Importance
The clinical utility of monitoring behavioral changes during intraoperative testing of subcallosal cingulate deep brain stimulation is unknown.
Objective
To characterize the structural connectivity correlates of deep brain stimulation–evoked behavioral effects using probabilistic tractography in depression.
Design, Setting, and Participants
Categorization of acute behavioral effects was conducted in 9 adults undergoing deep brain stimulation implantation surgery for chronic treatment-resistant depression in a randomized and blinded testing session at Emory University. Patients were studied from September 1, 2011, through June 30, 2013. Post hoc analyses of the structural tractography patterns mediating distinct categories of evoked behavioral effects were defined, including the best response overall. Data analyses were performed from May 1 through July 1, 2015.
Main Outcomes and Measures
Categorization of stimulation-induced transient behavioral effects and delineation of the shared white matter tracts mediating response subtypes.
Results
Among the 9 patients, 72 active and 36 sham trials were recorded. The following stereotypical behavior patterns were identified: changes in interoceptive (noted changes in body state in 30 of 72 active and 4 of 36 sham trials) and in exteroceptive (shift in attention from patient to others in 9 of 72 active and 0 sham trials) awareness. The best response was a combination of exteroceptive and interoceptive changes at a single left contact for all 9 patients. Structural connectivity showed that the best response contacts had a pattern of connections to the bilateral ventromedial frontal cortex (via forceps minor and left uncinate fasciculus) and to the cingulate cortex (via left cingulum bundle), whereas behaviorally salient but nonbest contacts had only cingulate involvement. The involvement of the 3 white matter bundles during stimulation of the best contacts suggests a mechanism for the observed transient “depression switch.”
Conclusions and Relevance
This analysis of transient behavior changes during intraoperative deep brain stimulation of the subcallosal cingulate and the subsequent identification of unique connectivity patterns may provide a biomarker of a rapid-onset depression switch to guide surgical implantation and to refine and optimize algorithms for the selection of contacts in long-term stimulation for treatment-resistant depression.
Intraoperative testing during deep brain stimulation (DBS) implantation surgery offers a unique window on localized brain function beyond its critical role in determining optimal targeting and stimulation settings. Intraoperative effects with transient stimulation have a well-recognized role during surgery for movement disorders, when such testing commonly is used to optimize clinically desirable changes in tremor, rigidity, or bradykinesia and to avoid adverse effects, such as diplopia and paresthesias.1 Although this strategy has been used most systematically for contemporary DBS techniques, behavioral and emotional responses to acute stimulation were first described during the early studies of intracranial self-stimulation conducted in the 1960s.2 With the development and testing of new clinical indications for DBS, these nonmotor acute behavioral phenomena have been increasingly observed with stimulation at various brain targets across multiple neuropsychiatric disorders, such as euphoria, involuntary smiles, and laughter within the nucleus accumbens3-5; despair,6 apathy, hypomania, and aggressive behavior7 in and around the subthalamic nucleus; panic8,9 in the posterior hypothalamus; and episodic memory recollections in the fornix.10
Deep brain stimulation of the subcallosal cingulate (SCC) white matter is an emerging strategy for treatment-resistant depression.11-19 In addition to growing evidence of the efficacy of long-term stimulation, transient changes in mood, attention, and social connectedness have been reported during intraoperative testing. These observations are not exclusive to the SCC target; such effects have also been described with stimulation in other putative depression targets, including the medial forebrain bundle and nucleus accumbens.20-23
To date, the experience of patients during SCC testing has been notable for certain stereotypical features. Patients commonly describe “a sudden calmness or lightness,” disappearance of a “void,” a sense of “connectedness,” increased interest, and even sudden brightening of the room.11 These responses occur with stimulation in either hemisphere, are specific to contacts and the delivered current dose, and most important, occur with stimulation at some but not all contacts along the DBS lead. These behavioral effects quickly and consistently fade with discontinuation of the stimulation. When present, the effects are unequivocal and reproducible for each patient; however, owing to the idiosyncratic nature of self-reports, quantification has yet to be standardized. These acute SCC stimulation effects are rarely duplicated outside the operating room and, if present, are considerably more subtle. As with acute stimulation effects seen with other DBS targets, whether these SCC stimulation effects reflect changes in local SCC or more widespread network function remains unclear.6,24 Also unknown is whether these effects predict a sustained antidepressant response to long-term DBS.
Structural connectivity analyses using diffusion magnetic resonance imaging and patient-specific tractography maps to define the extent of the white matter network affected by long-term SCC DBS have demonstrated that clinically effective SCC stimulation requires inclusion of the following 4 white matter bundles in each hemisphere: the forceps minor of the anterior corpus callosum connecting the 2 ventromedial frontal cortices, the cingulum bundle connecting the ipsilateral SCC to the rostral and dorsal anterior cingulate cortices, the uncinate fasciculus connecting the SCC to the ventromedial frontal cortex, and the frontal-subcortical fibers connecting the SCC to the basal ganglia, thalamus, and brainstem.24 Identification of these white matter bundles in advance of DBS lead implantation is now being tested prospectively to determine whether targeting and stimulation at this white matter hub improves clinical outcomes compared with standard methods.25 With this foundation, we examined similarly derived structural connectivity patterns of SCC stimulation mediating acute intraoperative behavior responses with the goal of identifying an intraoperative biomarker of optimal SCC lead placement.
From September 1, 2011, through June 30, 2013, 9 consecutive patients with severe, chronic, treatment-resistant major depressive disorder were enrolled in a research protocol at Emory University to test the safety and efficacy of SCC DBS.26 The protocol was approved by the institutional review board at Emory University and by the US Food and Drug Administration under Investigational Device Exemption G060028 sponsored by one of us (H.S.M.) and is monitored by the Data and Safety Monitoring Board of the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Emory University. All participants signed an informed consent to participate; all patients continue in the ongoing longitudinal study. The last follow-up for data presented herein is December 2013.
The study inclusion and exclusion criteria were identical to those previously published by Holtzheimer et al.14 In brief, patients were required to have been depressed for at least 12 months of the current depressive episode, with a minimum Hamilton Depression Rating Scale severity score of 20 (range, 0-50, with higher scores indicating increased severity)27; to have used at least 4 antidepressant treatments (including electroconvulsive therapy) without improvement; to have no significant psychiatric or medical comorbidities; and to be functionally disabled with a Global Assessment of Function score of less than 50 (range, 1-100, with higher scores indicating better function)28 (Table 1).
The surgical procedure for DBS lead and pulse generator implantation followed published methods.11,12,14 We extended a previously described frame-based, stereotaxic, anatomical localization protocol whereby the gray-white matter junction at the middle SCC region is identified on high-resolution T1-weighted magnetic resonance structural images.11,14 Target selection for this patient cohort also used an individualized preoperative deterministic tractography map to identify the location of the intersection of 4 white matter bundles recently shown to be necessary for effective antidepressant effects.29 The combined tractography and anatomical images guided standard localization of the DBS lead tip and trajectory using a surgical planning workstation (Stealth Station; Medtronic, Inc). Bilateral DBS leads (Libra system; St Jude Medical), each with 4 contacts (1.5-mm intercontact spacing), were placed and secured. Testing was then initiated with the patient awake and alert.
Intraoperative Behavior Response Assessments
The stimulation protocol consisted of 12 trials (1 at each of the 8 available contacts [4 left and 4 right sides] and 4 sham trials) with 3 minutes of stimulation on followed by 3 minutes of stimulation off using standard settings for long-term SCC stimulation (monopolar stimulation; frequency, 130 Hz; pulse width, 90 microseconds; and current, 6 mA). The order of active or sham trials was randomized, with the patient and the clinician rater blinded to the condition. The design of 3 minutes on and 3 minutes off maximized the likelihood of adequate time to capture acute (within the first minute) and sustained (maintained throughout the stimulation epoch) behavioral changes and to ensure that a new baseline was reestablished before the next trial. The procedure was videotaped to review, verify, and catalog patient comments outside the operating room. Patients were instructed to monitor themselves during each trial for any changes in sensation, feelings, mood, or thought and to describe any changes when queried. Self-reports were recorded at fixed points within each trial (1 minute after initiation of stimulation and again 1 minute after discontinuation of stimulation). At the conclusion of the protocol, responses for each of the 12 trials were reviewed and designated as present or absent. In trials with the response present, features of the response were further classified into 2 categorical types based on the salience, quality, and magnitude of the self-report. Response type 1 was defined by the presence solely of a perceived change in body state (ie, interoceptive awareness) or specific physical sensations. Response type 2 was characterized by a more complex set of evoked thoughts and feelings commonly indicated by a shift in attention from self to others (ie, exteroceptive awareness). The numbers of type 1 or type 2 responses were summed for each hemisphere, and a “best” contact was then selected reflecting the most robust combination of interoceptive awareness (type 1) and complex behavioral phenomena (type 2) overall. Once rankings were completed, the trials were unblinded and the contacts (left side, 1-4; right side, 1-4) were matched to the trial and response types. These classifications were subsequently used for structural connectivity analyses to define white matter tracts mediating the best vs any salient responses (type 1 or type 2 alone) and differences between the right and left hemisphere stimulation effects.
Imaging Acquisition Protocol
One week before surgery, magnetic resonance imaging was performed (3.0-T Tim-Trio scanner; Siemens Medical Solutions) at the Biomedical Imaging Technology Center of Emory University. A high-resolution, T1-weighted structural image was collected for each patient using a 3-dimensional, magnetization-prepared, rapid gradient-echo sequence with the following settings: repetition time, 2600 milliseconds; inversion time, 900 milliseconds; echo time, 3.02 milliseconds; flip angle, 8°; voxel resolution, 1 × 1 × 1 mm; number of sections, 176; and matrix, 224 × 256. Sixty noncollinear diffusion-weighted images were obtained using single-shot, spin-echo echo-planar imaging with the following settings: generalized autocalibrating parallel acquisition30 reduction factor, 2; field of view, 256 × 256; b value, 1000 s/mm2; voxel resolution, 2 × 2 × 2 mm; number of sections, 64; matrix, 128 × 128; repetition time, 11 300 milliseconds; echo time, 104 milliseconds; 4 nondiffusion-weighted images (b value, 0); and 2 different phase-encoding directions (anterior to posterior and posterior to anterior) to compensate for susceptibility-induced distortion. Three weeks after surgery, a high-resolution computed tomographic image was collected (LightSpeed 16; GE Medical System) (voxel size, 0.46 × 0.46 × 0.65 mm3) to identify a lead and contact location.
All image processing and analyses were performed using tools from the FMRIB Software Library (FSL) (http://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki/).31,32 First, T1-weighted images were normalized into a Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI)–152 template using a combination of linear and nonlinear registration methods (FLIRT and FNIRT, respectively, in FSL). Second, diffusion tensor imaging data underwent simultaneous Eddy current and movement correction (FSL), skull stripping (Brain Extraction Tool in FSL),33 and local tensor fitting (FMRIB Diffusion Toolbox in FSL).34 Then, diffusion tensor imaging data were coregistered to T1-weighted images using the Boundary-Based Registration35 method and normalized to the MNI-152 template using a previously calculated, nonlinear warping field from the T1-weighted normalization step.
Lead Localization and Modeling of Volume of Tissue Activated
The location of each of the 8 contacts was identified using a postoperative computed tomographic image for each patient (Figure 1). First, the computed tomographic image was transferred to T1 space by linear transformation, and each contact location was identified in T1 native space. Second, a patient-specific volume of tissue activated (VTA) was generated for each contact in T1 native space using DBS activation volume prediction modeling methods36-38 and the following stimulation settings: frequency, 130 Hz; pulse width, 90 microseconds; stimulation amplitude, 6 mA; and individual impedance measures. Last, the patient-specific VTAs were transformed to the MNI-152 template for use as seeds for the probabilistic tractography analyses.
Connectivity Analysis of Acute Intraoperative Behavior
Whole-brain probabilistic fiber tractography was generated from each of the patient-specific VTA seeds to construct a structural connectivity map (FMRIB Diffusion Toolbox in FSL).34 Eight different structural connectivity maps from patient-specific VTA seeds (4 contacts in each hemisphere) were generated for each patient. Five thousand streamlines were sent out from each voxel within the VTA with masking of the cerebrospinal fluid to reduce false-positive connections. A streamline density map was generated by the number of connected streamlines divided by the total number of streamlines sent out.39 Next, the streamline density map was binarized (ie, streamline was present or absent within given voxel) at a threshold value of 0.2% after optimization procedures that tested various threshold values (0.01% to approximately 1%).40 Last, the binarized maps were summed and divided by the total number of each specific response type to calculate the overlap map. To identify the specific white matter bundles mediating the defined response types, a common shared map (80% shared voxels) was generated for (1) the best contacts (regardless of hemisphere), (2) those contacts that were not best but nonetheless salient (left and right hemisphere contacts grouped separately), and (3) those contacts with no response. All data analysis was performed from May 1 through July 1, 2015.
Intraoperative Behavior Response Characteristics
Among the 9 patients, a total of 108 individual stimulation trials were recorded (72 active [36 per hemisphere] and 36 sham). Thirty of the 72 active contacts generated a type 1 interoceptive response (17 in the left hemisphere and 13 in the right hemisphere) and 9 contacts generated a type 2 exteroceptive response (all in the left hemisphere), whereas 42 contacts evoked no response. Of the 36 sham trials, only 4 generated a mild type 1 response and none generated a type 2 response. As previously observed, a behavioral switch was apparent to patients within the first minute of the initiation of the stimulation, and effects were sustained while stimulation remained on. Patients generally noted a clear fading of any effects within the first minute after discontinuation and a return to their pretrial baseline after about 2 minutes.
The best contact for each of the 9 patients was always in the left hemisphere. Self-reports from these best contacts showed robust types 1 and 2 responses, including “lightening of mood”; “feeling warm”; “lighter”; “feeling more connected”; “I can get outside of myself to pay attention to you”; noticing objects, people, and activities ongoing in the operating room; and interest and perceived capacity to engage in various personally relevant activities if they were home (eg, taking a shower, washing the dishes, and walking the dog). Salient (positive but not best) responses occurred with equal frequency with stimulation of contacts in either hemisphere. Self-reported statements included “lifting,” “less heaviness,” “less tension,” “increased air and ability to breathe,” and “a feeling of relief.” These physical sensations were commonly accompanied by changes in facial expression observed by the rater (eg, eyes widening, softening of corrugator muscle contractions) and increases in verbal fluency and speech output. Overall, the salient responses primarily involved changes in interoceptive awareness (type 1 responses). In contrast, the best responses were consistently characterized by a combination of interoceptive changes (type 1) and exteroceptive attention and engagement (type 2). Samples of spontaneous statements and clinical observations from the 9 patients are listed in Table 2.
Behavioral Connectivity Analyses
The following 3 common white matter bundles were affected by stimulation of the 9 left-sided contacts mediating a best response: the uncinate fasciculus connecting to the ipsilateral ventromedial frontal cortex, the forceps minor connecting to the bilateral ventromedial frontal cortices, and the left cingulum bundle connecting to the ipsilateral anterior cingulate cortex (Figure 2).41,42 White matter bundles mediating the salient responses were limited to the ipsilateral cingulate bundle with a mirror pattern for right- and left-sided contacts (Figure 3). In contrast to the best and salient contacts, the no-behavior contacts shared no common pathways regardless of hemisphere.
This study characterizes the tractography patterns mediating the transient, stereotypical behavioral changes evoked by acute high-frequency SCC stimulation performed during DBS lead implantation surgery in patients with treatment-resistant depression. Double-blinded evaluations during the acute stimulation test of each contact revealed 2 behavioral response patterns. Type 1, a change in the interoceptive state, was reported with stimulation at more than 1 contact in the right or the left hemisphere for most patients. Type 2 responses were more multifaceted, with complex shifts in attentional focus, social connectedness, and interest occurring only with stimulation of left hemisphere contacts. Contacts eliciting a robust type 2 response also showed a type 1 response immediately preceding the self-referential and action-oriented declarations. These 2-part responses were consistently the contact categorized as best for every patient. Structural connectivity analyses further demonstrated that these distinct response types were mediated by a differential effect on the ventromedial frontal cortex and cingulate with bilateral frontal white matter tracts distinguishing best from merely salient contacts where only the cingulate was involved. The responses were described mostly as a relief and lessening of a negative state rather than the sudden appearance of a positive mood as described in other DBS targets, such as the nucleus accumbens, the medial forebrain bundle, or the ventral capsule.
Interoceptive awareness has been defined operationally as a perception of physiological changes in body states,43-45 with quantitative measures of autonomic reactivity often used as a physiological surrogate. The role of the ventral, anterior, and midcingulate in these behaviors is well established. Functional imaging studies have demonstrated activity changes in the SCC and the midcingulate to correlate with the blood pressure, heart rate, and skin conductance changes during a variety of mental and emotional tasks.46-57 Activation of these regions has been similarly reported in studies of provoked visceral, somatic, and emotional pain.58-63 These regions have been further classified as key components in the resting-state salience network64,65 that mediates shifts between personally relevant internal and external stimuli.66 Changes in the SCC and in the dorsal anterior and dorsal midcingulate activity are reported across antidepressant treatment trials using various interventions, including SCC DBS.67 Similar changes in these regions are among the most robust with acute sad mood induction.68,69 Across studies, left- and right-sided activity changes have been reported, which is consistent with the lack of lateralized effects seen herein with acute stimulation. Although other regions, including the anterior insula and frontal cortex, are clearly critical to a full interoceptive experience and a complete recovery from depression,70 the common involvement of the cingulate in the type 1 salient and type 2 best responses is nonetheless consistent with this principal role of the cingulate in interoceptive processing.
Exteroceptive awareness, in contrast, is defined as the perception of external stimuli and the shifting of attention away from the self toward the environment.71 Imaging studies have repeatedly demonstrated a role of the ventromedial frontal cortex in activities using exteroceptive engagement, such as mentalizing, self-knowledge, and outcome monitoring.72 Ventromedial frontal cortex activity has been demonstrated during self-referential processing of emotional words and pictures,73-75 in generating external vs internal emotional states,76 and in recognizing and imagining the experience of others.77,78 With particular relevance to depression, the medial frontal cortex has an important role in protecting the execution of long-term mental plans from immediate environmental or internal demands.79 Patients with depression commonly show hypoactivity of these regions and inappropriate deactivation during negative mood challenge.80,81 Not surprisingly, the medial frontal cortex has a critical role in the resting-state default mode network and has shown hyperconnectivity to the SCC in treatment-resistant depression.82 Failure of this system to shift toward external stimuli and instead remain stuck inside one’s self is the very definition of the treatment-resistant depressive state. The common involvement of bilateral medial frontal fibers linking directly to the SCC and passing fibers connecting the medial frontal cortex to the amygdala via the uncinate fasciculus during stimulation of the best contacts suggests a mechanism for the observed transient depression switch from a pervasive state stuck at negative to sensing a sudden capacity to get outside of one’s self.
The significant overlap of the tract patterns seen with the best contacts with those mediating long-term antidepressant response29 further suggests that these transient behavioral effects may provide a behavioral biomarker of the optimal site for long-term DBS, particularly at the left hemisphere contact. In support of this hypothesis, 7 of the 9 patients using the type 2 best left hemisphere contact had a response to treatment at 6 months.
This study has a number of limitations. Intraoperative testing is a strenuous task for patients, and some important responses may have been missed owing to the patient’s pain or fatigue. Therefore, the lack of response to a particular contact does not mean that a particular contact is not the desired target or affects a certain circuit. Clinical ratings, although performed in a double-blinded fashion, can still misinterpret some observations that are unique to individuals; furthermore, not all salient responses are of the same intensity. Continuous measures, such as heart rate, skin conductance, or facial or speech recognition technology, may be preferable for the capture of more nuanced phenomena than the categorical metrics used herein. Additional studies are focused on concurrent or antecedent changes in autonomic reactivity, facial expression, and speech output and electrophysiological changes in the SCC and frontal cortex. In regard to the tractography technique, resolution limits are set by the acquisition protocol and analytic methods. Therefore, other shared bundles, particularly small or more variable subcortical tracts, may have been missed.
At present, whether left-sided stimulation alone would be adequate to achieve a full antidepressant response remains untested because only bilateral stimulation has been evaluated. However, recent tractography studies of effective bilateral stimulation25 demonstrate that bilateral cingulum and uncinate fasciculus are needed in addition to crossing fibers within the forceps minor. Explicit comparisons of right- and left-sided unilateral stimulation await future investigation.
In treatment-resistant depression, pervasive negative mood and mental anguish, disinterest, social disengagement, and paucity of thought and action are prominent clinical features. We posit that acute stimulation in the requisite combination of fibers affecting the bilateral ventral frontal cortex, the anterior cingulate, and the midcingulate has immediate but transient effects on these core depression features and represents the first stage of depression network engagement required for long-term antidepressant effects of SCC DBS.
Corresponding Author: Ki Sueng Choi, PhD, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Emory University School of Medicine, 101 Woodruff Cir NE, Woodruff Memorial Research Bldg, Ste 4303, Atlanta, GA 30322 (kchoi8@emory.edu).
Accepted for Publication: July 29, 2015.
Published Online: September 26, 2015. doi:10.1001/jamaneurol.2015.2564.
Author Contributions: Drs Choi and Riva-Posse contributed equally to this work. Dr Choi had full access to all the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.
Study concept and design: All authors.
Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: All authors.
Drafting of the manuscript: Choi, Riva-Posse, Mayberg.
Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: All authors.
Statistical analysis: Choi.
Obtained funding: Mayberg.
Administrative, technical, or material support: Choi, Gross, Mayberg.
Study supervision: Gross, Mayberg.
Conflict of Interest Disclosures: Dr Gross reports serving as a paid consultant to Medtronic, Inc, and St Jude Medical Corp. Medtronic and St Jude Medical Corp develop products related to the research described in this article. Dr Mayberg reports consulting agreement with St Jude Medical Neuromodulation, which has licensed her intellectual property to develop SCC DBS for the treatment of severe depression (US 2005/0033379A1). The terms of these arrangements for Drs Mayberg and Gross have been reviewed and approved by Emory University in accordance with their conflict of interest policies. No other disclosures were reported.
Funding/Support: This study was supported by the Dana Foundation, by the Hope for Depression Research Foundation, and by grant R01 NS059736 from the National Institutes of Health. The study device was donated by St Jude Medical, Inc.
Role of the Funder/Sponsor: The funding sources had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, or interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.
Additional Contributions: Psychiatrists Steve Garlow, MD, PhD, and Andrea Crowell, MD, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Science, Emory University School of Medicine, screened, enrolled, and managed ongoing deep brain stimulation treatment of the course of the study. Justin Rajendra, BA, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Science, Emory University School of Medicine, provided image and data analysis; and Sinead Quinn, BA, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Science, Emory University School of Medicine, coordinated research. All contributors listed are coinvestigators or staff on the grants that supported this study.
1.Rezai
AR, Kopell
BH, Gross
RE,
et al. Deep brain stimulation for Parkinson’s disease: surgical issues.
Mov Disord. 2006;21(suppl 14):S197-S218.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 3.Nuttin
BJ, Gabriëls
LA, Cosyns
PR,
et al. Long-term electrical capsular stimulation in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder.
Neurosurgery. 2003;52(6):1263-1272.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 4.Okun
MS, Bowers
D, Springer
U,
et al. What’s in a “smile?” Intra-operative observations of contralateral smiles induced by deep brain stimulation.
Neurocase. 2004;10(4):271-279.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 6.Bejjani
BP, Damier
P, Arnulf
I,
et al. Transient acute depression induced by high-frequency deep-brain stimulation.
N Engl J Med. 1999;340(19):1476-1480.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 7.Bejjani
BP, Houeto
JL, Hariz
M,
et al. Aggressive behavior induced by intraoperative stimulation in the triangle of Sano.
Neurology. 2002;59(9):1425-1427.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 8.Rasche
D, Rinaldi
PC, Young
RF, Tronnier
VM. Deep brain stimulation for the treatment of various chronic pain syndromes.
Neurosurg Focus. 2006;21(6):1-8.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 9.Shapira
NA, Okun
MS, Wint
D,
et al. Panic and fear induced by deep brain stimulation.
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2006;77(3):410-412.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 10.Hamani
C, McAndrews
MP, Cohn
M,
et al. Memory enhancement induced by hypothalamic/fornix deep brain stimulation.
Ann Neurol. 2008;63(1):119-123.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 11.Mayberg
HS, Lozano
AM, Voon
V,
et al. Deep brain stimulation for treatment-resistant depression.
Neuron. 2005;45(5):651-660.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 12.Lozano
AM, Mayberg
HS, Giacobbe
P, Hamani
C, Craddock
RC, Kennedy
SH. Subcallosal cingulate gyrus deep brain stimulation for treatment-resistant depression.
Biol Psychiatry. 2008;64(6):461-467.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 13.Lozano
AM, Giacobbe
P, Hamani
C,
et al. A multicenter pilot study of subcallosal cingulate area deep brain stimulation for treatment-resistant depression.
J Neurosurg. 2012;116(2):315-322.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 14.Holtzheimer
PE, Kelley
ME, Gross
RE,
et al. Subcallosal cingulate deep brain stimulation for treatment-resistant unipolar and bipolar depression.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2012;69(2):150-158.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 15.Puigdemont
D, Pérez-Egea
R, Portella
MJ,
et al. Deep brain stimulation of the subcallosal cingulate gyrus: further evidence in treatment-resistant major depression.
Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2012;15(1):121-133.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 16.Kennedy
SH, Giacobbe
P, Rizvi
SJ,
et al. Deep brain stimulation for treatment-resistant depression: follow-up after 3 to 6 years.
Am J Psychiatry. 2011;168(5):502-510.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 17.Merkl
A, Schneider
GH, Schönecker
T,
et al. Antidepressant effects after short-term and chronic stimulation of the subgenual cingulate gyrus in treatment-resistant depression.
Exp Neurol. 2013;249:160-168.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 18.Ramasubbu
R, Anderson
S, Haffenden
A, Chavda
S, Kiss
ZH. Double-blind optimization of subcallosal cingulate deep brain stimulation for treatment-resistant depression: a pilot study.
J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2013;38(5):325-332.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 19.Guinjoan
SM, Mayberg
HS, Costanzo
EY,
et al. Asymmetrical contribution of brain structures to treatment-resistant depression as illustrated by effects of right subgenual cingulum stimulation.
J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2010;22(3):265-277.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 20.Schlaepfer
TE, Cohen
MX, Frick
C,
et al. Deep brain stimulation to reward circuitry alleviates anhedonia in refractory major depression.
Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33(2):368-377.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 21.Schlaepfer
TE, Bewernick
BH, Kayser
S, Mädler
B, Coenen
VA. Rapid effects of deep brain stimulation for treatment-resistant major depression.
Biol Psychiatry. 2013;73(12):1204-1212.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 22.Bewernick
BH, Hurlemann
R, Matusch
A,
et al. Nucleus accumbens deep brain stimulation decreases ratings of depression and anxiety in treatment-resistant depression.
Biol Psychiatry. 2010;67(2):110-116.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 23.Bewernick
BH, Kayser
S, Sturm
V, Schlaepfer
TE. Long-term effects of nucleus accumbens deep brain stimulation in treatment-resistant depression: evidence for sustained efficacy.
Neuropsychopharmacology. 2012;37(9):1975-1985.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 24.Stefurak
T, Mikulis
D, Mayberg
H,
et al. Deep brain stimulation for Parkinson’s disease dissociates mood and motor circuits: a functional MRI case study.
Mov Disord. 2003;18(12):1508-1516.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 25.Riva-Posse
P. Identification of critical white matter pathways involved in efficacious subcallosal cingulate deep brain stimulation: implications for prospective target selection [abstract].
Biol Psychiatry. 2014;7S(9):S15.
Google Scholar 28.American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th ed, text revision. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2000.
29.Riva-Posse
P, Choi
KS, Holtzheimer
PE,
et al. Defining critical white matter pathways mediating successful subcallosal cingulate deep brain stimulation for treatment-resistant depression.
Biol Psychiatry. 2014;76(12):963-969.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 30.Griswold
MA, Jakob
PM, Heidemann
RM,
et al. Generalized autocalibrating partially parallel acquisitions (GRAPPA).
Magn Reson Med. 2002;47(6):1202-1210.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 31.Smith
SM, Jenkinson
M, Woolrich
MW,
et al. Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL.
Neuroimage. 2004;23(suppl 1):S208-S219.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 34.Behrens
TE, Woolrich
MW, Jenkinson
M,
et al. Characterization and propagation of uncertainty in diffusion-weighted MR imaging.
Magn Reson Med. 2003;50(5):1077-1088.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 35.Greve
DN, Fischl
B. Accurate and robust brain image alignment using boundary-based registration.
Neuroimage. 2009;48(1):63-72.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 37.Lujan
JL, Chaturvedi
A, Malone
DA, Rezai
AR, Machado
AG, McIntyre
CC. Axonal pathways linked to therapeutic and nontherapeutic outcomes during psychiatric deep brain stimulation.
Hum Brain Mapp. 2012;33(4):958-968.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 38.McIntyre
CC, Miocinovic
S, Butson
CR. Computational analysis of deep brain stimulation.
Expert Rev Med Devices. 2007;4(5):615-622.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 40.Li
L, Rilling
JK, Preuss
TM, Glasser
MF, Hu
X. The effects of connection reconstruction method on the interregional connectivity of brain networks via diffusion tractography.
Hum Brain Mapp. 2012;33(8):1894-1913.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 41.Wakana
S, Caprihan
A, Panzenboeck
MM,
et al. Reproducibility of quantitative tractography methods applied to cerebral white matter.
Neuroimage. 2007;36(3):630-644.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 42.Hua
K, Zhang
J, Wakana
S,
et al. Tract probability maps in stereotaxic spaces: analyses of white matter anatomy and tract-specific quantification.
Neuroimage. 2008;39(1):336-347.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 44.Craig
AD. How do you feel? interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body.
Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002;3(8):655-666.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 46.Critchley
HD, Elliott
R, Mathias
CJ, Dolan
RJ. Neural activity relating to generation and representation of galvanic skin conductance responses: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study.
J Neurosci. 2000;20(8):3033-3040.
PubMedGoogle Scholar 47.Critchley
HD, Mathias
CJ, Josephs
O,
et al. Human cingulate cortex and autonomic control: converging neuroimaging and clinical evidence.
Brain. 2003;126(pt 10):2139-2152.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 48.Critchley
HD, Wiens
S, Rotshtein
P, Ohman
A, Dolan
RJ. Neural systems supporting interoceptive awareness.
Nat Neurosci. 2004;7(2):189-195.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 50.Critchley
HD, Eccles
J, Garfinkel
SN. Interaction between cognition, emotion, and the autonomic nervous system.
Handb Clin Neurol. 2013;117:59-77.
PubMedGoogle Scholar 53.Pereira
EA, Wang
S, Paterson
DJ, Stein
JF, Aziz
TZ, Green
AL. Sustained reduction of hypertension by deep brain stimulation.
J Clin Neurosci. 2010;17(1):124-127.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 54.Pollatos
O, Kirsch
W, Schandry
R. Brain structures involved in interoceptive awareness and cardioafferent signal processing: a dipole source localization study.
Hum Brain Mapp. 2005;26(1):54-64.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 56.Lane
RD, Fink
GR, Chau
PM, Dolan
RJ. Neural activation during selective attention to subjective emotional responses.
Neuroreport. 1997;8(18):3969-3972.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 58.Vogt
BA, Sikes
RW. The medial pain system, cingulate cortex, and parallel processing of nociceptive information.
Prog Brain Res. 2000;122:223-235.
PubMedGoogle Scholar 61.Wager
TD, van Ast
VA, Hughes
BL, Davidson
ML, Lindquist
MA, Ochsner
KN. Brain mediators of cardiovascular responses to social threat, part II: prefrontal-subcortical pathways and relationship with anxiety.
Neuroimage. 2009;47(3):836-851.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 63.Mayer
EA, Gupta
A, Kilpatrick
LA, Hong
JY. Imaging brain mechanisms in chronic visceral pain.
Pain. 2015;156(suppl 1):S50-S63.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 65.Seeley
WW, Menon
V, Schatzberg
AF,
et al. Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control.
J Neurosci. 2007;27(9):2349-2356.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 66.Sridharan
D, Levitin
DJ, Menon
V. A critical role for the right fronto-insular cortex in switching between central-executive and default-mode networks.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2008;105(34):12569-12574.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 68.Mayberg
HS, Liotti
M, Brannan
SK,
et al. Reciprocal limbic-cortical function and negative mood: converging PET findings in depression and normal sadness.
Am J Psychiatry. 1999;156(5):675-682.
PubMedGoogle Scholar 69.Liotti
M, Mayberg
HS, Brannan
SK, McGinnis
S, Jerabek
P, Fox
PT. Differential limbic-cortical correlates of sadness and anxiety in healthy subjects: implications for affective disorders.
Biol Psychiatry. 2000;48(1):30-42.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 71.Damasio
AR. The somatic marker hypothesis and the possible functions of the prefrontal cortex.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1996;351(1346):1413-1420.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 73.Fossati
P. Neural correlates of emotion processing: from emotional to social brain.
Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2012;22(suppl 3):S487-S491.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 75.Simpson
JR
Jr, Snyder
AZ, Gusnard
DA, Raichle
ME. Emotion-induced changes in human medial prefrontal cortex, I: during cognitive task performance.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2001;98(2):683-687.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 76.Reiman
EM, Lane
RD, Ahern
GL,
et al. Neuroanatomical correlates of externally and internally generated human emotion.
Am J Psychiatry. 1997;154(7):918-925.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 77.Nyberg
L, Kim
AS, Habib
R, Levine
B, Tulving
E. Consciousness of subjective time in the brain.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010;107(51):22356-22359.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 78.Addis
DR, Pan
L, Vu
MA, Laiser
N, Schacter
DL. Constructive episodic simulation of the future and the past: distinct subsystems of a core brain network mediate imagining and remembering.
Neuropsychologia. 2009;47(11):2222-2238.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 79.Koechlin
E, Hyafil
A. Anterior prefrontal function and the limits of human decision-making.
Science. 2007;318(5850):594-598.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 80.Liotti
M, Mayberg
HS. The role of functional neuroimaging in the neuropsychology of depression.
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2001;23(1):121-136.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 81.Krüger
S, Seminowicz
D, Goldapple
K, Kennedy
SH, Mayberg
HS. State and trait influences on mood regulation in bipolar disorder: blood flow differences with an acute mood challenge.
Biol Psychiatry. 2003;54(11):1274-1283.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 82.Greicius
MD, Flores
BH, Menon
V,
et al. Resting-state functional connectivity in major depression: abnormally increased contributions from subgenual cingulate cortex and thalamus.
Biol Psychiatry. 2007;62(5):429-437.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref