: MMSE (score range: 0-30, with higher scores indicating better cognitive performance), Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST; score range: 0-91, with higher scores indicating correct responses in 90 seconds), Logical Memory Delayed Recall test (score range: 0-25, with higher scores indicating more items recalled), and Free and Cued Selective Reminding Test (FCSRT; score range: 0-48, with higher scores indicating more items recalled). We examined 2 FCSRT scores: free recall across all 3 trials (maximum score of 48) and total recall, which is a sum of both free recall and cueing of items that were not freely recalled across all 3 trials (maximum score of 48).24,25 To assess subjective reports of cognition, we used the Cognitive Function Index (CFI), a questionnaire that was administered separately to the participants and their study partner (score range: 0-15, with higher scores indicating worse reported cognitive function).26 Depressive symptoms were evaluated with the Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS), an instrument that does not include questions about sleep disturbance (score range: 0-15, with higher scores indicating higher depressive symptoms). In a lifestyle questionnaire, participants were asked to report their typical number of hours of sleep per night, minutes of daytime naps per day, and caffeinated and/or alcoholic drinks per day.

All participants received an injection of 10 mCi of fluorine 18–labeled-florbetapir and underwent a PET scan 50 to 70 minutes after injection. Mean cortical standardized uptake value ratio was calculated using a whole cerebellar reference region.19 All analyses used continuous standardized uptake value ratios.

Statistical Analysis

Demographic characteristics of the sleep duration groups (short sleep duration: ≤6 hours, normal sleep duration: 7-8 hours, and long sleep duration: ≥9 hours) were compared using χ2 tests for categorical variables and analysis of variance for continuous variables. Associations between sleep duration and outcomes were assessed using linear regression. To explore nonlinear associations with sleep duration, analyses were repeated using a 3-factor variable (short sleep duration: ≤6 hours, normal sleep duration: 7-8 hours, and long sleep duration: ≥9 hours7,27) in linear regressions, with all 3 post hoc contrasts examined (short vs normal sleep duration, long vs normal sleep duration, and short vs long sleep duration). Logistic regression was performed to identify the association between sleep duration and the maximum FCSRT total recall score (48 vs <48), given that most older individuals who are clinically normal are at the upper score limit but that slight decrements in cueing are associated with early changes related to AD.24,25,28

All analyses included the following covariates: age, sex, years of education, self-identified race/ethnicity, number of APOE ε2 alleles, and number of APOE ε4 alleles. No multiple comparisons correction was performed. All statistical analyses were performed in R, version 4.0.2 (R Foundation for Statistical Computing). We considered 2-sided P < .05 to be statistically significant.

We visualized associations by reclassifying sleep duration into 5 groups (≤5 hours [n = 288; 7%], 6 hours [n = 897; 20%], 7 hours [n = 1574; 36%], 8 hours [n = 1375; 31%], and ≥9 hours [n = 283; 6%]), using the adjusted mean and SE of each outcome by sleep duration group. This strategy was used for data visualization purposes only; group contrasts were not performed across the 5 groups.

Results

The 4417 participants included in the study consisted of 2618 women (59%) and 1799 men (40%) with a mean (SD) age of 71.3 (4.7) years. When examined individually, all demographic variables (age, sex, years of education, and race/ethnicity) were associated with sleep duration (Table 1). In a multiple regression model that examined all demographic variables simultaneously, sex, years of education, and race/ethnicity were independently associated with sleep duration. Specifically, female sex (β [SE] = 0.07 [0.03]; P = .03) and greater years of education (β [SE] = 0.02 [0.01]; P = .002) were both significantly associated with self-reported longer nightly sleep duration. Compared with non-Hispanic White participants, non-Hispanic Black or African American participants reported a mean (SD) sleep duration of 37.9 (5.0) minutes less (P < .001), non-Hispanic Asian participants reported 27.3 (5.2) minutes less (P < .001), and Latino or Hispanic White participants reported 15.0 (6.1) minutes less (P = .01).

Associations between sleep duration and Aβ burden, cognitive function, and lifestyle outcomes are presented in Table 2. Higher Aβ burden was associated with fewer hours of nightly sleep (β [SE] = –0.01 [0.00]; P = .005) (Figure 1). When comparing short and long sleep durations with normal (7 to 8 hours) sleep duration, only short sleep duration was associated with elevated Aβ (short vs normal sleep duration: β [SE] = 0.01 [0.01], P = .048; long vs normal sleep duration: β [SE] = 0.00 [0.01], P = .99) (Table 2).

No significant linear associations were found between sleep duration and objective cognitive test performance. However, when categorized by short, normal, or long sleep duration, the differences in cognitive performance were observed across groups, suggesting nonlinear associations. Participants with short sleep duration performed significantly worse on the MMSE than those who reported normal sleep duration (β [SE] = –0.08 [0.04]; P = .04) (Figure 2A; Table 2) and Logical Memory Delayed Recall test (β [SE] = –0.23 [0.11]; P = .03) (Figure 2B; Table 2), and they were less likely to obtain the maximum FCSRT total recall score (β [SE] = –0.18 [0.07]; P = .01) (Table 2). Conversely, participants with long sleep duration performed significantly worse on the DSST than those who reported normal sleep duration (β [SE] = –1.17 [0.52]; P = .02) (Figure 2C; Table 2). In addition, those who had short sleep duration performed better on the FCSRT free recall than those with long sleep duration (β [SE] = 0.73 [0.35]; P = .04) (Table 2). No other differences in cognitive test scores were found between the short and long sleep duration groups.

Subjective report of cognitive ability, measured by the CFI, also differed on the basis of sleep duration. Better self-reported cognitive function (lower score on the participant-assessed CFI) had a linear association with longer sleep duration (β [SE] = –0.06 [0.03]; P = .03) (Table 2). Short and long sleep durations also were both associated with worse self-reported cognitive function compared with normal sleep duration on the participant-assessed CFI (short vs normal sleep duration: β [SE] = 0.34 [0.07], P < .001; long vs normal sleep duration: β [SE] = 0.57 [0.12], P < .001]) (Figure 2D; Table 2). The study partner–assessed CFI was also worse for the short and long sleep duration groups vs the normal sleep duration group (short vs normal sleep duration: β [SE] = 0.22 [0.06], P < .001; long vs normal sleep duration: β [SE] = 0.33 [0.11], P = .003) (Figure 2E; Table 2). No differences in study partner–assessed CFI scores were found between the short and long sleep duration groups (β [SE] = –0.11 [0.12]; P = .34).

Greater endorsement of depressive symptoms on the GDS had a linear association with shorter sleep duration (β [SE] = –0.06 [0.02]; P = .005) (Figure 3A; Table 2), and both short and long sleep duration groups reported more depressive symptoms than the normal sleep duration group (short vs normal sleep duration: β [SE] = 0.31 [0.05], P < .001; long vs normal sleep duration: β [SE] = 0.39 [0.09], P < .001) (Table 2). No linear association was observed between body mass index (BMI) and sleep duration, but BMI was elevated in both short and long sleep duration groups (short vs normal sleep duration: β [SE] = 0.48 [0.17], P = .01; long vs normal sleep duration: β [SE] = 0.97 [0.31], P = .002) (Figure 3B; Table 2). Self-reported caffeine consumption was not associated with sleep duration. Self-reported alcohol consumption had a linear association with nighttime sleep duration (β [SE] = 0.07 [0.02]; P < .001) (Table 2), and those in the long sleep duration group consumed more alcoholic drinks compared with those in both normal and short sleep duration groups (long vs normal sleep duration: β [SE] = 0.38 [0.07], P < .001; long vs short sleep duration: β [SE] = –0.41 [0.08], P < .001) (Table 2). Longer daytime nap was associated with less continuous nighttime sleep duration (β [SE] = –0.86 [0.32]; P = .01) (Table 2) and was significantly elevated in participants in both short and long sleep duration groups vs normal sleep duration group (short vs normal sleep duration: β [SE] = 2.66 [0.77], P = .001; long vs normal sleep duration: β [SE] = 3.62 [1.38], P = .01; short vs long duration: β [SE] = –0.96 [1.48], P = .52) (Figure 3C; Table 2).

Two additional sets of analyses were performed to ascertain whether associations between nighttime sleep duration and Aβ burden as well as nighttime sleep duration and cognition remained significant after controlling for lifestyle factors. First, we repeated the analyses with Aβ PET and cognitive outcomes as the dependent variables and included lifestyle variables as the covariates (summarized in eTable 1 in the Supplement). Adding these covariates did not change the association between nighttime sleep duration and Aβ or cognitive outcomes, with the exception of DSST score, which was associated with GDS score, BMI, and daytime napping duration but was no longer significantly associated with long sleep duration. Napping was a significant independent factor in FCSRT free recall (β [SE] = –0.01 [0.00]; P < .001) and participant-assessed CFI (β [SE] = 0.00 [0.00]; P < .001) scores in addition to nighttime sleep duration (eTable 1 in the Supplement).

Second, given the nonlinear association between nighttime sleep and daytime nap duration (Figure 3C), we tested the interaction between nighttime sleep and daytime nap to ascertain whether the associations with napping were dependent on short vs long sleep duration. However, we found no significant interaction between nocturnal sleeping and daytime napping, with the exception of FCSRT total recall performance (β [SE] = –0.01 [0.01]; P = .03) (eTable 2 and eFigure 2 in the Supplement).

Discussion

We found that self-reported short and long sleep durations were associated with worse outcomes in cognitively healthy older adults. Sleep duration of 6 hours or less was associated with higher Aβ burden and lower memory performance. In contrast, sleep duration of 9 hours or more was associated with worse executive function test performance. Both short and long sleep durations were associated with worse self-reported cognitive function and multiple lifestyle outcomes, including higher BMI, greater depressive symptoms, and more time spent napping during the day, suggesting a U-shaped association between sleep duration and these variables. Overall, this pattern highlights that multiple independent factors are associated with sleep duration in aging.

We identified an association between self-reported shorter sleep duration and greater Aβ burden. This finding is consistent with results of a previous study of 70 older adults, which showed that self-reported short sleep duration was associated with elevated Aβ burden,15 and studies that found that sleep restriction accelerated Aβ deposition in rodent models of AD.29,30 The association between sleep loss and Aβ accumulation has been attributed to increased production of Aβ at synapses during wake vs sleep29,31 combined with a reduction in clearance of Aβ through the glymphatic system, which is preferentially active during non–rapid eye movement sleep.32-34 In the present cross-sectional study, the subjective measure of short sleep duration may serve as a proxy measure for other indices of poor sleep quality. This suggestion is supported by previous work that reported an association between greater Aβ burden and several metrics of decreased sleep quality, including subjective sleep quality,35,36 objective sleep efficiency and fragmentation,37,38 and disruptions of non–rapid eye movement slow wave activity.39-41 The association between short sleep duration and Aβ is also consistent with our finding that short sleep duration was associated with subtle deficits in memory (per Logical Memory Delayed Recall test and FCSRT total recall scores). Although the direction of these associations is unclear, the findings in this study provide further support to the theory that short sleep in aging is associated with early AD processes.42,43 The causality of these associations cannot be established with these cross-sectional data.

We found no difference in association with Aβ burden between long and normal sleep durations. To our knowledge, this study was the first to examine whether long sleep duration, which is believed to be a marker of poor health in older adults,44,45 is associated with Aβ levels. The finding suggests that early AD processes do not mediate the established association between long sleep duration and cognitive decline.46 Long sleep duration was associated with a number of other phenotypes related to aging (eg, worse executive function, worse subjective cognition, higher GDS score, higher BMI), highlighting that the adverse impact of longer sleep is generally associated with aging processes that are independent of early Aβ. Reported long sleep duration reflected a small proportion of the population in the Netherlands, United Kingdom, and United States,3 drawing attention to a unique feature of the present study: the ability to analyze this group in a large sample of older individuals with normal cognition characterized through Aβ PET. This group has largely been ignored in biomarker cohorts that typically comprised fewer than 200 individuals. This study found that, although this group had no evidence of elevated Aβ levels, it demonstrated subtle cognitive decline, especially in executive function (per DSST and FCSRT free recall scores). We also found some evidence of a dissociation within the FCSRT scores, with subtle deficits in the free recall portion (which is believed to capture frontal retrieval and attention processes) associated with long sleep duration and the cueing component (which is more specific to AD and may be mediated by the medial temporal lobe47) associated with short sleep. Overall, the association between executive function and long sleep duration highlights the role of additional age-related processes. Future work is needed to explore the neural correlates of cognitive aging in individuals with long sleep duration.

The findings on cognition build on previous descriptions of inverted U-shaped associations between self-reported sleep duration and cognition in aging, in which extreme sleep durations were associated with worse cognitive performance.6,7,13,14,46 A meta-analysis of sleep duration and cognition in aging reported that this U-shaped association is generally consistent across the literature, but the associations with specific cognitive domains are inconsistent.13 It remains controversial whether some cognitive domains are more affected by extreme sleep duration than other domains and whether short and long sleep durations may be associated with distinct patterns of cognitive dysfunction. In a large, well-characterized cohort of healthy older adults who met the screening criteria for the A4 Study, we found a distinction between sleep duration phenotypes in which those with short sleep duration performed worse on tests of memory and those with long sleep duration performed worse in an executive function task.

Subjective cognitive function, as assessed with the CFI,26 also showed a U-shaped association with sleep duration, with both short and long duration phenotypes endorsing worse cognitive function compared with normal sleep duration. The CFI focuses on ability to perform activities of daily living but excludes sleep. This pattern held for both participant-assessed and study partner–assessed CFI, which meant that older individuals who were sleeping outside the recommended 7 to 8 hours (and their study partners) reported worse cognitive function, even when adjusted for other known factors in late-life cognition, such as demographic characteristics and APOE ε2 and ε4 alleles. This association supports a previous finding that CFI scores were associated with worse subjective sleep quality,48 and this finding is important because CFI performance is a sign of subsequent clinical progression.26

Sleep duration was shorter in male participants; individuals with fewer years of education; and individuals who identified as non-Hispanic Black or African American, non-Hispanic Asian, or Latino or Hispanic White, replicating previous reports that sociodemographic variables were associated with sleep duration in late life.21,22,49,50 The substantive differences in sleep duration across racial/ethnic groups provide further evidence that disparities in sleep may be associated with disparities in other areas, such as cardiovascular and metabolic health.21,51 Although socioeconomic factors likely play a role,52,53 previous examinations of the link between racial/ethnic identity and sleep duration have suggested that racial discrimination and perceived racism are associated with less sleep.54-56 More data are needed from racial/ethnic minority populations to assess whether the sleep duration associations reported in studies involving mostly non-Hispanic White participants (such as the present cohort) are generalizable to the population at large.

Sleep duration was associated with several lifestyle variables of health outcomes in aging. First, those with short and long sleep durations reported more depressive symptoms than individuals who slept 7 to 8 hours, which is consistent with previous reports that linked subjective sleep disruption to mood in older adults.4,5 Second, BMI was elevated in participants with short and long sleep durations. Insufficient sleep has been associated with higher BMI in several studies of older adults, and both short and long sleep durations have been found to increase the risk for cardiovascular disease.10,57,58 The association between BMI and AD risk is complex, with higher BMI in midlife associated with late-life dementia risk but lower BMI in late life associated with elevated Aβ.59 Thus, further work is needed to evaluate the associations among BMI, early-onset AD, and sleep pattern across the life span.

Third, participants in both short and long sleep duration groups reported napping longer during the day. Napping behaviors in aging are complex but have been established as a risk factor in future decline, independent of nighttime sleep,60 which is consistent with a finding in the present study that nighttime and daytime sleep independently relate to some measures of cognition. In this study, the U-shaped association between nocturnal sleep duration and daytime napping suggests that individuals with short sleep duration may compensate with napping, whereas those with long sleep duration may experience excessive sleepiness throughout the day. Analyses that included an interaction term between sleep duration and daytime nap duration found that daytime napping was an independent factor in several models (ie, FCSRT free recall, DSST, and CFI), suggesting that daytime napping is independently associated with these outcomes in addition to nocturnal sleep duration. In models that included multiple lifestyle factors (ie, BMI, GDS, and daytime napping) as covariates, these variables were generally not associated with Aβ or cognition. The exception was the DSST, with which nocturnal sleep was no longer significantly associated (although a pattern of continuous sleep duration was found); instead, all lifestyle variables showed significant associations with this cognitive measure. This finding implies that multiple factors (eg, depression, BMI, and daytime napping) may be independently associated with worse executive function and may have an upstream physiological basis, such as cardiovascular health, that was not measured in the current study.

Limitations

This study has some limitations. First, the study lacked data on sleep-disordered breathing, which is a risk factor for cognitive decline61,62 and elevated Aβ63-66 but is typically not associated with self-reported sleep duration.67 We also did not have data on use of medications that might affect sleep duration and could be associated with worse cognitive function.7 Second, we used self-reported sleep duration information rather than sleep data from objective measures, such as actigraphy or polysomnography. Subjective (self-reported) and objective sleep times differ and may represent distinct signals of an individual’s sleep health.3,68 Comparisons of these objective and subjective measures of sleep in older adults have shown that these measures are typically not correlated,69-71 suggesting that self-reported measures may carry information that is different from that in objective measures.72

Third, the cross-sectional design of this study did not allow us to establish the direction of associations between sleep and the outcome variables, which is likely bidirectional. Fourth, the modest effect sizes of some of the associations with self-reported sleep duration limited the clinical implications of these findings.

Conclusions

Findings from this cross-sectional study show that short and long sleep duration were both associated with worse outcomes in older adults with normal cognition; the specific differences between sleep duration phenotypes suggest their distinct physiological bases. The association between sleep duration and multiple intersecting health outcomes, such as greater Aβ burden, greater depressive symptoms, higher BMI, and cognitive decline, highlight the importance of maintaining adequate sleep in late life.

Back to top
Article Information

Accepted for Publication: July 8, 2021.

Published Online: August 30, 2021. doi:10.1001/jamaneurol.2021.2876

Corresponding Author: Joseph R. Winer, PhD, Department of Neurology and Neurological Sciences, Stanford University School of Medicine, 453 Quarry Road, Palo Alto, CA 94304 (jwiner@stanford.edu).

Author Contributions: Dr Winer had full access to all of the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.

Concept and design: Winer, Mormino.

Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: All authors.

Drafting of the manuscript: Winer, Goldstein-Piekarski, Mormino.

Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Winer, Deters, Kennedy, Jin, Poston.

Statistical analysis: Winer, Jin, Mormino.

Obtained funding: Poston, Mormino.

Administrative, technical, or material support: Kennedy, Goldstein-Piekarski.

Supervision: Poston, Mormino.

Conflict of Interest Disclosures: Dr Poston reported receiving grants from the National Institutes of Health (NIH) during the conduct of the study; grants from NIH and Michael J. Fox Foundation for Parkinson's Research outside the submitted work; and consulting fees from CuraSen Therapeutics Inc. Dr Mormino reported receiving grants from NIH, personal fees from Roche and Lilly during the conduct of the study. No other disclosures were reported.

Funding/Support: This study was supported by grants P30AG066515 (Drs Winer, Poston, and Mormino) and K01AG051718 (Dr Mormino) from the National Institutes on Aging.

Role of the Funder/Sponsor: The funder had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

References
1.
Mander  BA, Winer  JR, Walker  MP.  Sleep and human aging.   Neuron. 2017;94(1):19-36. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2017.02.004 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
Ohayon  MM, Carskadon  MA, Guilleminault  C, Vitiello  MV.  Meta-analysis of quantitative sleep parameters from childhood to old age in healthy individuals: developing normative sleep values across the human lifespan.   Sleep. 2004;27(7):1255-1273. doi:10.1093/sleep/27.7.1255 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
Kocevska  D, Lysen  TS, Dotinga  A,  et al.  Sleep characteristics across the lifespan in 1.1 million people from the Netherlands, United Kingdom and United States: a systematic review and meta-analysis.   Nat Hum Behav. 2021;5(1):113-122. doi:10.1038/s41562-020-00965-x PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Franzen  PL, Buysse  DJ.  Sleep disturbances and depression: risk relationships for subsequent depression and therapeutic implications.   Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2008;10(4):473-481. doi:10.31887/DCNS.2008.10.4/plfranzen PubMedGoogle Scholar
5.
Potvin  O, Lorrain  D, Belleville  G, Grenier  S, Préville  M.  Subjective sleep characteristics associated with anxiety and depression in older adults: a population-based study.   Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2014;29(12):1262-1270. doi:10.1002/gps.4106 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Devore  EE, Grodstein  F, Duffy  JF, Stampfer  MJ, Czeisler  CA, Schernhammer  ES.  Sleep duration in midlife and later life in relation to cognition.   J Am Geriatr Soc. 2014;62(6):1073-1081. doi:10.1111/jgs.12790 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Virta  JJ, Heikkilä  K, Perola  M,  et al.  Midlife sleep characteristics associated with late life cognitive function.   Sleep. 2013;36(10):1533-1541, 1541A. doi:10.5665/sleep.3052PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Lim  ASP, Yu  L, Kowgier  M, Schneider  JA, Buchman  AS, Bennett  DA.  Modification of the relationship of the apolipoprotein E ε4 allele to the risk of Alzheimer disease and neurofibrillary tangle density by sleep.   JAMA Neurol. 2013;70(12):1544-1551. doi:10.1001/jamaneurol.2013.4215 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Lysen  TS, Luik  AI, Ikram  MK, Tiemeier  H, Ikram  MA.  Actigraphy-estimated sleep and 24-hour activity rhythms and the risk of dementia.   Alzheimers Dement. 2020;16(9):1259-1267. doi:10.1002/alz.12122 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Cappuccio  FP, Cooper  D, D’Elia  L, Strazzullo  P, Miller  MA.  Sleep duration predicts cardiovascular outcomes: a systematic review and meta-analysis of prospective studies.   Eur Heart J. 2011;32(12):1484-1492. doi:10.1093/eurheartj/ehr007 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
Cappuccio  FP, D’Elia  L, Strazzullo  P, Miller  MA.  Quantity and quality of sleep and incidence of type 2 diabetes: a systematic review and meta-analysis.   Diabetes Care. 2010;33(2):414-420. doi:10.2337/dc09-1124 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Hirshkowitz  M, Whiton  K, Albert  SM,  et al.  National Sleep Foundation’s updated sleep duration recommendations: final report.   Sleep Health. 2015;1(4):233-243. doi:10.1016/j.sleh.2015.10.004 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Lo  JC, Groeger  JA, Cheng  GH, Dijk  D-J, Chee  MWL.  Self-reported sleep duration and cognitive performance in older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis.   Sleep Med. 2016;17:87-98. doi:10.1016/j.sleep.2015.08.021 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
Ma  Y, Liang  L, Zheng  F, Shi  L, Zhong  B, Xie  W.  Association between sleep duration and cognitive decline.   JAMA Netw Open. 2020;3(9):e2013573. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2020.13573 PubMedGoogle Scholar
15.
Spira  AP, Gamaldo  AA, An  Y,  et al.  Self-reported sleep and β-amyloid deposition in community-dwelling older adults.   JAMA Neurol. 2013;70(12):1537-1543. doi:10.1001/jamaneurol.2013.4258 PubMedGoogle Scholar
16.
Jack  CR  Jr, Bennett  DA, Blennow  K,  et al.  NIA-AA research framework: toward a biological definition of Alzheimer’s disease.   Alzheimers Dement. 2018;14(4):535-562. doi:10.1016/j.jalz.2018.02.018 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
Jagust  W.  Imaging the evolution and pathophysiology of Alzheimer disease.   Nat Rev Neurosci. 2018;19(11):687-700. doi:10.1038/s41583-018-0067-3 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
Sperling  RA, Rentz  DM, Johnson  KA,  et al.  The A4 study: stopping AD before symptoms begin?   Sci Transl Med. 2014;6(228):228fs13. doi:10.1126/scitranslmed.3007941 PubMedGoogle Scholar
19.
Sperling  RA, Donohue  MC, Raman  R,  et al; A4 Study Team.  Association of factors with elevated amyloid burden in clinically normal older individuals.   JAMA Neurol. 2020;77(6):735-745. doi:10.1001/jamaneurol.2020.0387 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Deters  KD, Napolioni  V, Sperling  RA,  et al.  Amyloid PET imaging in self-identified non-Hispanic Black participants of the anti-amyloid in asymptomatic Alzheimer’s disease (A4) study.   Neurology. 2021;96(11):e1491-e1500. doi:10.1212/WNL.0000000000011599 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
Kingsbury  JH, Buxton  OM, Emmons  KM.  Sleep and its relationship to racial and ethnic disparities in cardiovascular disease.   Curr Cardiovasc Risk Rep. 2013;7(5). doi:10.1007/s12170-013-0330-0 PubMedGoogle Scholar
22.
Carnethon  MR, De Chavez  PJ, Zee  PC,  et al.  Disparities in sleep characteristics by race/ethnicity in a population-based sample: Chicago Area Sleep Study.   Sleep Med. 2016;18:50-55. doi:10.1016/j.sleep.2015.07.005 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
23.
Donohue  MC, Sperling  RA, Salmon  DP,  et al; Australian Imaging, Biomarkers, and Lifestyle Flagship Study of Ageing; Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative; Alzheimer’s Disease Cooperative Study.  The preclinical Alzheimer cognitive composite: measuring amyloid-related decline.   JAMA Neurol. 2014;71(8):961-970. doi:10.1001/jamaneurol.2014.803 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Mormino  EC, Papp  KV, Rentz  DM,  et al.  Early and late change on the preclinical Alzheimer’s cognitive composite in clinically normal older individuals with elevated amyloid β.   Alzheimers Dement. 2017;13(9):1004-1012. doi:10.1016/j.jalz.2017.01.018 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
Grober  E, Sanders  AE, Hall  C, Lipton  RB.  Free and cued selective reminding identifies very mild dementia in primary care.   Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2010;24(3):284-290. doi:10.1097/WAD.0b013e3181cfc78b PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
Amariglio  RE, Donohue  MC, Marshall  GA,  et al; Alzheimer’s Disease Cooperative Study.  Tracking early decline in cognitive function in older individuals at risk for Alzheimer disease dementia: the Alzheimer’s Disease Cooperative Study Cognitive Function Instrument.   JAMA Neurol. 2015;72(4):446-454. doi:10.1001/jamaneurol.2014.3375 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
27.
Kronholm  E, Sallinen  M, Suutama  T, Sulkava  R, Era  P, Partonen  T.  Self-reported sleep duration and cognitive functioning in the general population.   J Sleep Res. 2009;18(4):436-446. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2869.2009.00765.x PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
28.
Papp  KV, Rentz  DM, Mormino  EC,  et al.  Cued memory decline in biomarker-defined preclinical Alzheimer disease.   Neurology. 2017;88(15):1431-1438. doi:10.1212/WNL.0000000000003812 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
29.
Kang  J-E, Lim  MM, Bateman  RJ,  et al.  Amyloid-beta dynamics are regulated by orexin and the sleep-wake cycle.   Science. 2009;326(5955):1005-1007. doi:10.1126/science.1180962 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
30.
Roh  JH, Huang  Y, Bero  AW,  et al.  Disruption of the sleep-wake cycle and diurnal fluctuation of β-amyloid in mice with Alzheimer’s disease pathology.   Sci Transl Med. 2012;4(150):150ra122. doi:10.1126/scitranslmed.3004291 PubMedGoogle Scholar
31.
Lucey  BP, Hicks  TJ, McLeland  JS,  et al.  Effect of sleep on overnight cerebrospinal fluid amyloid β kinetics.   Ann Neurol. 2018;83(1):197-204. doi:10.1002/ana.25117 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
32.
Xie  L, Kang  H, Xu  Q,  et al.  Sleep drives metabolite clearance from the adult brain.   Science. 2013;342(6156):373-377. doi:10.1126/science.1241224 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
33.
Hablitz  LM, Plá  V, Giannetto  M,  et al.  Circadian control of brain glymphatic and lymphatic fluid flow.   Nat Commun. 2020;11(1):4411. doi:10.1038/s41467-020-18115-2 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
34.
Fultz  NE, Bonmassar  G, Setsompop  K,  et al.  Coupled electrophysiological, hemodynamic, and cerebrospinal fluid oscillations in human sleep.   Science. 2019;366(6465):628-631. doi:10.1126/science.aax5440 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
35.
Branger  P, Arenaza-Urquijo  EM, Tomadesso  C,  et al.  Relationships between sleep quality and brain volume, metabolism, and amyloid deposition in late adulthood.   Neurobiol Aging. 2016;41:107-114. doi:10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2016.02.009 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
36.
Sprecher  KE, Koscik  RL, Carlsson  CM,  et al.  Poor sleep is associated with CSF biomarkers of amyloid pathology in cognitively normal adults.   Neurology. 2017;89(5):445-453. doi:10.1212/WNL.0000000000004171 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
37.
Ju  YE-S, McLeland  JS, Toedebusch  CD,  et al.  Sleep quality and preclinical Alzheimer disease.   JAMA Neurol. 2013;70(5):587-593. doi:10.1001/jamaneurol.2013.2334 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
38.
Ettore  E, Bakardjian  H, Solé  M,  et al.  Relationships between objectives sleep parameters and brain amyloid load in subjects at risk for Alzheimer’s disease: the INSIGHT-preAD Study.   Sleep. 2019;42(9):zsz137. doi:10.1093/sleep/zsz137 PubMedGoogle Scholar
39.
Mander  BA, Marks  SM, Vogel  JW,  et al.  β-amyloid disrupts human NREM slow waves and related hippocampus-dependent memory consolidation.   Nat Neurosci. 2015;18(7):1051-1057. doi:10.1038/nn.4035 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
40.
Varga  AW, Wohlleber  ME, Giménez  S,  et al.  Reduced slow-wave sleep is associated with high cerebrospinal fluid Aβ42 levels in cognitively normal elderly.   Sleep. 2016;39(11):2041-2048. doi:10.5665/sleep.6240 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
41.
Winer  JR, Mander  BA, Kumar  S,  et al.  Sleep disturbance forecasts β-amyloid accumulation across subsequent years.   Curr Biol. 2020;30(21):4291-4298.e3. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2020.08.017 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
42.
Mander  BA, Winer  JR, Jagust  WJ, Walker  MP.  Sleep: a novel mechanistic pathway, biomarker, and treatment target in the pathology of Alzheimer’s disease?   Trends Neurosci. 2016;39(8):552-566. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2016.05.002 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
43.
Wang  C, Holtzman  DM.  Bidirectional relationship between sleep and Alzheimer’s disease: role of amyloid, tau, and other factors.   Neuropsychopharmacology. 2020;45(1):104-120. doi:10.1038/s41386-019-0478-5 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
44.
Grandner  MA, Drummond  SPA.  Who are the long sleepers? Towards an understanding of the mortality relationship.   Sleep Med Rev. 2007;11(5):341-360. doi:10.1016/j.smrv.2007.03.010 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
45.
Benito-León  J, Louis  ED, Bermejo-Pareja  F.  Cognitive decline in short and long sleepers: a prospective population-based study (NEDICES).   J Psychiatr Res. 2013;47(12):1998-2003. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychires.2013.09.007 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
46.
Yaffe  K, Falvey  CM, Hoang  T.  Connections between sleep and cognition in older adults.   Lancet Neurol. 2014;13(10):1017-1028. doi:10.1016/S1474-4422(14)70172-3 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
47.
Lemos  R, Duro  D, Simões  MR, Santana  I.  The free and cued selective reminding test distinguishes frontotemporal dementia from Alzheimer’s disease.   Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2014;29(7):670-679. doi:10.1093/arclin/acu031 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
48.
Nicolazzo  J, Xu  K, Lavale  A,  et al.  Sleep symptomatology is associated with greater subjective cognitive concerns: findings from the community-based Healthy Brain Project.   Sleep. 2021;zsab097. doi:10.1093/sleep/zsab097 PubMedGoogle Scholar
49.
Leng  Y, Wainwright  NWJ, Cappuccio  FP,  et al.  Self-reported sleep patterns in a British population cohort.   Sleep Med. 2014;15(3):295-302. doi:10.1016/j.sleep.2013.10.015 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
50.
George  KM, Peterson  RL, Gilsanz  P,  et al.  Racial/ethnic differences in sleep quality among older adults: Kaiser Healthy Aging and Diverse Life Experiences (KHANDLE) study.   Ethn Dis. 2020;30(3):469-478. doi:10.18865/ed.30.3.469 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
51.
Grandner  MA, Williams  NJ, Knutson  KL, Roberts  D, Jean-Louis  G.  Sleep disparity, race/ethnicity, and socioeconomic position.   Sleep Med. 2016;18:7-18. doi:10.1016/j.sleep.2015.01.020 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
52.
Stamatakis  KA, Kaplan  GA, Roberts  RE.  Short sleep duration across income, education, and race/ethnic groups: population prevalence and growing disparities during 34 years of follow-up.   Ann Epidemiol. 2007;17(12):948-955. doi:10.1016/j.annepidem.2007.07.096 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
53.
Whinnery  J, Jackson  N, Rattanaumpawan  P, Grandner  MA.  Short and long sleep duration associated with race/ethnicity, sociodemographics, and socioeconomic position.   Sleep. 2014;37(3):601-611. doi:10.5665/sleep.3508 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
54.
Slopen  N, Williams  DR.  Discrimination, other psychosocial stressors, and self-reported sleep duration and difficulties.   Sleep. 2014;37(1):147-156. doi:10.5665/sleep.3326 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
55.
Thomas  KS, Bardwell  WA, Ancoli-Israel  S, Dimsdale  JE.  The toll of ethnic discrimination on sleep architecture and fatigue.   Health Psychol. 2006;25(5):635-642. doi:10.1037/0278-6133.25.5.635 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
56.
Beatty  DL, Hall  MH, Kamarck  TA,  et al.  Unfair treatment is associated with poor sleep in African American and Caucasian adults: Pittsburgh SleepSCORE project.   Health Psychol. 2011;30(3):351-359. doi:10.1037/a0022976 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
57.
Altman  NG, Izci-Balserak  B, Schopfer  E,  et al.  Sleep duration versus sleep insufficiency as predictors of cardiometabolic health outcomes.   Sleep Med. 2012;13(10):1261-1270. doi:10.1016/j.sleep.2012.08.005 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
58.
Knutson  KL.  Sleep duration and cardiometabolic risk: a review of the epidemiologic evidence.   Best Pract Res Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2010;24(5):731-743. doi:10.1016/j.beem.2010.07.001 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
59.
Hsu  DC, Mormino  EC, Schultz  AP,  et al; Harvard Aging Brain Study.  Lower late-life body-mass index is associated with higher cortical amyloid burden in clinically normal elderly.   J Alzheimers Dis. 2016;53(3):1097-1105. doi:10.3233/JAD-150987 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
60.
Leng  Y, Redline  S, Stone  KL, Ancoli-Israel  S, Yaffe  K.  Objective napping, cognitive decline, and risk of cognitive impairment in older men.   Alzheimers Dement. 2019;15(8):1039-1047. doi:10.1016/j.jalz.2019.04.009 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
61.
Yaffe  K, Laffan  AM, Harrison  SL,  et al.  Sleep-disordered breathing, hypoxia, and risk of mild cognitive impairment and dementia in older women.   JAMA. 2011;306(6):613-619. doi:10.1001/jama.2011.1115 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
62.
Osorio  RS, Gumb  T, Pirraglia  E,  et al; Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative.  Sleep-disordered breathing advances cognitive decline in the elderly.   Neurology. 2015;84(19):1964-1971. doi:10.1212/WNL.0000000000001566 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
63.
Ju  Y-ES, Finn  MB, Sutphen  CL,  et al.  Obstructive sleep apnea decreases central nervous system-derived proteins in the cerebrospinal fluid.   Ann Neurol. 2016;80(1):154-159. doi:10.1002/ana.24672 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
64.
Sharma  RA, Varga  AW, Bubu  OM,  et al.  Obstructive sleep apnea severity affects amyloid burden in cognitively normal elderly. A longitudinal study.   Am J Respir Crit Care Med. 2018;197(7):933-943. doi:10.1164/rccm.201704-0704OC PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
65.
André  C, Rehel  S, Kuhn  E,  et al; Medit-Ageing Research Group.  Association of sleep-disordered breathing with Alzheimer disease biomarkers in community-dwelling older adults: a secondary analysis of a randomized clinical trial.   JAMA Neurol. 2020;77(6):716-724. doi:10.1001/jamaneurol.2020.0311 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
66.
Bubu  OM, Pirraglia  E, Andrade  AG,  et al; Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative.  Obstructive sleep apnea and longitudinal Alzheimer’s disease biomarker changes.   Sleep. 2019;42(6):zsz048. doi:10.1093/sleep/zsz048 PubMedGoogle Scholar
67.
Duarte  RLM, Mendes  BA, Oliveira-E-Sá  TS, Magalhães-da-Silveira  FJ, Gozal  D.  Perception of sleep duration in adult patients with suspected obstructive sleep apnea.   PLoS One. 2020;15(8):e0238083. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0238083 PubMedGoogle Scholar
68.
Lauderdale  DS, Knutson  KL, Yan  LL, Liu  K, Rathouz  PJ.  Self-reported and measured sleep duration: how similar are they?   Epidemiology. 2008;19(6):838-845. doi:10.1097/EDE.0b013e318187a7b0 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
69.
Landry  GJ, Best  JR, Liu-Ambrose  T.  Measuring sleep quality in older adults: a comparison using subjective and objective methods.   Front Aging Neurosci. 2015;7:166. doi:10.3389/fnagi.2015.00166 PubMedGoogle Scholar
70.
Matthews  KA, Patel  SR, Pantesco  EJ,  et al.  Similarities and differences in estimates of sleep duration by polysomnography, actigraphy, diary, and self-reported habitual sleep in a community sample.   Sleep Health. 2018;4(1):96-103. doi:10.1016/j.sleh.2017.10.011 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
71.
Kaplan  KA, Hardas  PP, Redline  S, Zeitzer  JM; Sleep Heart Health Study Research Group.  Correlates of sleep quality in midlife and beyond: a machine learning analysis.   Sleep Med. 2017;34:162-167. doi:10.1016/j.sleep.2017.03.004 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
72.
Buysse  DJ.  Sleep health: can we define it? Does it matter?   Sleep. 2014;37(1):9-17. doi:10.5665/sleep.3298 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
×