[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Figure 1. Patient flow. Twenty percent of patients were excluded from the analysis because of a history of stroke or missing data. EF indicates ejection fraction.

Figure 1. Patient flow. Twenty percent of patients were excluded from the analysis because of a history of stroke or missing data. EF indicates ejection fraction.

Figure 2. Memory composite score vs ejection fraction by age quartile.

Figure 2. Memory composite score vs ejection fraction by age quartile.

Table 1. Unadjusted Memory Composite Scores for Significant Categorical Variables Among 207 Patients
Table 1. Unadjusted Memory Composite Scores for Significant Categorical Variables Among 207 Patients
Table 2. Multivariate Analysis of the Memory Composite Scores Among 207 Patients
Table 2. Multivariate Analysis of the Memory Composite Scores Among 207 Patients
Table 3. Results of Post Hoc Multivariate Analysis of Ejection Fraction in the Quartile 63 Years or Older on the Memory Component Score Components
Table 3. Results of Post Hoc Multivariate Analysis of Ejection Fraction in the Quartile 63 Years or Older on the Memory Component Score Components
1.
 Cardiogenic dementia [editorial].  Lancet. 1977;1(8001):27-2863661PubMedGoogle Scholar
2.
Almeida OP, Flicker L. The mind of a failing heart: a systematic review of the association between congestive heart failure and cognitive functioning.  Intern Med J. 2001;31(5):290-29511512600PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
Bennett SJ, Sauvé MJ. Cognitive deficits in patients with heart failure: a review of the literature.  J Cardiovasc Nurs. 2003;18(3):219-24212837012PubMedGoogle Scholar
4.
Tilvis RS, Kähönen-Väre MH, Jolkkonen J, Valvanne J, Pitkala KH, Strandberg TE. Predictors of cognitive decline and mortality of aged people over a 10-year period.  J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2004;59(3):268-27415031312PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Almeida OP, Tamai S. Congestive heart failure and cognitive functioning amongst older adults.  Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 2001;59(2-B):324-32911460173PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Jefferson AL, Poppas A, Paul RH, Cohen RA. Systemic hypoperfusion is associated with executive dysfunction in geriatric cardiac patients.  Neurobiol Aging. 2007;28(3):477-48316469418PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Trojano L, Antonelli Incalzi R, Acanfora D, Picone C, Mecocci P, Rengo F.Congestive Heart Failure Italian Study Investigators.  Cognitive impairment: a key feature of congestive heart failure in the elderly.  J Neurol. 2003;250(12):1456-146314673579PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Zuccalà G, Cattel C, Manes-Gravina E, Di Niro MG, Cocchi A, Bernabei R. Left ventricular dysfunction: a clue to cognitive impairment in older patients with heart failure.  J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1997;63(4):509-5129343133PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Hoth KF, Poppas A, Moser DJ, Paul RH, Cohen RA. Cardiac dysfunction and cognition in older adults with heart failure.  Cogn Behav Neurol. 2008;21(2):65-7218541980PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Vogels RL, Oosterman JM, van Harten B,  et al.  Profile of cognitive impairment in chronic heart failure.  J Am Geriatr Soc. 2007;55(11):1764-177017727641PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
Putzke JD, Williams MA, Rayburn BK, Kirklin JK, Boll TJ. The relationship between cardiac function and neuropsychological status among heart transplant candidates.  J Card Fail. 1998;4(4):295-3039924851PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Wolfe R, Worrall-Carter L, Foister K, Keks N, Howe V. Assessment of cognitive function in heart failure patients.  Eur J Cardiovasc Nurs. 2006;5(2):158-16416359923PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Lang CC, Mancini DM. Non-cardiac comorbidities in chronic heart failure.  Heart. 2007;93(6):665-67116488925PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
Román GC. Brain hypoperfusion: a critical factor in vascular dementia.  Neurol Res. 2004;26(5):454-45815265263PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Lang RM, Bierig M, Devereux RB,  et al; Chamber Quantification Writing Group; American Society of Echocardiography's Guidelines and Standards Committee; European Association of Echocardiography.  Recommendations for chamber quantification: a report from the American Society of Echocardiography's Guidelines and Standards Committee and the Chamber Quantification Writing Group, developed in conjunction with the European Association of Echocardiography, a branch of the European Society of Cardiology.  J Am Soc Echocardiogr. 2005;18(12):1440-146316376782PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Brandt J. The Hopkins Verbal Learning Test: development of a new verbal memory test with six equivalent forms.  Clin Neuropsychol. 1991;5:125-142Google ScholarCrossref
17.
Benedict RHB. Brief Visuospatial Memory Test–RevisedLutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources Inc; 1997
18.
Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. 3rd ed. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corp; 1997
19.
Lezak M, Howieson D, Loring D, Hannay H, Fischer J. Neuropsychological Assessment. 4th ed. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2004
20.
Radloff L. The CES-D Scale: a self-report depression scale for research in the general population.  Appl Psychol Meas. 1977;1:385-401Google ScholarCrossref
21.
Mori E. Impact of subcortical ischemic lesions on behavior and cognition.  Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2002;977:141-14812480744PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
Vogels RL, Oosterman JM, van Harten B,  et al.  Neuroimaging and correlates of cognitive function among patients with heart failure.  Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2007;24(6):418-42317938570PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
23.
Kumar R, Woo MA, Birrer BV,  et al.  Mammillary bodies and fornix fibers are injured in heart failure.  Neurobiol Dis. 2009;33(2):236-24219022386PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Afifi AK, Bergman RA. Functional Neuroanatomy. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill Companies; 1998
25.
Choi BR, Kim JS, Yang YJ,  et al.  Factors associated with decreased cerebral blood flow in congestive heart failure secondary to idiopathic dilated cardiomyopathy.  Am J Cardiol. 2006;97(9):1365-136916635612PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
Bornstein RA, Starling RC, Myerowitz PD, Haas GJ. Neuropsychological function in patients with end-stage heart failure before and after cardiac transplantation.  Acta Neurol Scand. 1995;91(4):260-2657625151PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
27.
Roman DD, Kubo SH, Ormaza S, Francis GS, Bank AJ, Shumway SJ. Memory improvement following cardiac transplantation.  J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1997;19(5):692-6979408799PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
28.
Schall RR, Petrucci RJ, Brozena SC, Cavarocchi NC, Jessup M. Cognitive function in patients with symptomatic dilated cardiomyopathy before and after cardiac transplantation.  J Am Coll Cardiol. 1989;14(7):1666-16722584554PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
29.
Sasoh M, Ogasawara K, Kuroda K,  et al.  Effects of EC-IC bypass surgery on cognitive impairment in patients with hemodynamic cerebral ischemia.  Surg Neurol. 2003;59(6):455-46312826338PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
30.
Vicenzini E, Ricciardi MC, Altieri M,  et al.  Cerebrovascular reactivity in degenerative and vascular dementia: a transcranial Doppler study.  Eur Neurol. 2007;58(2):84-8917565221PubMedGoogle Scholar
31.
Carey BJ, Eames PJ, Blake MJ, Panerai RB, Potter JF. Dynamic cerebral autoregulation is unaffected by aging.  Stroke. 2000;31(12):2895-290011108745PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
32.
Georgiadis D, Sievert M, Cencetti S,  et al.  Cerebrovascular reactivity is impaired in patients with cardiac failure.  Eur Heart J. 2000;21(5):407-41310666355PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
33.
Zelis R, Flaim SF. Alterations in vasomotor tone in congestive heart failure.  Prog Cardiovasc Dis. 1982;24(6):437-4596805039PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
34.
Macrae IM, Robinson MJ, Graham DI, Reid JL, McCulloch J. Endothelin-1-induced reductions in cerebral blood flow: dose dependency, time course, and neuropathological consequences.  J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 1993;13(2):276-2848436619PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
35.
Jefferson AL, Tate DF, Poppas A,  et al.  Lower cardiac output is associated with greater white matter hyperintensities in older adults with cardiovascular disease.  J Am Geriatr Soc. 2007;55(7):1044-104817608877PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
36.
Qiu C, Winblad B, Marengoni A, Klarin I, Fastbom J, Fratiglioni L. Heart failure and risk of dementia and Alzheimer disease: a population-based cohort study.  Arch Intern Med. 2006;166(9):1003-100816682574PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
37.
McCullough PA, Philbin EF, Spertus JA, Kaatz S, Sandberg KR, Weaver WD.Resource Utilization Among Congestive Heart Failure (REACH) Study.  Confirmation of a heart failure epidemic: findings from the Resource Utilization Among Congestive Heart Failure (REACH) study.  J Am Coll Cardiol. 2002;39(1):60-6911755288PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
38.
Rosen MI, Beauvais JE, Rigsby MO, Salahi JT, Ryan CE, Cramer JA. Neuropsychological correlates of suboptimal adherence to metformin.  J Behav Med. 2003;26(4):349-36012921008PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
39.
Stilley CS, Sereika S, Muldoon MF, Ryan CM, Dunbar-Jacob J. Psychological and cognitive function: predictors of adherence with cholesterol lowering treatment.  Ann Behav Med. 2004;27(2):117-12415026295PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
40.
Callahan CM, Hendrie HC, Tierney WM. Documentation and evaluation of cognitive impairment in elderly primary care patients.  Ann Intern Med. 1995;122(6):422-4297856990PubMedGoogle Scholar
41.
Stump TE, Callahan CM, Hendrie HC. Cognitive impairment and mortality in older primary care patients.  J Am Geriatr Soc. 2001;49(7):934-94011527485PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
42.
McLennan SN, Pearson SA, Cameron J, Stewart S. Prognostic importance of cognitive impairment in chronic heart failure patients: does specialist management make a difference?  Eur J Heart Fail. 2006;8(5):494-50116504580PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
43.
Bosworth HB, Steinhauser KE, Orr M, Lindquist JH, Grambow SC, Oddone EZ. Congestive heart failure patients' perceptions of quality of life: the integration of physical and psychosocial factors.  Aging Ment Health. 2004;8(1):83-9114690872PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
44.
Di Carlo A, Baldereschi M, Amaducci L,  et al; The Italian Longitudinal Study on Aging.  Cognitive impairment without dementia in older people: prevalence, vascular risk factors, impact on disability.  J Am Geriatr Soc. 2000;48(7):775-78210894316PubMedGoogle Scholar
45.
Temple RO, Putzke JD, Boll TJ. Neuropsychological performance as a function of cardiac status among heart transplant candidates: a replication.  Percept Mot Skills. 2000;91(3, pt 1):821-82511153855PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
Original Contributions
Aug 2011

Association of Low Ejection Fraction With Impaired Verbal Memory in Older Patients With Heart Failure

Author Affiliations

Author Affiliations: Department of Neurology, St Luke’s–Roosevelt Hospital Center (Dr Festa) and Department of Neurology (Drs Festa, Marchidann, Schmidt, Lantz, Marshall, and Lazar), Biostatistics (Ms Jia and Dr Cheung), Psychiatry (Dr Shapiro), Medicine-Cardiology (Drs Mancini and Deng), Surgery (Dr Naka), and Neurological Surgery (Dr Lazar), Columbia University Medical Center, New York, New York.

Arch Neurol. 2011;68(8):1021-1026. doi:10.1001/archneurol.2011.163
Abstract

Background Cognitive dysfunction has a major role in health outcomes for cardiac patients. The association of cognitive dysfunction with heart failure is well established, but the cardiac variables that contribute to cognition are not well understood.

Objectives To investigate the relationships among age, memory, and left ventricular ejection fraction (EF) in patients with heart failure.

Design Retrospective study.

Setting Academic medical center.

Participants A total of 207 patients with heart failure underwent neuropsychological assessment of memory on standardized tests.

Main Outcome Measures Patients were grouped by age quartiles, and memory function was compared in those with an EF below 30% vs those with an EF of 30% or higher.

Results Demographic, cognitive, and medical variables having a significant association with a memory composite score were identified in a univariate linear regression analysis. In a multivariate linear model that adjusted for significant covariates, there was a significant interaction between age and EF for memory function. Patients younger than 63 years maintained stable memory function across EF levels, but patients 63 years or older showed a significant decline in memory performance when EF dropped below 30% (P < .02). Post hoc multivariate analysis showed that verbal delayed recall and recognition were the components of memory most affected by low EF.

Conclusion The effect of EF on memory differs by age such that older patients with lower EFs have significantly reduced verbal memory function.

The association of heart failure (HF) with cognitive decline was suggested 3 decades ago when the search began for a cardiac mechanism of cerebral dysfunction.1 In subsequent studies,2,3 a profile of cognitive impairment was described in which memory and attention deficits predominated. Cognitive deficits were reported in 30% to 80% of patients with HF.2 In patients older than 75 years with HF who were followed up for 5 years, the relative risk for cognitive decline associated with HF was 1.80 (95% confidence interval, 1.02-3.27).4

Cognitive impairment has been studied in older patients with HF, but the results have been inconsistent.5-8 Several studies5,8-10 found a correlation of cognitive impairment with decreased left ventricular ejection fraction (EF). Among geriatric patients with an EF below 30%, it was found that 53% had Mini-Mental State Examination scores lower than 24, with impairment of attention.8 However, other studies6,11,12 failed to replicate this relationship. The health care management of older patients with chronic HF can present additional challenges. Noncardiac HF comorbidities, such as depression, respiratory disorders, renal dysfunction, and polypharmacy,13 may adversely affect cognitive functioning in older patients with HF.

Conflicting evidence about the association of EF with cognitive function suggests a complex relationship between patient variables and the cardiovascular factors that influence cognition. Our objective was to investigate the relationships between age, EF, and memory among patients with HF in a multivariate linear model that included other cognitive, demographic, and medical factors. We hypothesized that low EF would correlate with memory dysfunction because hypoxia-sensitive structures in the brain, such as the hippocampus, may be affected by low-flow states.14 We examined the effects of EF on memory using a retrospective study design in a large sample of patients with HF.

Methods

The study was approved by the institutional review board at Columbia University Medical Center. Between September 1, 2006, and September 30, 2008, a total of 258 adult patients with HF underwent neurocognitive assessment by a clinical neuropsychologist (J.R.F., E.R.L., or R.M.L.) as part of their routine clinical evaluation for potential heart transplantation candidacy. All patients were 17 years or older, were undergoing active medical management for HF, and were hemodynamically stable. Patients were not receiving mechanical circulatory (left ventricular assist device) support at the time of assessment. In addition to neurocognitive testing, recorded variables included EF, HF cause, medical history, current medications, and demographic information.

Measures

Left ventricular EF was assessed by transthoracic echocardiography according to American Society of Echocardiography standards.15 Neuropsychological functioning was evaluated using standardized measures as part of a 1-hour test battery. All cognitive test results were converted to age-corrected z scores using published normative data.16-19 A z score of 0 indicates performance at the level of the age-adjusted population mean, and each 1-point change in z score indicates a 1-SD difference from the mean. Verbal memory was assessed using the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test16 total (immediate) recall, delayed recall, and recognition recall subscales. The Hopkins Verbal Learning Test is a word-list learning verbal memory test in which 12 words are presented in 3 learning trials. Visual memory was assessed using the Brief Visuospatial Memory Test–Revised17 total (immediate) recall and delayed recall subscales. The Brief Visuospatial Memory Test–Revised is a figure-drawing visual memory test in which an array of 6 simple geometric figures is presented in 3 learning trials. The memory composite score (MCS) equals the mean of the summed visual and verbal memory z scores: this composite is weighted with more verbal memory scores. The MCS is expressed as a z score, with negative scores indicating worse performance.

Attention, executive functioning, and self-reported depressive symptoms as measured by the Center for Epidemiological Studies–Depression Scale (CES-D) score were also analyzed to assess their effect on memory. Attention was assessed using the Digit Span subtest of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale III18 and the Trail Making Test Part A.19 The Digit Span requires repetition of increasingly longer digit strings, forward and backward. Age-corrected scaled scores were converted to z scores (mean [SD] score, 10 [3]). In the Trail Making Test Part A, a line is drawn to connect numbered circles, in consecutive order, that are randomly arranged on a page. Times to complete the trail were converted to z scores. The attention composite score equals the mean of the Digit Span and Trail Making Test Part A z scores. Executive functioning was assessed using the Trail Making Test Part B19 and the Controlled Oral Word Association Test.19 The Trail Making Test Part B requires connection of randomly arranged, numbered, and lettered circles, alternating between them. Times to complete the trail were converted to z scores. The Trail Making Test Part B was discontinued after 3 minutes only if the neuropsychologist determined that the patient was incapable of continuing to completion or if it was necessary to maintain patient cooperation, in which case a z score of −5.0 was assigned to clearly indicate impairment on the measure. The Controlled Oral Word Association Test includes 3 one-minute trials requiring the production of words beginning with a given letter (F, A, or S). The sums of all acceptable words were converted to z scores. The Executive Composite Score equals the mean of the Trail Making Test Part B and Controlled Oral Word Association Test z scores. Self-reported depressive symptoms were measured using the CES-D.20 This is a 20-item scale with scores ranging from 0 to 60, with higher scores indicating more severe depressive symptoms. A conventionally accepted score cutoff is 16 for mild depressive symptoms.20 Raw scores were used for the analysis.

Variables

Education was coded as less than high school vs high school or higher. Heart failure cause was categorized as ischemic vs nonischemic. Age was categorized based on distributional quartiles (≤45, 46-55, 56-62, or ≥63 years) The EF variable was dichotomized at 30% (<30% vs ≥30%) based on findings in a previous study8 of EF and cognition.

Statistical analysis

Commercially available statistical software (SAS, version 9.2; SAS Institute, Inc, Cary, North Carolina) was used for data analysis. Patient characteristics were compared between included vs excluded patients using Fisher exact test for discrete variables and Wilcoxon rank sum test for continuous variables. The MCS was compared for categorical variables using analysis of variance. The effects of age and EF on the MCS were assessed using multivariate linear regression analysis. We first fitted a base model that included age, EF, and their interaction as independent variables to assess the EF effect within each age quartile. Univariate linear regression analysis was performed to identify other covariates that have a significant effect on the MCS. Covariates with P < .05 in the univariate analyses were then added to the primary model one at a time in a forward variable selection; only covariates with P < .05, adjusting for other factors in a multivariate linear model, were retained in the forward selection procedure. Therefore, this final multivariate linear model allowed us to assess the effects of age and EF, with adjustments for other significant factors. To better understand the effects of EF on memory, multivariate linear regression analysis was applied to the specific components of the MCS. Although EF was dichotomized to enhance interpretation of the results, we also performed sensitivity analyses in which multivariate linear models were fitted using EF as a continuous variable.

Results

Of 258 patients, 51 were excluded from the MCS analysis because of 1 or more of the following reasons: history of stroke (n = 22) or missing information on EF (n = 2), MCS (n = 11), executive functioning composite score (n = 26), attention composite score (n = 5), or CES-D (n = 8). The final analysis included 207 patients, 80.2% of the original sample (Figure 1). Compared with included patients, excluded patients were older (P = .06), were less educated (P = .01), were more likely to have an ischemic HF cause (P = .06), and had a lower attention composite score (P < .01). Potential covariates analyzed for significance in the univariate linear regression analysis included the following: sex, EF, race/ethnicity, serum urea nitrogen and creatinine levels, self-reported depressive symptoms on the CES-D, and the use of statins, benzodiazepines, β-blockers, or antidepressants, as well as a history of diabetes mellitus, cardiac arrest, valve surgery, coronary artery bypass grafting, and pacemaker or defibrillator implantation. The groups were similar on these variables.

The 207 patients included in the analysis ranged in age from 17 to 72 years. Patients were predominantly male (74%), and 60.4% were white, 24.6% African American, 6.8% Hispanic, 4.8% Asian, 1.0% Middle Eastern, and 2.4% multiethnic or of other race/ethnicity. Education was less than high school in 10.1% and high school or higher in 89.9% of the sample. Overall, the mean MCS was 1.2 SD below the normative age-corrected mean. Discontinuation of the Trail Making Test Part B occurred in 14 patients (6.8%).

In the univariate analyses, the following factors were significantly associated with low MCS: history of hypertension (P = .03), use of benzodiazepines (P = .05), nonwhite race/ethnicity (P = .003), less than high school education (P = .01), low attention composite score (P < .001), low executive functioning composite score (P < .001), and self-rated depressive symptoms as measured by the CES-D score (P < .001). For each significant categorical variable, the unadjusted mean (SE) MCS was evaluated using analysis of variance (Table 1).

In the primary model with age and EF, low EF was associated with low memory function among patients 63 years or older (P = .008). Scatterplots of the MCS and EF data by age quartile are shown in Figure 2. After adding the significant covariates from the univariate analyses, the significance of EF remained. Among patients 63 years or older, an EF below 30% decreased the MCS on average by 0.95 (95% confidence interval, 0.18-1.72; P = .02). Detailed results of the multivariate linear model are given in Table 2. For the sensitivity analysis in which EF is used as a continuous variable, low EF was significantly associated with low MCS (P < .001), supporting the robustness of the results. In the final multivariate linear model, other factors associated with low MCS were low attention composite score (P = .004), low executive functioning composite score (P < .001), and self-reported depressive symptoms as measured by the CES-D score (P = .01).

Post hoc multivariate analyses were conducted to identify the components of memory affected by low EF in older patients. The same multivariate linear model used to evaluate the MCS was applied individually to each of 5 components of the MCS. The effect of low EF on memory function in patients with HF older than 62 years was greatest on verbal recall memory (Hopkins Verbal Learning Test delayed recall, P = .02) and verbal recognition recall (Hopkins Verbal Learning Test recognition recall, P = .05), even after adjusting for the effects of attention composite score, executive functioning composite score, and self-reported depressive symptoms as measured by the CES-D score. Although the effects on the other 3 memory components were not significant, the point estimates of the effects were all in the same direction (higher EF was associated with better memory), suggesting similar trends. Because EF effects in the other 3 age quartiles were not significant, Table 3 gives only the results of the multivariate analysis of EF in the quartile 63 years or older on the MCS components, with each row representing the estimated effect of EF on the individual component of the MCS, adjusting for race/ethnicity, attention composite score, executive functioning composite score, and self-reported depressive symptoms as measured by the CES-D score. The conclusions remained the same when sensitivity analyses were performed to assess the effect of EF as a continuous variable by age quartile.

Comment

The results of this study demonstrate that the effect of EF on memory differs by age. In patients with HF 63 years or older, decreased EF was associated with significantly worse memory (>1.4 SD lower on clinical measures). In contrast, memory remained stable in patients 62 years or younger, regardless of EF. There was no effect of low EF in the 3 youngest quartiles. Considering all age quartiles together, the absence of a significant effect of EF on memory function demonstrates how the effect of EF on memory can be masked by failing to take age into account. Our results may explain the mixed results found in previous studies5-12 about the effect of EF on cognitive functioning, as the effect of age observed herein may have been obscured in investigations that did not examine age as a variable or where participants were younger.

Our main finding of a significant interaction between age and EF for verbal memory function was unchanged by inclusion of other demographic, cognitive, and medical variables. The MCS was weighted with more verbal tests, and that likely drove the significance of the composite memory analysis. Although it is well established that attention is frequently deficient in patients with HF, neither attention nor executive function fully explained the dysfunction in verbal memory. However, attention and executive functioning were significant in the memory multivariate linear model, suggesting a role in the disruption of verbal memory function through involvement of the frontosubcortical circuits.21

Immediate, delayed, and recognition memory data were separately examined to determine whether individual memory components were more affected by age and EF. The impairment observed in delayed recall was significant and suggests a deficit in the storage of semantic memory. The failure in recognition memory was also significant, indicating that the deficit is not attributable to dysfunction in memory retrieval. Increased cerebral atrophy in patients with HF, particularly in the medial temporal lobe22 and mamillary bodies and fornix,23 may have contributed to the memory storage deficit seen in this cohort. However, the dissociation of verbal and visual memory was an unexpected finding. While memory is a complex function involving widespread neuronal circuits, storage of new memories likely involves the association cortices of the temporal and occipital lobes for auditory and visual memory, respectively.24 The results of imaging studies22,23 suggest a greater influence on the temporal association cortices in patients with HF and may explain the differential effect of HF on verbal memory.

The mechanism by which EF has a role in memory function among older patients with HF is not fully established. Among the patients herein, histories of comorbidities and cardiac surgical procedures, as well as renal function test results and current medication use, did not alter the interaction between age and EF for verbal memory function. Our finding that EF had a greater effect on memory in the oldest quartile suggests that older patients may have less capacity to compensate for low EF. The findings of several small studies5,8-10 noted a relationship between HF, particularly low EF, and impaired cognition in older individuals. Other studies11,12 failed to correlate memory with EF. Cerebral hemodynamics are potential mechanisms of memory dysfunction in HF. Compared with control subjects, global cerebral blood flow was reduced by 19% in patients with congestive HF.25 In other studies, although heart transplantation improved cognition in a few patients,26,27 it did not in others, despite restoration of global cerebral blood flow.28 Another potential mechanism affecting cognition and global cerebral blood flow is cerebrovascular autoregulation. Changes in cerebrovascular autoregulation have been tied to restoration29 and decline30 in cognition. Dynamic cerebrovascular autoregulation was unaffected by aging in nonclinical populations31; however, it was significantly reduced in patients with HF compared with age-matched controls.32 In patients with low EF, vasoconstriction caused by higher catecholamine levels33 and endothelin 1 increases34 may contribute to impaired cerebral autoregulation.

Neuropathological differences among patients in our study may have contributed to the differential effects on memory function demonstrated by the age quartiles. Greater cerebrovascular disease burden,35 neurodegenerative disease, and cerebral atrophy may be mechanisms of memory dysfunction among older patients. Heart failure almost doubles the risk of incident dementia among older adults.36 Future studies should examine ischemic cerebrovascular disease burden and regional atrophy to elucidate additional mechanisms of cognitive dysfunction in patients with HF.

There are significant implications of cognitive dysfunction in HF. The prevalence of HF is increasing in Western countries, particularly among older segments of the population.37 Based on conservative estimates, cognitive dysfunction due to HF may affect at least 1 million patients with HF in the United States.27 Dysfunction in cognition is an independent predictor of compliance with medication use and medical appointments,38,39 thereby mediating health outcomes in cardiac patients. Not only is cognitive functioning important in chronic self-management of HF, but also moderate to severe cognitive impairment is associated with increased use of medical services and with mortality.40,41 In a 2006 study,42 a Mini-Mental State Examination score lower than 24 was associated with a 2.33-fold increased relative risk of death even among those participating in an HF management plan. Cognitive functioning is also an important dimension of quality of life43 and is essential to activities of daily living.44 Identifying older patients with HF who are at risk for greater cognitive dysfunction can improve treatment outcomes and may ultimately reduce the use of medical services.

There are limitations of this study. The addition of a control group could have strengthened our analysis. Attempts were made to examine the effect of race/ethnicity on memory function in the absence of published normative data for different racial/ethnic groups. Analyses of the effect of race/ethnicity on memory function will be strengthened in the future with appropriate normative data. Many other potential variables not analyzed herein may more fully explain the complex interplay of compensatory mechanisms that mediate cognition, such as the use of drugs that were excluded from our analysis, drug interactions, inflammatory markers, and cardiac, cerebrovascular, and neurodegenerative disease variables. Ejection fraction is a measure of left ventricular function but does not fully reflect cardiac output and consequent cerebral perfusion, which may be influenced by other factors.25 Cardiac output and cardiac index may be better measures of global cardiac function than EF. However, similar studies found weak11 or no45 correlation of cardiac output with cognition.

In summary, an interaction exists between age and EF such that older patients with low EF had significantly reduced memory, particularly verbal delayed recall and recognition. However, the effect of low EF on memory in these patients is not entirely explained by EF. Further research is needed to elucidate additional mechanisms of cognitive dysfunction in patients with HF, including neurodegeneration, inflammation, white matter disease burden, cerebral hypoperfusion, cerebral dysautoregulation, and alternative measures of global cardiac function.

Back to top
Article Information

Correspondence: Joanne R. Festa, PhD, Department of Neurology, St Luke’s–Roosevelt Hospital Center, 425 W 59th St, Ste 6A, New York, NY 10019 (jfesta@chpnet.org).

Accepted for Publication: February 16, 2011.

Author Contributions:Study concept and design: Festa, Shapiro, Deng, Marshall, and Lazar. Acquisition of data: Festa, Naka, Deng, and Lantz. Analysis and interpretation of data: Festa, Jia, Cheung, Marchidann, Schmidt, Mancini, Marshall, and Lazar. Drafting of the manuscript: Festa, Cheung, Marchidann, Schmidt, and Naka. Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Festa, Jia, Cheung, Marchidann, Shapiro, Mancini, Deng, Lantz, Marshall, and Lazar. Statistical analysis: Jia, Cheung, and Schmidt. Obtained funding: Shapiro and Lazar. Administrative, technical, and material support: Festa, Mancini, Lantz, and Lazar. Study supervision: Festa, Lantz, Marshall, and Lazar.

Financial Disclosure: None reported.

Funding/Support: This work was supported by the Nathaniel Wharton Fund (Dr Shapiro) and by grant P50 HL077096 from the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute, National Institutes of Health (Dr Lazar).

This article was corrected for errors on August 17, 2011.

References
1.
 Cardiogenic dementia [editorial].  Lancet. 1977;1(8001):27-2863661PubMedGoogle Scholar
2.
Almeida OP, Flicker L. The mind of a failing heart: a systematic review of the association between congestive heart failure and cognitive functioning.  Intern Med J. 2001;31(5):290-29511512600PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
Bennett SJ, Sauvé MJ. Cognitive deficits in patients with heart failure: a review of the literature.  J Cardiovasc Nurs. 2003;18(3):219-24212837012PubMedGoogle Scholar
4.
Tilvis RS, Kähönen-Väre MH, Jolkkonen J, Valvanne J, Pitkala KH, Strandberg TE. Predictors of cognitive decline and mortality of aged people over a 10-year period.  J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2004;59(3):268-27415031312PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Almeida OP, Tamai S. Congestive heart failure and cognitive functioning amongst older adults.  Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 2001;59(2-B):324-32911460173PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Jefferson AL, Poppas A, Paul RH, Cohen RA. Systemic hypoperfusion is associated with executive dysfunction in geriatric cardiac patients.  Neurobiol Aging. 2007;28(3):477-48316469418PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Trojano L, Antonelli Incalzi R, Acanfora D, Picone C, Mecocci P, Rengo F.Congestive Heart Failure Italian Study Investigators.  Cognitive impairment: a key feature of congestive heart failure in the elderly.  J Neurol. 2003;250(12):1456-146314673579PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Zuccalà G, Cattel C, Manes-Gravina E, Di Niro MG, Cocchi A, Bernabei R. Left ventricular dysfunction: a clue to cognitive impairment in older patients with heart failure.  J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1997;63(4):509-5129343133PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Hoth KF, Poppas A, Moser DJ, Paul RH, Cohen RA. Cardiac dysfunction and cognition in older adults with heart failure.  Cogn Behav Neurol. 2008;21(2):65-7218541980PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Vogels RL, Oosterman JM, van Harten B,  et al.  Profile of cognitive impairment in chronic heart failure.  J Am Geriatr Soc. 2007;55(11):1764-177017727641PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
Putzke JD, Williams MA, Rayburn BK, Kirklin JK, Boll TJ. The relationship between cardiac function and neuropsychological status among heart transplant candidates.  J Card Fail. 1998;4(4):295-3039924851PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Wolfe R, Worrall-Carter L, Foister K, Keks N, Howe V. Assessment of cognitive function in heart failure patients.  Eur J Cardiovasc Nurs. 2006;5(2):158-16416359923PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Lang CC, Mancini DM. Non-cardiac comorbidities in chronic heart failure.  Heart. 2007;93(6):665-67116488925PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
Román GC. Brain hypoperfusion: a critical factor in vascular dementia.  Neurol Res. 2004;26(5):454-45815265263PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Lang RM, Bierig M, Devereux RB,  et al; Chamber Quantification Writing Group; American Society of Echocardiography's Guidelines and Standards Committee; European Association of Echocardiography.  Recommendations for chamber quantification: a report from the American Society of Echocardiography's Guidelines and Standards Committee and the Chamber Quantification Writing Group, developed in conjunction with the European Association of Echocardiography, a branch of the European Society of Cardiology.  J Am Soc Echocardiogr. 2005;18(12):1440-146316376782PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Brandt J. The Hopkins Verbal Learning Test: development of a new verbal memory test with six equivalent forms.  Clin Neuropsychol. 1991;5:125-142Google ScholarCrossref
17.
Benedict RHB. Brief Visuospatial Memory Test–RevisedLutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources Inc; 1997
18.
Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. 3rd ed. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corp; 1997
19.
Lezak M, Howieson D, Loring D, Hannay H, Fischer J. Neuropsychological Assessment. 4th ed. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2004
20.
Radloff L. The CES-D Scale: a self-report depression scale for research in the general population.  Appl Psychol Meas. 1977;1:385-401Google ScholarCrossref
21.
Mori E. Impact of subcortical ischemic lesions on behavior and cognition.  Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2002;977:141-14812480744PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
Vogels RL, Oosterman JM, van Harten B,  et al.  Neuroimaging and correlates of cognitive function among patients with heart failure.  Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2007;24(6):418-42317938570PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
23.
Kumar R, Woo MA, Birrer BV,  et al.  Mammillary bodies and fornix fibers are injured in heart failure.  Neurobiol Dis. 2009;33(2):236-24219022386PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Afifi AK, Bergman RA. Functional Neuroanatomy. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill Companies; 1998
25.
Choi BR, Kim JS, Yang YJ,  et al.  Factors associated with decreased cerebral blood flow in congestive heart failure secondary to idiopathic dilated cardiomyopathy.  Am J Cardiol. 2006;97(9):1365-136916635612PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
Bornstein RA, Starling RC, Myerowitz PD, Haas GJ. Neuropsychological function in patients with end-stage heart failure before and after cardiac transplantation.  Acta Neurol Scand. 1995;91(4):260-2657625151PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
27.
Roman DD, Kubo SH, Ormaza S, Francis GS, Bank AJ, Shumway SJ. Memory improvement following cardiac transplantation.  J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1997;19(5):692-6979408799PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
28.
Schall RR, Petrucci RJ, Brozena SC, Cavarocchi NC, Jessup M. Cognitive function in patients with symptomatic dilated cardiomyopathy before and after cardiac transplantation.  J Am Coll Cardiol. 1989;14(7):1666-16722584554PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
29.
Sasoh M, Ogasawara K, Kuroda K,  et al.  Effects of EC-IC bypass surgery on cognitive impairment in patients with hemodynamic cerebral ischemia.  Surg Neurol. 2003;59(6):455-46312826338PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
30.
Vicenzini E, Ricciardi MC, Altieri M,  et al.  Cerebrovascular reactivity in degenerative and vascular dementia: a transcranial Doppler study.  Eur Neurol. 2007;58(2):84-8917565221PubMedGoogle Scholar
31.
Carey BJ, Eames PJ, Blake MJ, Panerai RB, Potter JF. Dynamic cerebral autoregulation is unaffected by aging.  Stroke. 2000;31(12):2895-290011108745PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
32.
Georgiadis D, Sievert M, Cencetti S,  et al.  Cerebrovascular reactivity is impaired in patients with cardiac failure.  Eur Heart J. 2000;21(5):407-41310666355PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
33.
Zelis R, Flaim SF. Alterations in vasomotor tone in congestive heart failure.  Prog Cardiovasc Dis. 1982;24(6):437-4596805039PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
34.
Macrae IM, Robinson MJ, Graham DI, Reid JL, McCulloch J. Endothelin-1-induced reductions in cerebral blood flow: dose dependency, time course, and neuropathological consequences.  J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 1993;13(2):276-2848436619PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
35.
Jefferson AL, Tate DF, Poppas A,  et al.  Lower cardiac output is associated with greater white matter hyperintensities in older adults with cardiovascular disease.  J Am Geriatr Soc. 2007;55(7):1044-104817608877PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
36.
Qiu C, Winblad B, Marengoni A, Klarin I, Fastbom J, Fratiglioni L. Heart failure and risk of dementia and Alzheimer disease: a population-based cohort study.  Arch Intern Med. 2006;166(9):1003-100816682574PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
37.
McCullough PA, Philbin EF, Spertus JA, Kaatz S, Sandberg KR, Weaver WD.Resource Utilization Among Congestive Heart Failure (REACH) Study.  Confirmation of a heart failure epidemic: findings from the Resource Utilization Among Congestive Heart Failure (REACH) study.  J Am Coll Cardiol. 2002;39(1):60-6911755288PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
38.
Rosen MI, Beauvais JE, Rigsby MO, Salahi JT, Ryan CE, Cramer JA. Neuropsychological correlates of suboptimal adherence to metformin.  J Behav Med. 2003;26(4):349-36012921008PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
39.
Stilley CS, Sereika S, Muldoon MF, Ryan CM, Dunbar-Jacob J. Psychological and cognitive function: predictors of adherence with cholesterol lowering treatment.  Ann Behav Med. 2004;27(2):117-12415026295PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
40.
Callahan CM, Hendrie HC, Tierney WM. Documentation and evaluation of cognitive impairment in elderly primary care patients.  Ann Intern Med. 1995;122(6):422-4297856990PubMedGoogle Scholar
41.
Stump TE, Callahan CM, Hendrie HC. Cognitive impairment and mortality in older primary care patients.  J Am Geriatr Soc. 2001;49(7):934-94011527485PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
42.
McLennan SN, Pearson SA, Cameron J, Stewart S. Prognostic importance of cognitive impairment in chronic heart failure patients: does specialist management make a difference?  Eur J Heart Fail. 2006;8(5):494-50116504580PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
43.
Bosworth HB, Steinhauser KE, Orr M, Lindquist JH, Grambow SC, Oddone EZ. Congestive heart failure patients' perceptions of quality of life: the integration of physical and psychosocial factors.  Aging Ment Health. 2004;8(1):83-9114690872PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
44.
Di Carlo A, Baldereschi M, Amaducci L,  et al; The Italian Longitudinal Study on Aging.  Cognitive impairment without dementia in older people: prevalence, vascular risk factors, impact on disability.  J Am Geriatr Soc. 2000;48(7):775-78210894316PubMedGoogle Scholar
45.
Temple RO, Putzke JD, Boll TJ. Neuropsychological performance as a function of cardiac status among heart transplant candidates: a replication.  Percept Mot Skills. 2000;91(3, pt 1):821-82511153855PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
×