Key PointsQuestion
Does playing high school football have a statistically and clinically significant adverse association with cognitive impairment and depression at 65 years of age?
Findings
In this cohort study using data from the Wisconsin Longitudinal Study among men graduating high school in Wisconsin in 1957, there was no statistically or clinically significant harmful association between playing football in high school and increased cognitive impairment or depression later in life, on average.
Meaning
For men who attended high school in the late 1950s, playing high school football did not appear to be a major risk factor for later-life cognitive impairment or depression; for current athletes, this study provides information on the risk of playing sports today that have a similar head trauma exposure risk as high school football played in the 1950s.
Importance
American football is the largest participation sport in US high schools and is a leading cause of concussion among adolescents. Little is known about the long-term cognitive and mental health consequences of exposure to football-related head trauma at the high school level.
Objective
To estimate the association of playing high school football with cognitive impairment and depression at 65 years of age.
Design, Setting, and Participants
A representative sample of male high school students who graduated from high school in Wisconsin in 1957 was studied. In this cohort study using data from the Wisconsin Longitudinal Study, football players were matched between March 1 and July 1, 2017, with controls along several baseline covariates such as adolescent IQ, family background, and educational level. For robustness, 3 versions of the control condition were considered: all controls, those who played a noncollision sport, and those who did not play any sport.
Exposures
Athletic participation in high school football.
Main Outcomes and Measures
A composite cognition measure of verbal fluency and memory and attention constructed from results of cognitive assessments administered at 65 years of age. A modified Center for Epidemiological Studies’ Depression Scale score was used to measure depression. Secondary outcomes include results of individual cognitive tests, anger, anxiety, hostility, and heavy use of alcohol.
Results
Among the 3904 men (mean [SD] age, 64.4 [0.8] years at time of primary outcome measurement) in the study, after matching and model-based covariate adjustment, compared with each control condition, there was no statistically significant harmful association of playing football with a reduced composite cognition score (–0.04 reduction in cognition vs all controls; 97.5% CI, –0.14 to 0.05) or an increased modified Center for Epidemiological Studies’ Depression Scale depression score (–1.75 reduction vs all controls; 97.5% CI, –3.24 to –0.26). After adjustment for multiple testing, playing football did not have a significant adverse association with any of the secondary outcomes, such as the likelihood of heavy alcohol use at 65 years of age (odds ratio, 0.68; 95% CI, 0.32-1.43).
Conclusions and Relevance
Cognitive and depression outcomes later in life were found to be similar for high school football players and their nonplaying counterparts from mid-1950s in Wisconsin. The risks of playing football today might be different than in the 1950s, but for current athletes, this study provides information on the risk of playing sports today that have a similar risk of head trauma as high school football played in the 1950s.
More than 1 million students played high school American football in 2014,1 but, recently, many have questioned the safety of the sport2,3 or even called for its ban.4 These concerns have been driven partially by reports of chronic traumatic encephalopathy,5,6 increased risks of neurodegenerative disease,7 and associations between a history of concussion and cognitive impairment and depression later in life8-10 among retired professional football players.
Although they are concerning, most reports of chronic traumatic encephalopathy among retired professional football players are based on posthumously donated brains and are potentially affected by referral bias. One exception, a study by Bieniek et al,11 examined a larger, nonselected brain bank and found a higher rate of chronic traumatic encephalopathy among former athletes than nonathletes. However, they determined participation in contact sports by querying only medical records, potentially underestimating the true rate of participation and leading to information bias.12 Without population-level samples, it is difficult to estimate the base rate of long-term dysfunction among professional football players, much less among high school players.
There has been limited work examining the association of playing high school football with cognitive impairment and depression later in life. One study found an association between increased exposure to head trauma and cognitive impairment and depression among former high school and college players.13 A population-level study by Savica et al14 found that high school football players were not at a higher risk of neurodegenerative disease than their male classmates who did not play football. Although they are informative, these studies have limitations for elucidating an association between playing football and cognitive impairment; the former study13 used volunteer participants, and the latter14 did not control for any potential confounders, such as adolescent IQ or family background.15-18
In the absence of a randomized clinical trial, the criterion standard for studying the association of an exposure with an outcome is a prospective longitudinal study that follows a population sample and measures many potential confounders.19-21 We use data from such a study, the Wisconsin Longitudinal Study (WLS) of graduates from Wisconsin high schools in 1957,22 to study the long-term association of playing high school football with cognitive impairment and depression. The WLS contains a rich set of baseline variables related to family background, adolescent characteristics including adolescent IQ, and educational level that all may be linked to health in later life,15-18 high school football participation, and cognitive and psychological well-being assessments at 54, 65, and 72 years of age.
After constructing the participant matches but before examining the outcome data, we registered the study at clincialtrials.gov (identifier: NCT02833129) and posted our protocol with matching results online to arXiv (identifier: arXiv:1607.01756), as recommended by Rubin.23 The University of Pennsylvania institutional review board approved the protocol for this matched observational study, and WLS interviewers obtained oral informed consent from participants.
In 1957, the WLS randomly sampled 10 317 Wisconsin high school graduates (one-third of all graduates) and has observed them since then.22,24 Data on participation in football were recorded following systematic reviews of high school yearbooks between January 2005 and February 2011. Participation data were unavailable for 1018 of the 4991 men (20.4%) in the WLS. An additional 69 men (1.4%) played sports with a high incidence of head trauma (eg, soccer, hockey, wrestling, and lacrosse). The remaining 3904 men (78.2%) were eligible for this study. Of these men, primary outcomes were unavailable for 1212 (31.0%), who were removed from our primary analysis. Of the remaining 2692 men, 834 (31.0%) played football and 1858 did not. Further details of eligibility and inclusion criteria can be found in the eAppendix in the Supplement. The WLS data set does not include history of concussion and total exposure to football prior to high school. These factors have been associated with cognitive decline in later life,8,9,13,25 but we were unable to explicitly control for them in our analysis.
Primary and Secondary Outcomes
We considered 2 primary outcomes measured at 65 years of age (mean [SD] age, 64.4 [0.8] years) related to depression and cognitive impairment. We measured depression with the WLS-modified Center for Epidemiological Studies’ Depression Scale (CES-D).26 We constructed a composite cognition measure by averaging the z scores from the Letter Fluency (LF) and Delayed Word Recall (DWR) tests. Of the measures of language and executive function and verbal memory attention in the WLS, these were the most consistent with the National Institute of Health’s recommended Common Data Elements in Traumatic Brain Injury research.27 A meta-analysis of the neuropsychological effects of mild traumatic brain injury identified LF as the task most sensitive to exposure.28 Impairments in DWR may be particularly relevant to cognitive defects later in life associated with chronic head injuries because prior research has identified associations between sports-related concussions and preclinical Alzheimer disease.8 For individuals missing either an LF or DWR score, we used the available z score instead of the mean of the 2 scores.
Secondary outcomes were the composite cognition score at 72 years of age, modified CES-D scores at 54 and 72 years of age, individual cognitive test scores, hostility indices, anxiety and anger indices derived from the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory29 and State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory-II,30 and whether the individual reported consuming 5 or more alcoholic drinks on at least 5 separate occasions in the month preceding the WLS interview at 54, 65, and 72 years of age.
Regular physical activity in early adulthood has been associated with slower rates of cognitive decline,31 potentially mediating any negative association of playing football and cognitive impairment. Accordingly, we examined whether football players were more likely than controls to engage in such activity at 35 years of age. See the eAppendix in the Supplement for more details on the outcomes.
To control for potential confounding variables, we used full matching to create matched sets containing 1 football player and 1 or more controls or 1 or more football players and 1 control. These sets optimally balance the distribution of the baseline variables listed in Table 1 between football players and controls.32 Our full matching procedure first applies a propensity score caliper33 and then minimizes the rank-based Mahalanobis distance between matched participants with similar propensity scores.34 Our objective in matching is to achieve standardized differences between exposed participants and controls on baseline variables below 0.2 SDs. Regression-based covariate adjustment has been shown to remove biases due to covariate imbalance of this magnitude.35,36
Our control group consisted of all participants who did not play high school football (all controls). We also considered 2 subsets as alternative control groups: those who did not play any high school sports (nonsport controls) and those who played a noncollision sport but did not play football (noncollision sport controls); eTable 2 in the Supplement tabulates participation of eligible individuals by sport. These alternative control groups may differ along unmeasured characteristics, such as personality and fitness, that could affect our outcomes. A convincing study would show consistent evidence (or lack thereof) of an exposure association across comparisons with each control group. Comparability of the 2 alternative control groups would suggest that such unmeasured characteristics did not significantly influence the outcomes of interest.37 To facilitate these 4 comparisons—football vs all controls, football vs noncollision sport controls, football vs nonsport controls, and noncollision sport vs nonsport controls—we built 4 separate full matches that were exactly matched on availability of LF, DWR, and modified CES-D scores at 65 years of age (eAppendix in the Supplement). Matching was performed prior to analysis between March 1 and July 1, 2017.
Attrition Analysis for the Availability of Outcome
eTable 3 in the Supplement shows the availability of primary outcomes. To examine whether playing high school football increased the likelihood of attrition from the WLS or the availability of outcomes at 65 years of age, we fitted logistic regressions to estimate the availability of the LF, DWR, and modified CES-D scores at 65 years of age using all baseline covariates and an exposure indicator.
Test Statistic for Exposure Association
For each continuous outcome, we fitted a multiple regression model using the same baseline covariates used in the matching, indicators for the matched sets, and an exposure indicator. We tested for an exposure association using a 2-tailed t test. P < .05 was considered significant. The combination of full matching and regression is robust and efficient.37-40 An identical procedure was used for binary outcomes except we fitted a conditional logit regression model and reported the outcomes as odds ratios (ORs). For each outcome variable, we judged effect sizes by the popular criterion attributed to Cohen: 0.2 SDs for small effects, 0.5 SDs for medium effects, and 0.8 SDs for large effects.41 For the combined cognition score, these cutoffs (on the absolute scale) are 0.16 for small effects, 0.41 for medium effects, and 0.65 for large effects; for the modified CES-D score, 2.56 for small effects, 6.41 for medium effects, and 10.25 for large effects.
Test for Unmeasured Confounding Using Postexposure Variables
Since acute cognitive dysfunction following concussions or mild traumatic brain injury resolves within 3 months in most patients,42-46 we hypothesized that any medium-term differences in outcomes before 35 years of age between football players and controls would be due to unmeasured differences that existed prior to high school graduation. To assess the presence of these differences, we tested whether playing football was associated with several medium-term postexposure variables (military service, years of education, occupational prestige of job held in 1974, and earnings in 1974) at α = .05, with no correction for multiple testing.
Ordered Hypothesis Testing
We tested each primary outcome at α = .025 and reported 97.5% CIs so that the overall familywise error rate was 0.05. To perform the aforementioned comparisons with different control groups without losing power owing to multiple testing, we used an ordered testing procedure47 (eAppendix in the Supplement).
For testing the association of playing football with the secondary outcomes, we used only matched sets constructed using all controls. We adjusted for multiple testing of secondary outcomes with the Benjamini-Hochberg procedure,48 controlling the false discovery rate at α = .05. Because physical activity at 35 years of age could mediate the association of playing football with the primary outcomes, we tested for football’s association with physical activity at α = .05 using the same matches as the primary analysis.
The number of ε4 variants of the apolipoprotein E gene (APOE [OMIM 107741]), a risk factor for developing Alzheimer disease,49 was available for a limited subset of participants. We assessed the balance of the number of ε4 variants and the availability of APOE data between football players and each control group. We repeated our primary analysis restricted to participants with APOE data and included the number of ε4 variants in the match. We then tested whether playing high school football and the presence of the APOE ε4 variant has an interaction association with long-term cognitive or mental health outcomes.
The availability of outcomes at multiple time points enabled us to compare rates of cognitive decline among football players and all controls. For each individual cognitive test and the modified CES-D score, we fitted a mixed-effects model with fixed effects for playing football, age, and their interaction, and a time-invariant random effect for each participant. We also included the covariates from Table 1 as fixed effects. This analysis without matching includes many of the 1212 individuals who were excluded from the primary analysis because they lacked LF, DWR, and modified CES-D scores at 65 years of age. We used a Benjamini-Hochberg correction to control the false discovery rate at α = .05.
Football players were not significantly more or less likely than controls to be missing the LF score (OR, 1.07; 97.5% CI, 0.90-1.28; P = .37), the DWR score (OR, 1.08; 97.5% CI, 0.91-1.29; P = .31), or the modified CES-D score (OR, 1.15; 97.5% CI, 0.96-1.39; P = .09) at 65 years of age.
Table 1 shows the standardized differences from matching football players with all controls. Before matching, the 2 groups were adequately balanced (standardized differences, <0.2) on all variables except high school size, participation in school government, and planned years of future education. After matching, all standardized differences were less than 0.2, so our covariate adjustment could remove remaining differences in the covariates.35,36 The other matches were similar (eTables 4-6 in the Supplement). The distributions of IQ, planned years of future education, and parental income for football players and matched controls were very similar (eFigure in the Supplement). eTables 7 to 11 in the Supplement provide further details on the matching results.
Testing Unmeasured Confounding Using Postexposure Variables
After matching, we found that playing high school football was not significantly associated with serving in the military between 1957 and 1975 (OR, 1.05; 97.5% CI, 0.91-1.22; P = .42), years of education up to 1975 (1.04; 97.5% CI, 0.87-1.25; P = .62), occupational prestige of job held in 1974 (–6.86; 97.5% CI, –29.67 to 15.96; P = .50), or earnings in 1974 (10.30; 97.5% CI, –1.85 to 22.45; P = .06). The distributions of postexposure variables were similar across football players and each version of the matched controls (eFigure in the Supplement).
The distributions of the primary outcomes for football players and each version of the matched controls were similar (Figure 1). Table 2 shows the estimated treatment effect in each of the 4 comparisons.
Football players did not have significantly different composite cognition scores than all controls (–0.04; 97.5% CI, –0.14 to 0.05; P = .37), and the 97.5% CI contained only effects smaller than the small effects cutoff (0.15) (Table 2). Compared with each control group, football players’ composite cognition scores were not significantly different, although when compared with nonsport controls, the 97.5% CI contained some effects above the small-size cutoff but none above the medium-size cutoff (0.41). Noncollision sport controls and nonsport controls were also not significantly different (–0.09; 97.5% CI, –0.20 to 0.03; P = .10).
Football players’ modified CES-D scores were significantly lower (–1.75; 97.5% CI, –3.24 to –0.26; P = .01) than all controls, meaning that they reported fewer depressive symptoms (Table 2). The 97.5% CI suggests that playing high school football could have a beneficial effect above the small-size cutoff (2.56) but not above the medium-size cutoff (6.4). Because the 97.5% CI contains only beneficial effects of football on depression, there is no evidence that football is associated with increased depression. Compared with each control group, we obtained similar findings, although the association of playing football with depression was not significant when compared with nonsport controls (–1.27; 97.5% CI, –2.89 to 0.35; P = .08). Noncollision sport controls and nonsport controls’ modified CES-D scores were also significantly different (0.67; 97.5% CI, –1.19 to 2.52; P = .42).
Table 3 shows the estimated association of playing football with secondary outcomes when comparing football players with all controls.50 After Benjamini-Hochberg correction, no estimated effects were significant. The estimates and 97.5% CIs for the association of playing football with cognitive impairment at 72 years of age (–0.01; 97.5% CI, –0.12 to 0.10) are similar to those at 65 years of age (–0.04; 97.5% CI, –0.14 to 0.05; P = .37). Relative to all controls, football players had lower modified CES-D scores at 54 years of age (−0.96; 97.5% CI, −2.65 to 0.73; unadjusted P = .20) and 72 years of age (−0.62; 97.5% CI, −2.78 to 1.53; unadjusted P = .52), but the differences were not statistically significant, and the 97.5% CIs contained only values smaller than the small effect cutoff.
Apart from the Immediate Word Recall test, none of the 97.5% CIs for individual cognitive tests contained values larger than the small effect cutoff. After Benjamini-Hochberg correction, the difference in Immediate Word Recall test scores at 72 years of age between football players and all controls was not significant (0.20; 97.5 CI, −0.02 to 0.41; unadjusted P = .04) (Table 3). Football players’ anger, anxiety, and hostility indices at all ages were not significantly different from all controls’ indices. Football players were not significantly more likely than controls to consume a large amount of alcohol at 54 years of age (0.49; 97.5% CI, 0.21-1.14; unadjusted P = .06), 65 years of age (0.68; 97.5% CI, 0.32-1.43; unadjusted P = .25), and 72 years of age (1.11; 97.5% CI, 0.18-6.68; unadjusted P = .90).
Compared with all controls and nonsport controls, at 35 years of age, football players were significantly more likely to engage in regular moderate to vigorous physical activity (all controls: OR, 1.23; 97.5% CI, 1.02-1.48; P = .02; nonsport controls: OR, 1.37; 97.5% CI, 1.11-1.70; P = .001). Football players were not significantly more or less likely to engage in such activity than noncollision sport controls. Noncollision sport controls were significantly more likely to engage in such activity than were nonsport controls.
Interaction of APOE ε4 Status With Playing High School Football
When we repeated our analysis using only individuals with available genotype data, our findings were similar (eTables 11-22 in the Supplement). The presence of an additional APOE ε4 variant does not significantly modify the association of playing high school football with modified CES-D scores (–1.49; 97.5% CI, –3.39 to 0.41; P = .08) or composite cognitive scores (0.01; 97.5% CI –0.11 to 0.14; P = .80) at 65 years of age (eTable 23 in the Supplement).
Mixed-Effects Models for Longitudinal Data
Figure 2 plots the longitudinal trajectories of mean scores for football players with mean covariate values as well as mean counterfactual scores for these same players had they not played football. After Benjamini-Hochberg correction, the association of playing football with these outcomes at all ages was not significant (eTable 24 in the Supplement). The changes in football players’ and controls’ test scores over time were not significantly different.
Among men who graduated from Wisconsin high schools in 1957, playing high school football was not adversely associated with cognitive impairment or depression later in life. Furthermore, the corresponding 97.5% CIs contain only small positive and negative effect sizes. Our findings are consistent with those of Savica et al,14 who found no differences in the incidence rates of neurodegenerative disease among football players and their non–football-playing classmates in a contemporaneous cohort.
Athletes and parents may consider our findings when weighing the potential risks and benefits of participation in high school athletics. Participation in high school sports, even contact sports with a high risk of head trauma, has potential benefits, including promoting physical activity, teamwork, and recreation. Indeed, in our cohort, playing any high school sport, football or otherwise, was associated with increased physical activity at 35 years of age. However, such participation is not without risk, and our findings do not speak to the benefits of common-sense measures, such as improving concussion management protocols.
Strengths and Limitations
Our study has several strengths along with some limitations. Strengths include the use of prospectively collected longitudinal data from a large population sample and careful control for several important confounders, including adolescent IQ, high school rank, and parent’s educational level. Using multiple control groups and comparing postexposure variables thought to be unaffected by playing football, we found no evidence of unmeasured confounding. However, because 31.0% of eligible participants were missing both primary outcomes, our results may be limited by selection bias. The attrition analysis found that football players were not significantly more or less likely to be missing either primary outcome, somewhat mitigating this concern.
Our findings may not generalize to current high school football players owing to changes in playing style, training technique, protective equipment, and rules aimed at improving safety. Although high school football today may be safer as a result of these changes, it is possible that the frequency and severity of head impacts have increased owing to the faster pace of the game, the larger body size of the players,51 or an increased perception of safety.52 Exposure to repeated head trauma before 12 years of age may lead to cognitive decline later in life,53 but information on the age of first exposure is unavailable in the data set.
Although we did not find evidence of a large mean association of playing high school football with cognitive impairment and depression later in life, some subgroups of football players may have an elevated risk of dysfunction later in life. Several studies have found that a history of multiple concussions may have long-term cognitive and behavioral consequences8,9,13,25 and that the frequency and severity of head impacts vary by team position (eg, quarterback, receiver).54 Concussion history and position information were unavailable. Further research is needed to examine the risks for these subgroups.
Among men graduating from high school in Wisconsin in 1957, we did not find evidence that playing football had a negative long-term association with cognitive functioning and mental health at 65 and 72 years of age. Although our findings may not generalize to current high school football players, they may be relevant to current athletes playing contact sports with similar mean levels of head trauma as among the WLS football players. Repeating our analysis with a younger cohort as they reach 65 years of age may improve our understanding of how the risks of playing football have evolved over time.
Accepted for Publication: April 7, 2017.
Corresponding Author: Dylan S. Small, PhD, Department of Statistics, The Wharton School, University of Pennsylvania, 400 Huntsman Hall, 3730 Walnut St, Philadelphia, PA 19104 (dsmall@wharton.upenn.edu).
Published Online: July 3, 2017. doi:10.1001/jamaneurol.2017.1317
Author Contributions: Messrs Deshpande and Hasegawa had full access to all the data in the study and take responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis. Messrs Deshpande and Hasegawa contributed equally to this work.
Study concept and design: Deshpande, Hasegawa, Rabinowitz, Whyte, Baiocchi, Karlawish, Master, Small.
Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: All authors.
Drafting of the manuscript: Deshpande, Hasegawa, Small.
Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: All authors.
Statistical analysis: Deshpande, Hasegawa, Tabatabaei, Baiocchi, Small.
Administrative, technical, or material support: Roan.
Study supervision: Small.
Conflict of Interest Disclosures: None reported.
Additional Contributions: Paul Rosenbaum, PhD, Department of Statistics, The Wharton School, University of Pennsylvania, provided assistance with the statistical methods; Joe Savard, BA, Department of Sociology, University of Wisconsin–Madison, assisted with accessing the Wisconsin Longitudinal Study data; and Lynn Selhat, MS, The Wharton School, University of Pennsylvania, assisted with copyediting comments. They were not compensated for their contributions.
1.National Federation of State High School Associations (NFHS). NFHS Handbook 2015-16. Indianapolis, IN: NFHS; 2015.
3.Pfister
T, Pfister
K, Hagel
B, Ghali
WA, Ronksley
PE. The incidence of concussion in youth sports: a systematic review and meta-analysis.
Br J Sports Med. 2016;50(5):292-297.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 5.Omalu
BI, DeKosky
ST, Minster
RL, Kamboh
MI, Hamilton
RL, Wecht
CH. Chronic traumatic encephalopathy in a National Football League player.
Neurosurgery. 2005;57(1):128-134.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 7.Lehman
EJ, Hein
MJ, Baron
SL, Gersic
CM. Neurodegenerative causes of death among retired National Football League players.
Neurology. 2012;79(19):1970-1974.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 8.Guskiewicz
KM, Marshall
SW, Bailes
J,
et al. Association between recurrent concussion and late-life cognitive impairment in retired professional football players.
Neurosurgery. 2005;57(4):719-726.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 9.Guskiewicz
KM, Marshall
SW, Bailes
J,
et al. Recurrent concussion and risk of depression in retired professional football players.
Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2007;39(6):903-909.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 10.Hart
J
Jr, Kraut
MA, Womack
KB,
et al. Neuroimaging of cognitive dysfunction and depression in aging retired National Football League players: a cross-sectional study.
JAMA Neurol. 2013;70(3):326-335.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 11.Bieniek
KF, Ross
OA, Cormier
KA,
et al. Chronic traumatic encephalopathy pathology in a neurodegenerative disorders brain bank.
Acta Neuropathol. 2015;130(6):877-889.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 12.Rothman
KJ, Lash
TL, Greenland
S. Modern Epidemiology. Philadelphia, PA: Lipincott Williams and Wilkins; 2008.
13.Montenigro
PH, Alosco
ML, Martin
BM,
et al. Cumulative head impact exposure predicts later-life depression, apathy, executive dysfunction, and cognitive impairment in former high school and college football players.
J Neurotrauma. 2017;34(2):328-340.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 14.Savica
R, Parisi
JE, Wold
LE, Josephs
KA, Ahlskog
JE. High school football and risk of neurodegeneration: a community-based study.
Mayo Clin Proc. 2012;87(4):335-340.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 15.Der
G, Batty
GD, Deary
IJ. The association between IQ in adolescence and a range of health outcomes at 40 in the 1979 US National Longitudinal Study of Youth.
Intelligence. 2009;37(6):573-580.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 16.Rogers
MA, Plassman
BL, Kabeto
M,
et al. Parental education and late-life dementia in the United States.
J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol. 2009;22(1):71-80.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 17.Everson-Rose
SA, Mendes de Leon
CF, Bienias
JL, Wilson
RS, Evans
DA. Early life conditions and cognitive functioning in later life.
Am J Epidemiol. 2003;158(11):1083-1089.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 18.Kaplan
GA, Turrell
G, Lynch
JW, Everson
SA, Helkala
E-L, Salonen
JT. Childhood socioeconomic position and cognitive function in adulthood.
Int J Epidemiol. 2001;30(2):256-263.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 19.Ban
VS, Madden
CJ, Bailes
JE, Hunt Batjer
H, Lonser
RR. The science and questions surrounding chronic traumatic encephalopathy.
Neurosurg Focus. 2016;40(4):E15.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 20.Gardner
RC, Yaffe
K. Epidemiology of mild traumatic brain injury and neurodegenerative disease.
Mol Cell Neurosci. 2015;66(pt B):75-80.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 21.Seichepine
DR, Stamm
JM, Daneshvar
DH,
et al. Profile of self-reported problems with executive functioning in college and professional football players.
J Neurotrauma. 2013;30(14):1299-1304.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 23.Rubin
DB. The design versus the analysis of observational studies for causal effects: parallels with the design of randomized trials.
Stat Med. 2007;26(1):20-36.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 24.Hauser
RM. The Wisconsin Longitudinal Study: designing a study of the life course. In: Elder
GH
Jr, Giele
JZ, eds. The Craft of Life Course Research. New York, NY: Guilford Press;2008:29-50.
25.Guskiewicz
KM, McCrea
M, Marshall
SW,
et al. Cumulative effects associated with recurrent concussion in collegiate football players: the NCAA Concussion Study.
JAMA. 2003;290(19):2549-2555.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 27.Wilde
EA, Whiteneck
GG, Bogner
J,
et al. Recommendations for the use of common outcome measures in traumatic brain injury research.
Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2010;91(11):1650-1660.e17.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 28.Belanger
HG, Curtiss
G, Demery
JA, Lebowitz
BK, Vanderploeg
RD. Factors moderating neuropsychological outcomes following mild traumatic brain injury: a meta-analysis.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2005;11(3):215-227.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 29.Spielberger
CD, Gorsuch
R, Lushene
RE, Vagg
P, Jacobs
GA. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (Form Y1). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press; 1983.
30.Spielberger
CD. State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources; 1999.
31.Richards
M, Hardy
R, Wadsworth
MEJ. Does active leisure protect cognition? evidence from a national birth cohort.
Soc Sci Med. 2003;56(4):785-792.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 32.Rosenbaum
PR. A characterization of optimal designs for observational studies.
J R Stat Soc B. 1991;53(3):597-610.
Google Scholar 33.Rosenbaum
PR.
Design of Observational Studies. New York, NY: Springer; 2010.
Crossref 35.Silber
JH, Rosenbaum
PR, Trudeau
ME,
et al. Multivariate matching and bias reduction in the surgical outcomes study.
Med Care. 2001;39(10):1048-1064.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 37.Rosenbaum
PR.
Observational Studies. New York, NY: Springer; 2002.
Crossref 41.Cohen
J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. 2nd ed. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1988.
42.Schretlen
DJ, Shapiro
AM. A quantitative review of the effects of traumatic brain injury on cognitive functioning.
Int Rev Psychiatry. 2003;15(4):341-349.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 43.Vanderploeg
RD, Curtiss
G, Belanger
HG. Long-term neuropsychological outcomes following mild traumatic brain injury.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2005;11(3):228-236.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 44.Rabinowitz
AR, Li
X, McCauley
SR,
et al. Prevalence and predictors of poor recovery from mild traumatic brain injury.
J Neurotrauma. 2015;32(19):1488-1496.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 45.McCrea
M, Guskiewicz
K, Randolph
C,
et al. Incidence, clinical course, and predictors of prolonged recovery time following sport-related concussion in high school and college athletes.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2013;19(1):22-33.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 46.McCrea
M, Guskiewicz
KM, Marshall
SW,
et al. Acute effects and recovery time following concussion in collegiate football players: the NCAA Concussion Study.
JAMA. 2003;290(19):2556-2563.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 49.Poirier
J, Davignon
J, Bouthillier
D, Kogan
S, Bertrand
P, Gauthier
S. Apolipoprotein E polymorphism and Alzheimer’s disease.
Lancet. 1993;342(8873):697-699.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 51.Anzell
AR, Potteiger
JA, Kraemer
WJ, Otieno
S. Changes in height, body weight, and body composition in American football players from 1942 to 2011.
J Strength Cond Res. 2013;27(2):277-284.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 53.Stamm
JM, Koerte
IK, Muehlmann
M,
et al. Age at first exposure to football is associated with altered corpus callosum white matter microstructure in former professional football players.
J Neurotrauma. 2015;32(22):1768-1776.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 54.Broglio
SP, Eckner
JT, Martini
D, Sosnoff
JJ, Kutcher
JS, Randolph
C. Cumulative head impact burden in high school football.
J Neurotrauma. 2011;28(10):2069-2078.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref