[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Table 1. 
Characteristics of Patients With PD, MSA, and PSP and Control Subjects*
Characteristics of Patients With PD, MSA, and PSP and Control Subjects*
Table 2. 
Cognitive Test Results in Patients With PD, MSA, and PSP and Control Subjects*
Cognitive Test Results in Patients With PD, MSA, and PSP and Control Subjects*
Table 3. 
Tapping Test in Patients With PD, MSA, and PSP and Control Subjects*
Tapping Test in Patients With PD, MSA, and PSP and Control Subjects*
Table 4. 
Ideomotor Apraxia in Patients With PD, MSA, and PSP and Control Subjects*
Ideomotor Apraxia in Patients With PD, MSA, and PSP and Control Subjects*
Table 5. 
Discriminant Analysis: Standardized Canonical Coefficients of Patient Groups vs Control Subjects for 11 Cognitive and Motor Tests*
Discriminant Analysis: Standardized Canonical Coefficients of Patient Groups vs Control Subjects for 11 Cognitive and Motor Tests*
1.
Hughes  AJDaniel  SEKilford  LLees  AJ Accuracy of clinical diagnosis of idiopathic Parkinson's disease: a clinico-pathological study of 100 cases.  J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1992;55181- 184Google ScholarCrossref
2.
Litvan  IAgid  YJankovic  J  et al.  Accuracy of clinical criteria for the diagnosis of progressive supranuclear palsy (Steele-Richardson-Olszewski syndrome).  Neurology. 1996;46922- 930Google ScholarCrossref
3.
Pillon  BDubois  BAgid  Y Testing cognition may contribute to the diagnosis of movement disorders.  Neurology. 1996;46329- 334Google ScholarCrossref
4.
Brown  RGMarsden  CD ‘Subcortical dementia': the neuropsychological evidence.  Neuroscience. 1988;25363- 387Google ScholarCrossref
5.
Lezak  MD Executive functions and motor performance.  Neuropsychological Assessment. New York, NY Oxford University Press1983;507- 532Google Scholar
6.
Pillon  BGouider-Khouja  NDeweer  B  et al.  Neuropsychological pattern of striatonigral degeneration: comparison with Parkinson's disease and progressive supranuclear palsy.  J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1995;58174- 179Google ScholarCrossref
7.
Robbins  TWJames  MOwen  AM  et al.  Cognitive deficits in progressive supranuclear palsy, Parkinson's disease, and multiple system atrophy in tests sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction.  J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1994;5779- 88Google ScholarCrossref
8.
Alexander  GEDelong  MRStrick  PL Parallel organization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex.  Annu Rev Neurosci. 1986;9357- 381Google ScholarCrossref
9.
Marsden  CDDeecke  LFreund  HJ  et al.  The functions of the supplementary motor area: summary of a workshop. Lüders  HO Advances in Neurology, Vol 70 Supplementary Sensorimotor Area. Philadelphia, Pa Lippincott-Raven Publishers1996;477- 487Google Scholar
10.
Heilman  KMRothi  LJG Apraxia. Heilman  KMValenstein  E Clinical Neuropsychology. 2nd ed. New York, NY Oxford University Press1985;131- 150Google Scholar
11.
Watson  RTShepherd Fleet  WGonzales-Rothi  LHeilman  KM Apraxia and the supplementary motor area.  Arch Neurol. 1986;43787- 792Google ScholarCrossref
12.
Pramstaller  PPMarsden  CD The basal ganglia and apraxia.  Brain. 1996;119319- 340Google ScholarCrossref
13.
Hoehn  MMYahr  MD Parkinsonism: onset, progression and mortality.  Neurology. 1967;17427- 442Google ScholarCrossref
14.
American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition.  Washington, DC American Psychiatric Association1994;
15.
Folstein  MFFolstein  SEMcHugh  PR Mini-mental state: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician.  J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12189- 198Google ScholarCrossref
16.
Meazzo  GCavarzeran  FZappalà  G  et al.  The Mini-Mental State Examination normative study of an Italian random sample.  Dev Neuropsychol. 1993;977- 85Google ScholarCrossref
17.
Quinn  N Multiple system atrophy—the nature of the beast.  J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1989;52(special suppl)78- 89Google ScholarCrossref
18.
Testa  DSavoiardo  MFetoni  V  et al.  Multiple system atrophy: clinical and MR observations on 42 cases.  Ital J Neurol Sci. 1993;14211- 216Google ScholarCrossref
19.
Litvan  IAgid  YCalne  D  et al.  Clinical research criteria for the diagnosis of progressive supranuclear palsy (Steele-Richardson-Olszewski syndrome): report of the NINDS-SPSP International Workshop.  Neurology. 1996;471- 9Google ScholarCrossref
20.
Savoiardo  MGirotti  FStrada  LCiceri  E Magnetic resonance imaging in progressive supranuclear palsy and other parkinsonian disorders.  J Neural Transm Suppl. 1994;4293- 110Google Scholar
21.
Raven  JC Guide to Using the Colored Progressive Matrices.  London, England HK Lewis1965; New York, NY Psychological Corpno date.
22.
Randt  CTBrown  EROsborne  DP A memory test for longitudinal measurement of mild to moderate deficits.  Clin Neuropsychol. 1980;2184- 194Google Scholar
23.
Novelli  GPapagno  CCapitani  ELaiacona  MVallar  GCappa  SF Tre test clinici di ricerca e produzione lessicale: taratura su soggetti normali.  Arch Psicol Neurol Psichiatria. 1986;47477- 506Google Scholar
24.
Benton  ALVarney  NRHamsher de S  K Visuo-spatial judgment: a clinical test.  Arch Neurol. 1978;35364- 367Google ScholarCrossref
25.
Nelson  HE Modified card sorting test sensitive to frontal lobe defects.  Cortex. 1976;12313- 324Google ScholarCrossref
26.
Basso  ACapitani  ELaiacona  M Raven's coloured progressive matrices: normative values on 305 adult normal controls.  Funct Neurol. 1987;2189- 194Google Scholar
27.
Spinnler  HTognoni  G Gruppo italiano per lo studio neuropsicologico dell'invecchiamento: standardizzazione e taratura italiana di test neuropsicologici.  Ital J Neurol Sci. 1987;(suppl 8)1- 120Google Scholar
28.
De Renzi  EMotti  FNichelli  P Imitating gestures: a quantitative approach to ideomotor apraxia.  Arch Neurol. 1980;376- 10Google ScholarCrossref
29.
Oldfield  RC The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh Inventory.  Neuropsychologia. 1971;997- 113Google ScholarCrossref
30.
SPSS Inc, SPSS-X (Statistical Package for the Social Sciences, Version 10).  New York, NY McGraw-Hill Book Co Inc1983;
31.
Rencher  AC Interpretation of canonical discriminant functions, canonical variates, and principal components.  Am Statistician. 1992;46217- 225Google Scholar
32.
Robbins  TWJames  MLange  KWOwen  AMQuinn  NPMarsden  CD Cognitive performance in multiple system atrophy.  Brain. 1992;115271- 291Google ScholarCrossref
33.
Owen  AMRobbins  TW Comparative neuropsychology of parkinsonian syndromes. Wolters  EC Mental Dysfunction in Parkinson's Disease. Dordrecht, the Netherlands ICG Printing1993;221- 241Google Scholar
34.
Testa  DFetoni  VSoliveri  PMusicco  MPalazzini  EGirotti  F Cognitive and motor performance in multiple system atrophy and Parkinson's disease compared.  Neuropsychologia. 1993;31207- 210Google ScholarCrossref
35.
Daniel  SEBruin  VMSLees  AJ The clinical and pathological spectrum of Steele-Richardson-Olszewski syndrome (progressive supranuclear palsy): a reappraisal.  Brain. 1995;118759- 770Google ScholarCrossref
36.
Kimura  DBarnett  HJMBurkhart  G The psychological test pattern in progressive supranuclear palsy.  Neuropsychologia. 1981;19301- 306Google ScholarCrossref
37.
Benecke  RRothwell  JCDick  JPRDay  BLMarsden  CD Disturbance of sequential movements in patients with Parkinson's disease.  Brain. 1987;110361- 379Google ScholarCrossref
38.
Pillon  BBlin  JVidailhet  M  et al.  The neuropsychological pattern of corticobasal degeneration: comparison with progressive supranuclear palsy and Alzheimer's disease.  Neurology. 1995;451477- 1483Google ScholarCrossref
39.
Cambier  JMasson  MViader  FLimodin  JStrube  A Le syndrome frontal de la paralysie supranucléaire progressive.  Rev Neurol. 1985;141528- 536Google Scholar
40.
Freund  HJ The apraxias. Asbury  AKMcKhann  GMMcDonald  WJ Diseases of the Nervous System Clinical Neurobiology. 2nd ed. Philadelphia, Pa WB Saunders Co1992;751- 767Google Scholar
41.
Heilman  KMRothi  LJGValenstein  E Two forms of ideomotor apraxia.  Neurology. 1982;32342- 346Google ScholarCrossref
42.
Goldenberg  GWimmer  AAuff  ESchnaberth  G Impairment of motor planning in patients with Parkinson's disease: evidence from ideomotor apraxia testing.  J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1986;491266- 1272Google ScholarCrossref
43.
Leiguarda  RCPramstaller  PPMerello  MStarkstein  SLees  AJMarsden  CD Apraxia in Parkinson's disease, progressive supranuclear palsy, multiple system atrophy, and neuroleptic-induced parkinsonism.  Brain. 1997;12075- 90Google ScholarCrossref
44.
Schneider  JAWatts  RLGearing  MBrewer  RPMirra  SS Corticobasal degeneration: neuropathologic and clinical heterogeneity.  Neurology. 1997;48959- 969Google ScholarCrossref
45.
Grisoli  MFetoni  VSavoiardo  MGirotti  FBruzzone  MG MRI in corticobasal degeneration.  Eur J Neurol. 1995;2547- 552Google ScholarCrossref
46.
Lantos  PL The neuropathology of progressive supranuclear palsy.  J Neural Transm Suppl. 1994;42137- 152Google Scholar
47.
Verny  MDuyckaerts  CAgid  YHauw  JJ The significance of cortical pathology in progressive supranuclear palsy: clinico-pathological data in 10 cases.  Brain. 1996;1191123- 1136Google ScholarCrossref
48.
Brooks  DJ PET studies in progressive supranuclear palsy.  J Neural Transm Suppl. 1994;42119- 134Google Scholar
49.
Papp  MILantos  PL The distribution of oligodendroglial inclusions in multiple system atrophy and its relevance to clinical symptomatology.  Brain. 1994;117235- 243Google ScholarCrossref
50.
Alexander  GECrutcher  MD Functional architecture of basal ganglia circuit: neural substrate of parallel processing.  Trends Neurosci. 1990;13266- 271Google ScholarCrossref
Original Contribution
March 1998

Cognitive Dysfunction and Impaired Organization of Complex Motility in Degenerative Parkinsonian Syndromes

Author Affiliations

From the National Institute of Neurology (Drs Monza, Soliveri, Fetoni, Testa, Caraceni, and Girotti) and the Institute of Advanced Biomedical Technologies (Dr Radice), Milan, and the Institute of Neurology, University of Parma, Parma (Dr Caffarra), Italy.

Arch Neurol. 1998;55(3):372-378. doi:10.1001/archneur.55.3.372
Abstract

Background  A frontostriatal pattern of cognitive decline, consisting of a frontal lobe–like syndrome without genuine cortical defects such as amnesia, apraxia, aphasia, or agnosia, is well established in basal ganglial diseases. Recent pathological investigations, however, have again noted cortical damage in progressive supranuclear palsy (PSP), suggesting that cortical defects could be present.

Objectives  To delineate the pattern of cognitive impairment and to detect higher-order motor impairments (including ideomotor apraxia) in parkinsonian syndromes.

Patients and Methods  We assessed ideomotor apraxia, and simple and sequential tapping in patients with Parkinson disease, multiple system atrophy, and PSP with similar disease severity, age range, and education. We also administered a comprehensive battery of neuropsychological tests to examine general intelligence, memory, executive functions, attention, and visuospatial orientation. The results were compared between groups and with a matched normal control group.

Results  Sequential tapping and the imitation of sequences of gestures were impaired in all patient groups, with patients with PSP performing worse than the other groups. Based on ideomotor apraxia scores and a qualitative analysis of errors, 3 patients with PSP and 2 with multiple system atrophy were considered apraxic. General intelligence and executive functions were compromised in all patient groups. The impairment of patients with PSP was more pervasive than that of the other groups, and included compromise of visuospatial functions, attention, and memory. Discriminant analysis of all cognitive and motor tests showed that the tapping and ideomotor apraxia tests best identified the patients vs control subjects.

Conclusions  The presence of cortical as well as subcortical damage in patients with PSP and those with multiple system atrophy is indicated by the presence of pervasive cognitive and motor disturbances in the former, substantial motor disorganization in the latter, and the finding of ideomotor apraxia in some patients with these diseases. Furthermore, the discovery that tests of motor and gesture best identified all patients vs control subjects is consistent with the existence of a common motor disorganization in these parkinsonian syndromes, in agreement with the known damage to the corticostriatal pathways in these conditions.

THE FULLY developed clinical pictures of Parkinson disease (PD), multiple system atrophy (MSA), and progressive supranuclear palsy (PSP) are usually distinctive, but uncertainties may arise at disease onset, and misdiagnoses are often reported in clinicopathological series.1,2 According to a recent review,3 neuropsychological testing may assist the clinical diagnosis of these diseases because PD and MSA are characterized by the absence of dementia, despite planning and recall deficits in the early stage of the diseases, whereas more severe disturbances of memory, executive functions, and behavior, often amounting to frank dementia, are present relatively early in PSP.

Following criticisms in the mid-1980s4 of the concept of subcortical dementia as applied to basal ganglial diseases, the notion of subcortical frontal decay has been used3 to describe a frontal lobe–like syndrome without genuine amnesia, aphasia, apraxia, or agnosia. A frontal lobe–like syndrome implies a conspicuous impairment of executive functions important for effective, self-sufficient adult behavior, including goal formulation, planning, carrying out goal-directed plans, and performing effectively.5 In patients with PD, MSA, and PSP, impairment of these functions is considered the earliest and most pronounced facet of cognitive deterioration,6,7 with dysfunction of the various striatofrontal circuits being regarded as the anatomical basis of these and the motor function impairments.8

The prefrontal areas of the cortex and their connections with the caudate nucleus are involved in the control of cognition and behavior, while the connections between the striatum and the premotor areas, especially the supplementary motor area (SMA), are implicated in the control of complex motility, particularly when guided by internal cues.9

Because of striatal dysfunction, prefrontal and premotor areas (including the SMA) become deafferented in parkinsonian syndromes. Impairments in the execution of rapid, alternating, and sequential movements, the reproduction of rhythms, and bimanual coordination are found in SMA dysfunction in humans and other primates.9 These findings are not explained by the presence of bradykinesia alone and raise the possibility that there may be a higher-order motor impairment associated with premotor area dysfunction in parkinsonian syndromes. Furthermore, the relationship between these impairments and apraxia has not been explored.

Apraxia is defined as a disorder of skilled movements not caused by weakness, akinesia, deafferentiation, or intellectual deterioration. According to Heilman and Rothi,10 patients with ideomotor apraxia are impaired in the selection, sequence, and spatial orientation of movements involved in gestures (including emblems and pantomimes). Apraxia due to frontal lesions is much less common than that due to parietal lesions, and few cases due to SMA lesions have been described. Watson et al11 studied 2 patients with lesions of the left mesial hemisphere, including the SMA, and found evidence of ideomotor apraxia, particularly for transitive gestures that required complex distal movements of the hands. The authors suggested that the left SMA translates the temporospatial representations of learned transitive movements—that are stored in the left inferior parietal lobe—into motor programs.

The role of the basal ganglia in apraxia also requires clarification. In a review on basal ganglia and apraxia,12 apraxia due solely to basal ganglial lesions was rare; however, it was concluded that the term limb-kinetic apraxia should be used to describe some motor disturbances found in basal ganglial diseases.

In view of these considerations, it appeared to us that the assertion that apraxia was absent in the frontal lobe–like syndromes of parkinsonian disorders should be reconsidered. In this study, therefore, we sought to determine whether ideomotor apraxia is present in parkinsonian syndromes and to reassess the pattern of cognitive impairment to define which cognitive dysfunctions best distinguish patients with these syndromes from normal control subjects. We tested patients with PD, MSA, and PSP compared with a group of normal control subjects using a comprehensive neuropsychological battery, an ideomotor apraxia test evaluating arm and finger gestures, and a computerized tapping test.

Subjects and methods

Nineteen patients with MSA (7 men and 12 women) and 19 patients with PSP (10 men and 9 women) consecutively referred to the Istituto Nazionale Neurologico, Milano, Italy, from the beginning of December 1994 through February 1996 as outpatients or hospitalized patients were considered for the study. We selected 14 patients with PD (7 men and 7 women) matched to those with PSP and with MSA for age, education, and disease severity (Hoehn and Yahr scale at the time of cognitive evaluation)13 from among those being treated at the same hospital. In addition, 18 normal control subjects (4 men and 14 women) were enrolled from patients' relatives and patients with lumbar disk herniation. None of the patients or control subjects had major depression according to criteria of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition (DSM-IV).14 Four patients with PSP were demented according to criteria of the DSM-IV and were excluded. All subjects included in the study had a Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) score15 greater than 24 after adjustment for age and education.16

Parkinson disease was diagnosed by the presence of akinesia plus 1 or more of the following: muscular rigidity, rest tremor, or postural instability. All patients with PD had a progressive disease course, a good response to levodopa therapy, and motor fluctuations as a result of this therapy, characterized by dyskinesias and on-off. None had any of the exclusion criteria for PD established by the Parkinson Disease Society Brain Bank.1

The patients with MSA had probable striatonigral degeneration–type illness, according to Quinn's clinical criteria.17 The clinical diagnosis was supported by magnetic resonance examination, which revealed putamenal signal hypotensity on T2-weighted images, suggesting the deposition of iron or other paramagnetic substances.18 None had magnetic resonance signs of cerebellar or brainstem atrophy.

Progressive supranuclear palsy was diagnosed using the clinical criteria of the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke Society for PSP International Workshop.19 All patients had probable PSP as diagnosed by a gradually progressive course, vertical supranuclear palsy (with down-gaze abnormalities), and prominent postural instability with unexplained falls. In addition, none had the exclusion criteria of Litvan et al,2 including unilateral dystonia, alien-limb syndrome, early cortical dementia, prominent cerebellar signs, dysautonomia, hallucinations, or focal lesions on clinical and radiological examinations. The clinical diagnosis of PSP was supported in 9 patients by magnetic resonance findings that showed atrophy of the midbrain and the region around the third ventricle.20 In the other 5 patients, magnetic resonance imaging showed only moderate and symmetric cerebral atrophy.

All patients with PD were receiving levodopa at a mean (±SD) daily dosage of 1004.8 mg (±369.3 mg); 4 patients were also taking dopamine agonists (1, cabergoline; and 3, bromocriptine mesylate), and 2 patients were taking small doses of anticholinergic agents. Of the 19 patients with MSA, 16 were taking levodopa at a mean (±SD) daily dosage of 754.6 mg (±474.4 mg); 1 of these was also taking cabergoline; another patient was receiving anticholinergic medication only; and 2 were not receiving any medication. Twelve patients with PSP were taking levodopa (mean [±SD] daily dosage, 609.4 [±258.6] mg); 1 was also receiving bromocriptine; and another was also receiving selegiline hydrochloride; 3 patients with PSP were not receiving any medication. Motor and cognitive tests were performed when patients taking levodopa were receiving the greatest motor benefit from this therapy (which, in the case of several patients with MSA and PSP, was minimal).

The neuropsychological battery comprised the MMSE; the Raven Progressive Matrices 47 Test,21 which assesses logical reasoning and visuospatial organization; the Short Tale Test22 for long-term verbal learning; the Phonemic Verbal Fluency Test,23 which evaluates verbal fluency and strategic ability to find words; the Visual Search Test,23 a test of attention and visual scanning; the Visuospatial Orientation Line Test of Benton et al,24 which assesses the visual ability to distinguish the orientation of lines in space; and the Nelson modification25 of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, which examines set-shifting ability.

The Raven test results were adjusted for age and education according to Basso et al26; MMSE scores were adjusted for age and education according to Meazzo et al16; and the other neuropsychological test scores, except those of the Nelson test, were adjusted for age and education according to Spinnler and Tognoni.27

Subjects were administered the De Renzi ideomotor apraxia test,28 which consisted of 12 symbolic and 12 nonsymbolic movements of the arm and hand. These were divided arbitrarily into 2 sets, 12 involving single gestures and 12 involving sequences of gestures. All but 2 subjects were right-handed according to the Edinburgh Inventory Test29; patients were asked to execute the movements on imitation with their dominant arm or hand. For each movement, a maximum score of 3 was given, depending on whether the gesture was correctly imitated the first time or needed a second or a third presentation. Four types of errors were recognized and recorded: (1) clumsiness: including delay or inability to initiate gestures, reduced amplitude of movement, and perplexity in executing gestures; (2) sequence errors, including perseverations and mistaken ordering of gestures (the latter applied only to series of gestures); (3) orientation errors, when the gesture was readily recognizable but the limb segments were wrongly oriented in space; and (4) location errors, when the gestures were correct but located to the wrong target. Patients were also asked if they recognized the symbolic gestures and to say what they were.

For the simple tapping test, the number of taps executed during 15 seconds was counted electronically. The index finger of the dominant hand tapped a button of 2 cm in diameter positioned optimally for each subject. For in-sequence tapping, subjects were asked to repeat a previously demonstrated sequence of taps as rapidly as possible during 15 seconds. Two buttons 8 cm apart were used, and the patient had to execute, eg, 2 taps on the first button, followed by 1 tap on the second button. Only the number of taps corresponding to the correct sequence was registered by the recording device.

The Kolmogorov-Smirnov test was applied to the results of all variables to determine whether the values were distributed normally. Variables with normal distributions were analyzed by a 1-way analysis of variance. When analysis of variance revealed significant differences between means, the least significant difference procedure for post hoc comparisons was applied. Variables with non-normal distributions were analyzed by the nonparametric Kruskal-Wallis 1-way analysis of variance. For sequence tapping, simple tapping was the covariate.

Discriminant analysis was performed on the 11 cognitive and motor variables. The ability of each test to discriminate between the control group and individual disease groups was then assessed by the standardized canonical coefficients of the discriminant analysis. All statistical procedures were performed using a statistical software package (Statistical Package for the Social Sciences, version 4).30

Results

The 4 patients with PSP who were excluded from the analysis because of dementia did not differ in age, education, mean disease duration, or disease stage from the other patients with PSP. The 3 groups included in the final analysis consisted of 19 patients with MSA, 15 with PSP, and 14 with PD.

The patient groups and the control groups did not differ in age or education. The duration of illness was significantly greater in patients with PD than in those with MSA or PSP (P<.0001). As expected, illness severity assessed by the Hoehn and Yahr scale did not differ in the 3 groups. Table 1 shows the main characteristics of the 4 groups. The results of the cognitive tests are summarized in Table 2.

Analysis of variance revealed significant differences between the 4 groups in all cognitive tests. Post hoc comparisons showed that the scores of patients with PD were significantly worse than those of control subjects on the Raven, the attention, and the Nelson tests, whereas the scores of patients with MSA and those with PSP were significantly worse than those of control subjects on all cognitive tests. In addition, patients with MSA performed significantly worse than those with PD in the verbal fluency test, whereas patients with PSP performed worse than patients with PD and those with MSA in the verbal fluency, the Visual Search Test, and the Visuospatial Orientation Line Test of Benton; they were worse than patients with MSA but not those with PD on the Nelson test and worse than those with PD but not those with MSA on the Raven test.

Therefore, although no patient was demented according to DSM-IV criteria or had an MMSE score lower than 24, all patient groups performed worse than control subjects in the Raven intelligence test, with patients with PSP being especially compromised.

Considering the results of the tests examining executive functions, patients with MSA did significantly worse than patients with PD in verbal fluency, patients with PD did worse than those with MSA on the Nelson test (although the difference was not significant), and patients with PSP were the worst on both tests.

In the simple tapping task, there was a significant difference between groups (P=.001). Post hoc comparisons showed that patients with MSA and those with PSP were significantly slower than control subjects. Sequential tapping scores were also significantly different between groups (P<.001), with control subjects performing significantly better than the other 3 groups and patients with PD doing significantly better than patients with MSA and those with PSP.

To dissociate the effect of movement slowing (typical of all parkinsonian syndromes) from an impairment of sequencing, the results of sequential tapping were adjusted for simple tapping. The presence of bradykinesia could have affected the performance especially of patients with MSA and those with PSP, who generally responded poorly to levodopa or who were not receiving the drug. After this adjustment, the difference between the 4 groups in sequential tapping was still significant (P=.01), and all disease groups were significantly impaired compared with controls; in addition, patients with MSA and those with PSP performed significantly worse than patients with PD (Table 3). This result implies that the impairment in sequential tapping in patients with PD, MSA, and PSP is not simply the result of bradykinesia but due to an impairment of sequencing capability.

Performance imitating single and sequence of gestures differed significantly between groups (P=.002 and P<.0001, respectively), with all disease groups doing significantly worse than controls, and patients with PSP doing significantly worse than those with MSA or PD (Table 4).

According to De Renzi et al scoring,28 ideomotor apraxia is certainly present when a total score is less than 53 and certainly absent when a total score is greater than 62, with a score between 53 and 62 considered borderline. When the scores were examined individually, all normal subjects scored more than 62; 7 (50%) of 14 patients with PD, 4 (20%) of 19 patients with MSA, and 4 (27%) of 15 patients with PSP scored between 53 and 62 (borderline). None of the patients with PD scored below 53 (apraxia score) but 2 (10%) of 19 patients with MSA and 3 (20%) of 15 patients with PSP scored less than 53. However, all subjects recognized the symbolic gestures even when they could not imitate them correctly. Qualitative analysis of uncorrected gestures showed that all errors made by patients with PD were due to clumsiness. Patients with MSA and PSP often made more than 1 type of error: 85% of the errors made by patients with MSA were due to clumsiness, and 15% were due to sequence errors. Patients with PSP showed greater variability of error types: 50% were sequence errors, 30% were clumsiness errors, 10% were location errors, and 10% were orientation errors.

The classification matrix of the discriminant analysis31 on the 11 variables considered (cognitive and motor tests) showed that the battery of cognitive and motor tests used to identify the disease groups was valid (correct classification: controls, 85%; PSP patients, 89%; MSA patients, 80%; and PD patients, 64%).

Examination of the standardized canonical coefficients for the cognitive and motor test scores (Table 5), arising from a comparison between each patient group and control subjects (PD patients vs controls, MSA patients vs controls, and PSP patients vs controls), showed that in patients with PSP the highest absolute values of the coefficients were for the following tests (in descending order): sequential tapping, sequence of gestures, visual search, and verbal fluency. In patients with MSA, the canonical coefficients for sequential tapping, simple tapping, single gestures, and verbal fluency had the highest values, whereas, in patients with PD, the canonical coefficients for the Nelson test, sequential tapping, visual search, the Short Tale Test, and the Benton test had the highest values.

Comment

Unlike the 2 previous studies that compared cognitive performance in patients with MSA, PD, and PSP and normal control subjects,6,7 our patients with MSA and PSP were not selected for cognitive deterioration, but were referred consecutively to our center as outpatients or inpatients. The patients with PD were then selected as having motor impairment similar to that of patients with MSA and PSP.

In this population with parkinsonian syndromes, we found that patients with MSA and PSP were compromised compared with control subjects in all cognitive functions, ie, abstract reasoning (Raven test), learning and memory (Short Tale Test), visuospatial orientation (Benton test), attention and visuomotor ability (visual search), and executive functions (verbal fluency and Nelson test). The low cognitive test scores in patients with PSP highlight the pervasiveness of cognitive impairment in this unselected sample of patients. The patients with PD were impaired compared with control subjects only in the general intelligence test, the attention test, and the Nelson test.

Our findings on the Nelson and verbal fluency tests reproduce those of a recent article by Pillon et al6 comparing patients with MSA, PD, and PSP and control subjects, and they confirm the presence of frontal dysfunction in basal ganglia diseases. Pillon et al6 concluded that patients with MSA were impaired in spontaneous flexibility, those with PD were impaired in reactive flexibility, and patients with PSP were compromised in both.

A qualitatively different pattern of impairment of executive functions among patients with PD, MSA, and PSP was also found by Robbins et al32 and Owen and Robbins33 but again with more extensive compromise in PSP. Furthermore, Robbins et al7 and Pillon et al6 found that the executive functions that are controlled by the striatofrontal circuits are the earliest and most compromised functions in patients with parkinsonian syndromes.6,7

The cognitive test results in our patients with PD and MSA were similar to those found previously,6,34 whereas the patients with PSP had greater cognitive impairment, which is probably related to the greater striatal damage combined with pervasive pallidal degeneration that characterizes this disease compared with PD and MSA.35

The patients with PSP also performed worse than the other groups on the Benton visuospatial test and the Visual Search Test. The superior colliculi and periaqueductal gray matter are severely damaged in PSP and are likely to be implicated in the pathogenesis of the dysfunction in visual search and scanning ability in PSP, as indicated in early neuropsychological articles on this disease.36

In the simple tapping test, the performance of patients with PSP and MSA, but not of those with PD, was significantly worse than that of control subjects. Although patients with PD were selected as having the same Hoehn and Yahr stage as the other 2 disease groups at the time of testing, patients with PSP and MSA were more bradykinetic because they were not treated with or responded poorly to levodopa. However, when we covaried sequential tapping for simple tapping as a measure of bradykinesia, we still obtained a significant difference between patients with PSP and MSA and control subjects, to the advantage of the control subjects; but this time, patients with PD were also significantly worse than control subjects. Impairment in the execution of motor sequences is well documented in PD. Benecke et al37 found impairments of simultaneous and sequential movements in patients with PD,37 which they attributed to a dysfunction of the SMA. In addition, many reports have emphasized the importance of the SMA in memorizing, generating, and possibly learning sequential movements.9

The 3 disease groups were significantly worse than control subjects in the apraxia test, when imitating both single and sequence gestures, with patients with PSP being especially compromised. The definition of ideomotor apraxia includes the requirement that gesture disorders are not due to akinesia.10 The finding that, in all patients, most of the errors on the test for ideomotor apraxia were due to clumsiness testifies to the difficulty of diagnosing apraxia in patients with basal ganglia diseases. On the other hand, it is noteworthy that patients' performance was particularly bad in the sequence of gestures and that the most common errors of patients with MSA and PSP, after errors due to clumsiness, were sequence defects.

The substantial impairment in the sequence of tapping and gestures in our patients with MSA and PSP is consistent with the results of Pillon et al6,38 and Cambier and colleagues,39 whose patients with PSP were particularly compromised in sequential gestures that were part of a frontal score.

In the study by Benecke et al,37 the longer reaction times in sequential movements for patients with PD included an extra time not explained by the increase in single-movement reaction times. Therefore, sequence impairment does not appear to be explained by bradykinesia alone but seems to be a higher-order motor disturbance possibly due to involvement of the premotor areas.

Freund40 proposed that derangements in the premotor cortex (SMA and lateral premotor cortex) could give rise to what he called "frontal" or "executional" apraxias; these included limb-kinetic apraxia, disorders of motor learning, and rhythm production. We presume that a compromised ability to produce rhythm would also impair sequencing.

In the present study, 3 patients with PSP and 2 with MSA had De Renzi scores in the apraxia range. The errors made by the 2 patients with MSA and 1 patient with PSP were mostly due to clumsiness and sequencing errors, whereas the 2 other patients with PSP also made orientation and localization errors. Because of these orientation and localization errors, we considered the latter group of patients more securely apraxic. Following Freund's paradigm,40 however, the others would also be considered apraxic because when sequencing impairment is so severe as to derange the whole gesture, it is a mark of "true" apraxia.

Two types of ideomotor apraxia have been hypothesized41: a posterior type caused by lesions in the left parietal cortex where visuokinesthetic motor engrams are stored, and an anterior type caused by lesions anterior to the supramarginal gyrus that disconnect motor engrams from the premotor areas. In the former type, patients cannot execute skilled movements on command or imitation and cannot recognize or discriminate gestures because they have lost the representations; in the second type, patients cannot execute gestures on command or imitation but can recognize a motor act and can tell whether it is well performed. All our patients were able to recognize meaningful gestures and, therefore, had the anterior type of ideomotor apraxia, consistent with the known premotor area dysfunction in parkinsonian syndromes.

Goldenberg et al42 found that patients with PD were impaired on apraxia tests, which they attributed to defective motor memory, while a recent report by Leiguarda et al43 found that 13 of 45 patients with PD were apraxic. None of our patients with PD scored in the apraxia range. We propose 2 explanations for this. First, the PD group of Leiguarda et al had a lower mean MMSE score than our PD group and probably included patients with greater mental decay, some of whom may have had borderline dementia; it is well known that apraxia is common in cortical dementing pathologies. Second, the apraxia test used by these investigators43 has a narrow range, with a cutoff for apraxia at 33 and a maximum score of 36 for normal subjects. The De Renzi test of ideomotor apraxia that we used (validated on 200 normal subjects) has a cutoff for apraxia at 53 and a maximum score of 72; borderline apraxia is between 53 and 62, and the normal range is wide. This test is, therefore, more exacting for the diagnosis of apraxia than that of the previous study43; furthermore, it includes a greater number of sequence gestures, which are more sensitive in revealing impairments in patients with MSA and PSP than single gesture tasks.

The finding that our patients with PSP performed poorly on the apraxia test raises the problem of differentiating the diagnosis from corticobasal degeneration in which apraxia is one of the most important signs. A recent clinicopathological article underlined the difficulty of clinically differentiating corticobasal degeneration from PSP and even showed that the 2 conditions could coexist pathologically.44 We selected our PSP patients according to the clinical criteria of Litvan et al,19 which in all cases were correlated with pathological findings; thus, all our PSP patients had supranuclear gaze palsy and frequent falls, and none had an asymmetric akinetic-rigid syndrome. All underwent magnetic resonance imaging, and none displayed asymmetric frontoparietal atrophy characteristic of corticobasal degeneration.45

On the other hand, the presence of apraxia in PSP is consistent with cortical damage (involving the motor and premotor cortex) in this condition, as re-emphasized recently.46,47 In a pathological study of PSP,46 it was noted that neurofibrillary tangles were frequent in the frontal cortex, especially the precentral gyrus, and that density was related to the severity of neuronal loss. These pathological findings are also supported by positron emission tomographic studies showing a symmetric loss of frontal metabolism in PSP.48

Direct cortical involvement has also been found in MSA: the premotor areas show neural and oligodendroglial, basophilic, argyrophil, cytoplasmic inclusions that are relatively specific for this disease and point to a cortical origin of the motor disorganization in MSA.49

Conclusions

The more extensive impairment of cognitive functions in patients with PSP than in those with MSA or PD and the more substantial alteration in motor organization in patients with PSP and MSA compared with patients with PD are not explained by varying extents of frontal deafferentation related to striatal dysfunction8,50 but indicate direct involvement of the cortical premotor areas in PSP and MSA. Our discriminant analysis showed that the tests that best identified the patient groups compared with control subjects were the sequential tapping test and the ideomotor apraxia tests, ie, those requiring motor planning and sequence production. In fact, the extent of the motor disorganization in patients with MSA and PSP indicates a cumulative effect due to both striatal damage and premotor cortical involvement.

Accepted for publication August 8, 1997.

We thank D. C. Ward, BSc, for help with the English.

Reprints: Floriano Girotti, MD, Istituto Nazionale Neurologico, "C. Besta," Via Celoria 11, 20133 Milano, Italy.

References
1.
Hughes  AJDaniel  SEKilford  LLees  AJ Accuracy of clinical diagnosis of idiopathic Parkinson's disease: a clinico-pathological study of 100 cases.  J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1992;55181- 184Google ScholarCrossref
2.
Litvan  IAgid  YJankovic  J  et al.  Accuracy of clinical criteria for the diagnosis of progressive supranuclear palsy (Steele-Richardson-Olszewski syndrome).  Neurology. 1996;46922- 930Google ScholarCrossref
3.
Pillon  BDubois  BAgid  Y Testing cognition may contribute to the diagnosis of movement disorders.  Neurology. 1996;46329- 334Google ScholarCrossref
4.
Brown  RGMarsden  CD ‘Subcortical dementia': the neuropsychological evidence.  Neuroscience. 1988;25363- 387Google ScholarCrossref
5.
Lezak  MD Executive functions and motor performance.  Neuropsychological Assessment. New York, NY Oxford University Press1983;507- 532Google Scholar
6.
Pillon  BGouider-Khouja  NDeweer  B  et al.  Neuropsychological pattern of striatonigral degeneration: comparison with Parkinson's disease and progressive supranuclear palsy.  J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1995;58174- 179Google ScholarCrossref
7.
Robbins  TWJames  MOwen  AM  et al.  Cognitive deficits in progressive supranuclear palsy, Parkinson's disease, and multiple system atrophy in tests sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction.  J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1994;5779- 88Google ScholarCrossref
8.
Alexander  GEDelong  MRStrick  PL Parallel organization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex.  Annu Rev Neurosci. 1986;9357- 381Google ScholarCrossref
9.
Marsden  CDDeecke  LFreund  HJ  et al.  The functions of the supplementary motor area: summary of a workshop. Lüders  HO Advances in Neurology, Vol 70 Supplementary Sensorimotor Area. Philadelphia, Pa Lippincott-Raven Publishers1996;477- 487Google Scholar
10.
Heilman  KMRothi  LJG Apraxia. Heilman  KMValenstein  E Clinical Neuropsychology. 2nd ed. New York, NY Oxford University Press1985;131- 150Google Scholar
11.
Watson  RTShepherd Fleet  WGonzales-Rothi  LHeilman  KM Apraxia and the supplementary motor area.  Arch Neurol. 1986;43787- 792Google ScholarCrossref
12.
Pramstaller  PPMarsden  CD The basal ganglia and apraxia.  Brain. 1996;119319- 340Google ScholarCrossref
13.
Hoehn  MMYahr  MD Parkinsonism: onset, progression and mortality.  Neurology. 1967;17427- 442Google ScholarCrossref
14.
American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition.  Washington, DC American Psychiatric Association1994;
15.
Folstein  MFFolstein  SEMcHugh  PR Mini-mental state: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician.  J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12189- 198Google ScholarCrossref
16.
Meazzo  GCavarzeran  FZappalà  G  et al.  The Mini-Mental State Examination normative study of an Italian random sample.  Dev Neuropsychol. 1993;977- 85Google ScholarCrossref
17.
Quinn  N Multiple system atrophy—the nature of the beast.  J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1989;52(special suppl)78- 89Google ScholarCrossref
18.
Testa  DSavoiardo  MFetoni  V  et al.  Multiple system atrophy: clinical and MR observations on 42 cases.  Ital J Neurol Sci. 1993;14211- 216Google ScholarCrossref
19.
Litvan  IAgid  YCalne  D  et al.  Clinical research criteria for the diagnosis of progressive supranuclear palsy (Steele-Richardson-Olszewski syndrome): report of the NINDS-SPSP International Workshop.  Neurology. 1996;471- 9Google ScholarCrossref
20.
Savoiardo  MGirotti  FStrada  LCiceri  E Magnetic resonance imaging in progressive supranuclear palsy and other parkinsonian disorders.  J Neural Transm Suppl. 1994;4293- 110Google Scholar
21.
Raven  JC Guide to Using the Colored Progressive Matrices.  London, England HK Lewis1965; New York, NY Psychological Corpno date.
22.
Randt  CTBrown  EROsborne  DP A memory test for longitudinal measurement of mild to moderate deficits.  Clin Neuropsychol. 1980;2184- 194Google Scholar
23.
Novelli  GPapagno  CCapitani  ELaiacona  MVallar  GCappa  SF Tre test clinici di ricerca e produzione lessicale: taratura su soggetti normali.  Arch Psicol Neurol Psichiatria. 1986;47477- 506Google Scholar
24.
Benton  ALVarney  NRHamsher de S  K Visuo-spatial judgment: a clinical test.  Arch Neurol. 1978;35364- 367Google ScholarCrossref
25.
Nelson  HE Modified card sorting test sensitive to frontal lobe defects.  Cortex. 1976;12313- 324Google ScholarCrossref
26.
Basso  ACapitani  ELaiacona  M Raven's coloured progressive matrices: normative values on 305 adult normal controls.  Funct Neurol. 1987;2189- 194Google Scholar
27.
Spinnler  HTognoni  G Gruppo italiano per lo studio neuropsicologico dell'invecchiamento: standardizzazione e taratura italiana di test neuropsicologici.  Ital J Neurol Sci. 1987;(suppl 8)1- 120Google Scholar
28.
De Renzi  EMotti  FNichelli  P Imitating gestures: a quantitative approach to ideomotor apraxia.  Arch Neurol. 1980;376- 10Google ScholarCrossref
29.
Oldfield  RC The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh Inventory.  Neuropsychologia. 1971;997- 113Google ScholarCrossref
30.
SPSS Inc, SPSS-X (Statistical Package for the Social Sciences, Version 10).  New York, NY McGraw-Hill Book Co Inc1983;
31.
Rencher  AC Interpretation of canonical discriminant functions, canonical variates, and principal components.  Am Statistician. 1992;46217- 225Google Scholar
32.
Robbins  TWJames  MLange  KWOwen  AMQuinn  NPMarsden  CD Cognitive performance in multiple system atrophy.  Brain. 1992;115271- 291Google ScholarCrossref
33.
Owen  AMRobbins  TW Comparative neuropsychology of parkinsonian syndromes. Wolters  EC Mental Dysfunction in Parkinson's Disease. Dordrecht, the Netherlands ICG Printing1993;221- 241Google Scholar
34.
Testa  DFetoni  VSoliveri  PMusicco  MPalazzini  EGirotti  F Cognitive and motor performance in multiple system atrophy and Parkinson's disease compared.  Neuropsychologia. 1993;31207- 210Google ScholarCrossref
35.
Daniel  SEBruin  VMSLees  AJ The clinical and pathological spectrum of Steele-Richardson-Olszewski syndrome (progressive supranuclear palsy): a reappraisal.  Brain. 1995;118759- 770Google ScholarCrossref
36.
Kimura  DBarnett  HJMBurkhart  G The psychological test pattern in progressive supranuclear palsy.  Neuropsychologia. 1981;19301- 306Google ScholarCrossref
37.
Benecke  RRothwell  JCDick  JPRDay  BLMarsden  CD Disturbance of sequential movements in patients with Parkinson's disease.  Brain. 1987;110361- 379Google ScholarCrossref
38.
Pillon  BBlin  JVidailhet  M  et al.  The neuropsychological pattern of corticobasal degeneration: comparison with progressive supranuclear palsy and Alzheimer's disease.  Neurology. 1995;451477- 1483Google ScholarCrossref
39.
Cambier  JMasson  MViader  FLimodin  JStrube  A Le syndrome frontal de la paralysie supranucléaire progressive.  Rev Neurol. 1985;141528- 536Google Scholar
40.
Freund  HJ The apraxias. Asbury  AKMcKhann  GMMcDonald  WJ Diseases of the Nervous System Clinical Neurobiology. 2nd ed. Philadelphia, Pa WB Saunders Co1992;751- 767Google Scholar
41.
Heilman  KMRothi  LJGValenstein  E Two forms of ideomotor apraxia.  Neurology. 1982;32342- 346Google ScholarCrossref
42.
Goldenberg  GWimmer  AAuff  ESchnaberth  G Impairment of motor planning in patients with Parkinson's disease: evidence from ideomotor apraxia testing.  J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1986;491266- 1272Google ScholarCrossref
43.
Leiguarda  RCPramstaller  PPMerello  MStarkstein  SLees  AJMarsden  CD Apraxia in Parkinson's disease, progressive supranuclear palsy, multiple system atrophy, and neuroleptic-induced parkinsonism.  Brain. 1997;12075- 90Google ScholarCrossref
44.
Schneider  JAWatts  RLGearing  MBrewer  RPMirra  SS Corticobasal degeneration: neuropathologic and clinical heterogeneity.  Neurology. 1997;48959- 969Google ScholarCrossref
45.
Grisoli  MFetoni  VSavoiardo  MGirotti  FBruzzone  MG MRI in corticobasal degeneration.  Eur J Neurol. 1995;2547- 552Google ScholarCrossref
46.
Lantos  PL The neuropathology of progressive supranuclear palsy.  J Neural Transm Suppl. 1994;42137- 152Google Scholar
47.
Verny  MDuyckaerts  CAgid  YHauw  JJ The significance of cortical pathology in progressive supranuclear palsy: clinico-pathological data in 10 cases.  Brain. 1996;1191123- 1136Google ScholarCrossref
48.
Brooks  DJ PET studies in progressive supranuclear palsy.  J Neural Transm Suppl. 1994;42119- 134Google Scholar
49.
Papp  MILantos  PL The distribution of oligodendroglial inclusions in multiple system atrophy and its relevance to clinical symptomatology.  Brain. 1994;117235- 243Google ScholarCrossref
50.
Alexander  GECrutcher  MD Functional architecture of basal ganglia circuit: neural substrate of parallel processing.  Trends Neurosci. 1990;13266- 271Google ScholarCrossref
×