[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Figure 1.  Proportion of UK Biobank Participants Exhibiting a Cognitive Deficit at Baseline Testing
Proportion of UK Biobank Participants Exhibiting a Cognitive Deficit at Baseline Testing

The cross-sectional data showing the proportion (with 95% CIs) of 32 038 UK Biobank participants with a cognitive deficit (a failing score on 2 or more of 4 tests), according to quintile of retinal nerve fiber layer (RNFL) thickness measured in the outer nasal retinal subfield by optical coherence tomography.

Figure 2.  Proportion of UK Biobank Participants Exhibiting a Decline in Cognitive Function on Repeat Assessment
Proportion of UK Biobank Participants Exhibiting a Decline in Cognitive Function on Repeat Assessment

The number of cognitive tests with worse scores on follow-up testing was significantly associated with baseline retional nerve fiber layer (RNFL) thickness. The regression coefficient was 1.2 μm per test failed (P < .001). After controlling for potential confounders, including age, sex, race/ethnicity, Townsend deprivation index, height, refraction, and intraocular pressure, the regression coefficient was 1.1 μm per test failed (P < .001).

Table 1.  Baseline Characteristics (2009-2010) of All Participants Recruited With Baseline Optical Coherence Tomography (OCT) Results, Those Included in This Study, and Those With Follow-up Assessment (2012-2013)
Baseline Characteristics (2009-2010) of All Participants Recruited With Baseline Optical Coherence Tomography (OCT) Results, Those Included in This Study, and Those With Follow-up Assessment (2012-2013)
Table 2.  Multivariable Logistic Regression Modeling of the Association Between Retinal Nerve Fiber Layer (RNFL) Thickness and Risk of Failing 1 or More Tests (Compared With 0 Tests) at Baselinea
Multivariable Logistic Regression Modeling of the Association Between Retinal Nerve Fiber Layer (RNFL) Thickness and Risk of Failing 1 or More Tests (Compared With 0 Tests) at Baselinea
Table 3.  Multivariable Logistic Regression Modeling of the Association Between Retinal Nerve Fiber Layer (RNFL) Thickness and Risk of Worsening on 1 or More Follow-up Cognitive Function Tests (Compared With 0 Tests)a
Multivariable Logistic Regression Modeling of the Association Between Retinal Nerve Fiber Layer (RNFL) Thickness and Risk of Worsening on 1 or More Follow-up Cognitive Function Tests (Compared With 0 Tests)a
1.
GBD 2015 Neurological Disorders Collaborator Group.  Global, regional, and national burden of neurological disorders during 1990-2015: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2015.  Lancet Neurol. 2017;16(11):877-897. doi:10.1016/S1474-4422(17)30299-5PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
Saint Martin  M, Sforza  E, Barthélémy  JC,  et al; PROOF group study.  Long-lasting active lifestyle and successful cognitive aging in a healthy elderly population: the PROOF cohort.  Rev Neurol (Paris). 2017;173(10):637-644. doi:10.1016/j.neurol.2017.05.009PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
Mormino  EC, Betensky  RA, Hedden  T,  et al.  Synergistic effect of β-amyloid and neurodegeneration on cognitive decline in clinically normal individuals.  JAMA Neurol. 2014;71(11):1379-1385. doi:10.1001/jamaneurol.2014.2031PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Wirth  M, Villeneuve  S, Haase  CM,  et al.  Associations between Alzheimer disease biomarkers, neurodegeneration, and cognition in cognitively normal older people.  JAMA Neurol. 2013;70(12):1512-1519.PubMedGoogle Scholar
5.
Bateman  RJ, Xiong  C, Benzinger  TL,  et al; Dominantly Inherited Alzheimer Network.  Clinical and biomarker changes in dominantly inherited Alzheimer’s disease.  N Engl J Med. 2012;367(9):795-804. doi:10.1056/NEJMoa1202753PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Hebert  LE, Weuve  J, Scherr  PA, Evans  DA.  Alzheimer disease in the United States (2010-2050) estimated using the 2010 census.  Neurology. 2013;80(19):1778-1783. doi:10.1212/WNL.0b013e31828726f5PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Hebert  LE, Scherr  PA, Bienias  JL, Bennett  DA, Evans  DA.  Alzheimer disease in the US population: prevalence estimates using the 2000 census.  Arch Neurol. 2003;60(8):1119-1122. doi:10.1001/archneur.60.8.1119PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Brookmeyer  R, Johnson  E, Ziegler-Graham  K, Arrighi  HM.  Forecasting the global burden of Alzheimer’s disease.  Alzheimers Dement. 2007;3(3):186-191. doi:10.1016/j.jalz.2007.04.381PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Huang  D, Swanson  EA, Lin  CP,  et al.  Optical coherence tomography.  Science. 1991;254(5035):1178-1181. doi:10.1126/science.1957169PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Yang  Q, Reisman  CA, Wang  Z,  et al.  Automated layer segmentation of macular OCT images using dual-scale gradient information.  Opt Express. 2010;18(20):21293-21307. doi:10.1364/OE.18.021293PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
Paquet  C, Boissonnot  M, Roger  F, Dighiero  P, Gil  R, Hugon  J.  Abnormal retinal thickness in patients with mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease.  Neurosci Lett. 2007;420(2):97-99. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2007.02.090PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Weil  RS, Schrag  AE, Warren  JD, Crutch  SJ, Lees  AJ, Morris  HR.  Visual dysfunction in Parkinson’s disease.  Brain. 2016;139(11):2827-2843. doi:10.1093/brain/aww175PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Moreno-Ramos  T, Benito-León  J, Villarejo  A, Bermejo-Pareja  F.  Retinal nerve fiber layer thinning in dementia associated with Parkinson’s disease, dementia with Lewy bodies, and Alzheimer’s disease.  J Alzheimers Dis. 2013;34(3):659-664.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
Cheung  CY, Ong  YT, Hilal  S,  et al.  Retinal ganglion cell analysis using high-definition optical coherence tomography in patients with mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease.  J Alzheimers Dis. 2015;45(1):45-56.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Garcia-Martin  ES, Rojas  B, Ramirez  AI,  et al.  Macular thickness as a potential biomarker of mild Alzheimer’s disease.  Ophthalmology. 2014;121(5):1149-1151.e3. doi:10.1016/j.ophtha.2013.12.023PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Coppola  G, Di Renzo  A, Ziccardi  L,  et al.  Optical coherence tomography in Alzheimer’s Disease: a meta-analysis.  PLoS One. 2015;10(8):e0134750. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0134750PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
van Koolwijk  LME, Despriet  DDG, Van Duijn  CM,  et al.  Association of cognitive functioning with retinal nerve fiber layer thickness.  Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2009;50(10):4576-4580. doi:10.1167/iovs.08-3181PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
Khawaja  AP, Chan  MPY, Yip  JLY,  et al.  Retinal nerve Fiber layer measures and cognitive function in the EPIC-Norfolk cohort study.  Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2016;57(4):1921-1926. doi:10.1167/iovs.16-19067PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
19.
Shi  Z, Wu  Y, Wang  M,  et al.  Greater attenuation of retinal nerve fiber layer thickness in Alzheimer’s disease patients.  J Alzheimers Dis. 2014;40(2):277-283.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Ko  F, Foster  PJ, Strouthidis  NG,  et al; UK Biobank Eye & Vision Consortium.  Associations with retinal pigment epithelium thickness measures in a large cohort: results from the UK Biobank.  Ophthalmology. 2017;124(1):105-117. doi:10.1016/j.ophtha.2016.07.033PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
Patel  PJ, Foster  PJ, Grossi  CM,  et al; UK Biobank Eyes and Vision Consortium.  Spectral-domain optical coherence tomography imaging in 67 321 adults: associations with macular thickness in the UK Biobank study.  Ophthalmology. 2016;123(4):829-840. doi:10.1016/j.ophtha.2015.11.009PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
Cruz-Herranz  A, Balk  LJ, Oberwahrenbrock  T,  et al; IMSVISUAL consortium.  The APOSTEL recommendations for reporting quantitative optical coherence tomography studies.  Neurology. 2016;86(24):2303-2309. doi:10.1212/WNL.0000000000002774PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
23.
Tewarie  P, Balk  L, Costello  F,  et al.  The OSCAR-IB consensus criteria for retinal OCT quality assessment.  PLoS One. 2012;7(4):e34823. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0034823PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Hinton  DR, Sadun  AA, Blanks  JC, Miller  CA.  Optic-nerve degeneration in Alzheimer’s disease.  N Engl J Med. 1986;315(8):485-487. doi:10.1056/NEJM198608213150804PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
Kesler  A, Vakhapova  V, Korczyn  AD, Naftaliev  E, Neudorfer  M.  Retinal thickness in patients with mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease.  Clin Neurol Neurosurg. 2011;113(7):523-526. doi:10.1016/j.clineuro.2011.02.014PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
Whitson  HE, Farsiu  S, Stinnett  S,  et al.  Retinal imaging biomarkers for early diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease.  Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2015;56(7):389. http://iovs.arvojournals.org/article.aspx?articleid=2333793&resultClick=1Google Scholar
27.
Gao  L, Liu  Y, Li  X, Bai  Q, Liu  P.  Abnormal retinal nerve fiber layer thickness and macula lutea in patients with mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease.  Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2015;60(1):162-167. doi:10.1016/j.archger.2014.10.011PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
28.
Inzelberg  R, Ramirez  JA, Nisipeanu  P, Ophir  A.  Retinal nerve fiber layer thinning in Parkinson disease.  Vision Res. 2004;44(24):2793-2797. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2004.06.009PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
29.
Jonas  JB, Wang  YX, Wei  WB, Zhu  LP, Shao  L, Xu  L.  Cognitive function and subfoveal choroidal thickness: the Beijing Eye Study.  Ophthalmology. 2016;123(1):220-222. doi:10.1016/j.ophtha.2015.06.020PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
30.
Tilvis  RS, Kähönen-Väre  MH, Jolkkonen  J, Valvanne  J, Pitkala  KH, Strandberg  TE.  Predictors of cognitive decline and mortality of aged people over a 10-year period.  J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2004;59(3):268-274. doi:10.1093/gerona/59.3.M268PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
31.
Marquis  S, Moore  MM, Howieson  DB,  et al.  Independent predictors of cognitive decline in healthy elderly persons.  Arch Neurol. 2002;59(4):601-606. doi:10.1001/archneur.59.4.601PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
32.
Liew  G, Wong  TY, Mitchell  P, Cheung  N, Wang  JJ.  Retinopathy predicts coronary heart disease mortality.  Heart. 2009;95(5):391-394. doi:10.1136/hrt.2008.146670PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
33.
Curcio  CA, Drucker  DN.  Retinal ganglion cells in Alzheimer’s disease and aging.  Ann Neurol. 1993;33(3):248-257. doi:10.1002/ana.410330305PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
34.
Davies  DC, McCoubrie  P, McDonald  B, Jobst  KA.  Myelinated axon number in the optic nerve is unaffected by Alzheimer’s disease.  Br J Ophthalmol. 1995;79(6):596-600. doi:10.1136/bjo.79.6.596PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
35.
Parisi  V, Restuccia  R, Fattapposta  F, Mina  C, Bucci  MG, Pierelli  F.  Morphological and functional retinal impairment in Alzheimer’s disease patients.  Clin Neurophysiol. 2001;112(10):1860-1867. doi:10.1016/S1388-2457(01)00620-4PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
36.
Kergoat  H, Kergoat  MJ, Justino  L, Chertkow  H, Robillard  A, Bergman  H.  An evaluation of the retinal nerve fiber layer thickness by scanning laser polarimetry in individuals with dementia of the Alzheimer type.  Acta Ophthalmol Scand. 2001;79(2):187-191. doi:10.1034/j.1600-0420.2001.079002187.xPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
37.
Kergoat  H, Kergoat  MJ, Justino  L, Robillard  A, Bergman  H, Chertkow  H.  Normal optic nerve head topography in the early stages of dementia of the Alzheimer type.  Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2001;12(6):359-363. doi:10.1159/000051281PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
38.
Ward  ME, Chen  R, Huang  HY,  et al.  Individuals with progranulin haploinsufficiency exhibit features of neuronal ceroid lipofuscinosis.  Sci Transl Med. 2017;9(385):eaah5642. doi:10.1126/scitranslmed.aah5642PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
39.
Hood  DC, Raza  AS.  On improving the use of OCT imaging for detecting glaucomatous damage.  Br J Ophthalmol. 2014;98(suppl 2):ii1-ii9. doi:10.1136/bjophthalmol-2014-305156PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
Original Investigation
October 2018

Association of Retinal Nerve Fiber Layer Thinning With Current and Future Cognitive Decline: A Study Using Optical Coherence Tomography

Author Affiliations
  • 1National Institute for Health Research Biomedical Research Centre, Moorfields Eye Hospital NHS Foundation Trust NHS Foundation Trust and UCL Institute of Ophthalmology, London, England
  • 2Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, Oxford, England
  • 3Centre for Medical Informatics, Usher Institute for Population Health Sciences and Informatics, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, Scotland
  • 4Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, Alexandra House, London, England
  • 5Topcon Healthcare Solutions Research and Development, Oakland, New Jersey
JAMA Neurol. 2018;75(10):1198-1205. doi:10.1001/jamaneurol.2018.1578
Key Points

Question  Are changes in the retinal nerve fiber layer (RNFL) associated with current or future cognitive function in a large community cohort of healthy participants?

Findings  In this community-based cohort study of more than 500 000 UK residents aged 40 to 69 years who received optical coherence tomography measurements of RNFL thickness and cognitive testing, there was a significant association between RNFL thickness and cognitive function at baseline. Furthermore, those with a thinner RNFL were twice as likely to experience cognitive decline over 3 years.

Meaning  A thinner RNFL is associated with worse current cognitive function and may have a role in screening those at risk of future cognitive decline.

Abstract

Importance  Identifing potential screening tests for future cognitive decline is a priority for developing treatments for and the prevention of dementia.

Objective  To examine the potential of retinal nerve fiber layer (RNFL) thickness measurement in identifying those at greater risk of cognitive decline in a large community cohort of healthy people.

Design, Setting, and Participants  UK Biobank is a prospective, multicenter, community-based study of UK residents aged 40 to 69 years at enrollment who underwent baseline retinal optical coherence tomography imaging, a physical examination, and a questionnaire. The pilot study phase was conducted from March 2006 to June 2006, and the main cohort underwent examination for baseline measures from April 2007 to October 2010. Four basic cognitive tests were performed at baseline, which were then repeated in a subset of participants approximately 3 years later. We analyzed eyes with high-quality optical coherence tomography images, excluding those with eye disease or vision loss, a history of ocular or neurological disease, or diabetes. We explored associations between RNFL thickness and cognitive function using multivariable logistic regression modeling to control for demographic as well as physiologic and ocular variation.

Main Outcomes and Measures  Odds ratios (ORs) for cognitive performance in the lowest fifth percentile in at least 2 of 4 cognitive tests at baseline, or worsening results on at least 1 cognitive test at follow-up. These analyses were adjusted for age, sex, race/ethnicity, height, refraction, intraocular pressure, education, and socioeconomic status.

Results  A total of 32 038 people were included at baseline testing, for whom the mean age was 56.0 years and of whom 17 172 (53.6%) were women. A thinner RNFL was associated with worse cognitive performance on baseline assessment. A multivariable regression controlling for potential confounders showed that those in the thinnest quintile of RNFL were 11% more likely to fail at least 1 cognitive test (95% CI, 2.0%-2.1%; P = .01). Follow-up cognitive tests were performed for 1251 participants (3.9%). Participants with an RNFL thickness in the 2 thinnest quintiles were almost twice as likely to have at least 1 test score be worse at follow-up cognitive testing (quintile 1: OR, 1.92; 95% CI, 1.29-2.85; P < .001; quintile 2: OR, 2.08; 95% CI, 1.40-3.08; P < .001).

Conclusions and Relevance  A thinner RNFL is associated with worse cognitive function in individuals without a neurodegenerative disease as well as greater likelihood of future cognitive decline. This preclinical observation has implications for future research, prevention, and treatment of dementia.

Introduction

Cognitive decline is part of the spectrum of normal aging and is related to lifestyle.1,2 Accelerated cognitive decline indicates neurodegenerative pathology, which can be captured preclinically with brain imaging techniques and protein biomarkers.3,4 Brain imaging evidence for onset of neurodegenerative dementia precedes symptomatic, progressive decline by about 15 years.5

Dementia is the neurodegenerative condition that is contributing most substantially to the global disease burden, with an estimated prevalence of 45 956 000 patients worldwide.1 In high-income North America, dementia is ranked top among other neurological diseases for disability-adjusted life-years.1,2,6 The prevalence of dementia increases with age, affecting 11%, 32%, and 82% of people older than 65, 75, and 85 years respectively.6 Some projections suggest that, because of population aging, the prevalence of Alzheimer disease (AD), the most common form of dementia, may triple by 2050.1,6,7 Globally, an estimated 46 million people are living with dementia, a number that is expected to rise to 131 million by 2050.1 However, if onset can be delayed by just 1 year, the projected global burden would decrease by 9 million.8 In summary, the preclinical detection of neurodegeneration will be crucial for secondary prevention trials.5

A hindrance to the development of new treatments to prevent dementia is the lack of markers that help predict who will be affected.3,4 One potential screening test is retinal mophometry. The retina is the only part of the central retinal nervous system that can be directly visualized. Optical coherence tomography (OCT)9 is a rapid, noninvasive imaging tool that can produce 3-dimensional cross-sectional images of the retina and permits precise and accurate measurement of the thickness of individual retinal components.10Quiz Ref ID The retinal nerve fiber layer (RNFL) is the inner most layer of the retina and is comprised of the retinal ganglion cell axons, which link the outer neuroretina to the dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus, where synaptic connections lead to the visual cortex.

Quiz Ref IDThe RNFL is thinner in people with early AD compared with healthy, age-matched controls.11 Similar findings have been reported in studies of other neurodegenerative conditions that are associated with cognitive decline, such as Parkinson disease12 and Lewy body dementia.13 More recently, studies using OCT imaging have shown that the RNFL is thinner in people with early cognitive impairment.14-16 With 2 exceptions, studies of retinal structure and cognitive function have been small cross-sectional case series and case-control studies. A cross-sectional association between retinal anatomy and cognitive function has been documented in 2 larger community-based studies.17,18 Only 1 small, prospective study has shown that a mixed cohort of 78 people with normal or mildly impaired cognition who developed future cognitive decline also showed a greater reduction of RNFL thickness as measured by OCT over 25 months.19 In this context, we examined the association between RNFL thickness and cognitive function (both concurrent and future) in a large community-based cohort of healthy UK Biobank participants to determine the potential role for RNFL measurements as a screening test for preclinical cognitive decline in people without a neurodegenerative disease at baseline.

Methods

UK Biobank is a community-based cohort of 502 656 UK residents aged 40 to 69 years and registered with the UKNHS. Examinations were conducted between April 2007 and October 2010 at 22 study assessment centers (eMethods in the Supplement). The North West Multicenter Research Ethics Committee approved the study in accordance with the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki (reference No. 06/MRE08/65). The overall study protocol (http://www.ukbiobank.ac.uk/resources/) and protocols for individual tests (http://biobank.ctsu.ox.ac.uk/crystal/docs.cgi) are available online. Written consent was obtained via electronic signature pad. Participants answered a wide-ranging touch screen questionnaire that covered demographic, socioeconomic, and lifestyle information; environmental exposures; and personal as well as family medical history. During 2009 to 2010, additional examination components were added, including eye examinations and cognitive function. Visual acuity, autorefraction/keratometry (Tomey RC5000; Erlangen-Tennenlohe), Goldmann-corrected intraocular pressure (IOP), and cornea-corrected IOP (Ocular Response Analyzer; Reichert) were collected from 110 573 consecutive participants during 2009 to 2010, and retinal OCT measurements were undertaken in 67 321 participants (60.9%). Ophthalmic tests were performed at 6 centers and were distributed across the United Kingdom, including Croydon and Hounslow in greater London, Liverpool and Sheffield in northern England, Birmingham in the Midlands, and Swansea in Wales. All baseline examinations for this study were performed during 2009 to 2010, including ophthalmic measurements and basic cognitive function testing. During 2012 to 2013, repeated cognitive testing was performed in a subset of participants.

The OCT protocol is described in greater detail by Ko et al20 and Patel et al21 and is compliant with the APOSTEL guidelines.22 In brief, high-resolution spectral-domain OCT imaging of undilated eyes was performed in a dark, enclosed room using the Topcon 3D OCT 1000 Mk2 (Topcon Inc), on the same day as other physical measurements. We excluded OCT scans of poor quality according to the OSCAR-IB criteria.23 In addition to the comorbidities listed as exclusion criteria by the OSCAR-IB criteria, we also excluded patients with a visual acuity that was less than 6 of 7.5, an IOP that was 22 mm Hg or higher or 5 mm Hg or lower, self-reported ocular disorders (eg, recent eye surgery, corneal graft, ocular injury, glaucoma, macular degeneration), self-reported diabetes, or self-reported neurodegenerative disease. Finally, if both eyes of 1 participant were eligible for inclusion in this analysis, 1 eye was chosen at random. We used Stata/SE, version 13.1 (StataCorp) for the analysis. The selection of participants is described in eFigure 1 in the Supplement.

Quiz Ref IDBasic cognitive function was tested using touch screens at UK Biobank Assessment Centre, with baseline assessment conducted during 2009 to 2010 and a repeated assessment (including cognitive function) during 2012 to 2013. These tests included prospective memory, pairs matching, numeric and verbal reasoning, and reaction time. Numeric and verbal reasoning tested the capacity to solve logic problems and reasoning capacity independent of acquired knowledge. Test failure at baseline was defined as an incorrect answer on the first attempt of prospective memory, or doing worse than 95% of participants in pairs matching (>2 incorrect matches), numeric and verbal reasoning tests (score, <3), or reaction time (>770 milliseconds). The repeated assessment of cognitive function was performed during 2012 to 2013. A participant’s performance was considered worse on follow-up testing if the number of attempts increased on the prospective memory test, the number of incorrect matches increased on pairs matching, there was a decrease in the numeric and verbal reasoning test scores, or a reaction time slowed by at least 100 milliseconds.

Statistical Analysis

Stata/SE, version 13.1 (StataCorp) was used for the analysis, including the svy suite of commands extension package. Linear regression analyses were first used to test associations between the RNFL and cognitive function, both at baseline (number of tests failed) and on follow-up testing (number of tests with a worse result at follow-up). A logistic regression was then used to determine odds ratios for cognitive deficits and the decline for each quintile of RNFL thickness. We further tested to determine whether effects were additive (ie, doing poorly on 0/1/2/3/4 tests at baseline). Multivariable regression modeling was performed to adjust for potential confounders. When appropriate, 2-sided hypothesis testing was performed. The null hypothesis was rejected if P < .05 (also an indicator of statistical significance).

Results

Between September 2009 and June 2010, 67 321 people underwent OCT imaging. Of these, 32 038 people (47.6%) had high-quality OCT imaging results, scores for all cognitive tests, reported no neurological or ocular disease, and did not have diabetes (eFigure 1 in the Supplement). Of these, 1251 people (3.9%) with high-quality OCT scans and full additional data at baseline completed follow-up cognitive testing during 2012 to 13. Table 1 summarizes demographic, morphometric, and ophthalmic variables at baseline (2009-2010) for all participants with an OCT measure, the 32 038 included in this study, and the subset of those who also underwent follow-up assessment during 2012 to 2013. Compared with all participants who were recruited with an OCT measure available, participants in this study were less economically deprived, more highly educated, had a lower refractive error, and were less racially/ethnically diverse. The subset of participants with follow-up data were slightly older, more often white, had higher educational attainment, and included more nonsmokers when compared with the 32 038 who were included at baseline.

The mean (SD) age of the participants included in this study was 56.0 (8.21) years (95% CI 55.9-56.1), with a higher percentage of women (17 172 [53.6%]; 95% CI, 52.0-54.1) than men. The mean (SD) age at the second visit was 58.1 (7.1) years (95% CI, 57.7-58.5), with approximately equal numbers of women (637 [51.1%]) and men (609 [48.9%]). There was a predominance of white participants at both baseline and follow-up (29 576 [92.7%]; 95% CI, 92.4-92.9; and 1232 [98.6%]; 95% CI, 97.8-99.2, respectively). The mean (SD) Townsend deprivation index was −1.18 (2.91) at baseline (95% CI, −1.21 to −1.14; interquartile range, 4.23; more positive scores indicate greater deprivation; UK average, 0). Those included at follow-up were less disadvantaged than the UK average and less so than those at baseline (mean Townsend deprivation index, −2.49; 95% CI, −2.63 to −2.36; interquartile range, 2.62). More than one-third of participants at baseline had a degree and another quarter had a professional qualification or A-levels. Of participants who were undergoing follow-up cognitive testing, almost half (596 [47.7%]; 95% CI, 45.0-50.5) reported having a degree, less than one-quarter had a professional qualification or A-levels (290 [23.2%]; 95% CI, 21.0-25.6) and the remainder had a General Certificate of Secondary Education or lower.

A thinner baseline RNFL measurement was associated with worse performance on baseline cognitive tests (eFigures 2-5 in the Supplement; Figure 1). For each cognitive test (prospective memory, pairs matching, numeric and verbal reasoning, and reaction time) there was worse performance for each quintile of people with a thinner RNFL (eFigures 2-5 in the Supplement). Of those in the thinnest RNFL quantile, 475 people (7.4%) (95% CI, 6.8-8.1%) failed at least 2 of 4 cognitive tests (Figure 1) as compared with 267 (4.2%) (95% CI, 3.7%-4.7%) of those in the thickest RNFL quintile (P < .001). To quantify the association and account for other potential confounding, a multivariable logistic regression was used to adjust for the associations of age, sex, race/ethnicity, Townsend deprivation index, educational attainment, refractive error, and IOP, and to calculate the odds ratio of a cognitive deficit (Table 2). Those in the thinnest RNFL quintile were 11% (95% CI, 2%-21%; P = .01) more likely to fail 1 or more cognitive tests (as defined in the Methods previously), compared with those in the thickest quintile (Table 2).

Multivariable regression modeling of association between RNFL thickness and future worsening on 1 or more follow-up cognitive tests was performed, controlling for age, sex, height, race/ethnicity, refraction, IOP, Townsend deprivation index, and education (Table 3). Compared with those in the thickest RNFL quintile, those in the 2 thinnest quintiles were almost twice as likely (odds ratio, 1.92; 95% CI, 1.29-2.85; P < .001) to score worse on at least 1 cognitive test at follow-up (Table 3). Per quintile of RNFL thinning, there was an 18% increased risk of cognitive decline at 3-year follow-up (95% CI, 8%-29%; P < .001; Table 3). Baseline RNFL thickness was compared with the total number of cognitive tests with worse scores on follow-up testing (ie, whether a participant did worse on 0, 1, 2, 3, or 4 tests) (Figure 2). A thinner baseline RNFL was significantly associated with a future decline in a greater number of cognitive tests (linear regression, P < .001), even after controlling for potential confounders (Figure 2).

Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the largest study of its kind and the first to identify that future decline in cognitive function is associated with a thinner RNFL in a large, healthy community-based cohort. Quiz Ref IDThose in the lowest 2 quintiles of baseline RNFL distribution had twice the likelihood of a developing a decline in cognitive function over a 3-year follow-up interval compared with those in the top RNFL quintile (Table 3). As we expected, we observed a strong, consistent association between a thinner RNFL and poorer cognition in cross-sectional data. Furthermore, there was an incremental relationship between a thinner RNFL and poorer cognition in the longitudinal data (Figure 2). Our findings show that a thinner RNFL is a potential indicator for current impaired cognition and may have a potential role in screening for those at an increased risk of a future decline in cognitive function. These cognitive deficits and declines spanned a range of functional domains.

Strengths and Limitations

An important limitation of this study is that although UK Biobank participants were enrolled from a sampling frame that represented a cross-section of the UK population, the response rate was low. Quiz Ref IDConsequently, the representativeness of the study is limited, as participants were more often white, middle class, and educated. This means that the rates of cognitive impairment identified here will not necessarily be the same as those in the UK population, or of another Western European or North American population. However, we believe that the associations that we have identified are unlikely to be the result of an intrinsic bias in the data, and therefore we feel the overall conclusions are valid for populations of Western European descent.

The number of participants enrolled in UK Biobank required that a balance be struck between detailed, in-depth full clinical testing and the need to complete a cognitive assessment efficiently on hundreds of thousands of participants. Whether the resultant large cognitive data set is strengthened or weakened by this approach is unclear. By using tests that were sensitive to the population range of performance, declines across the population can be detected. This increases the sensitivity of the study to detect changes and its relevance to population-based early disease stage screening. From an etiologic perspective, this study does not attempt to identify specific cognitive domains linked with RNFL thickness. The range of tests available to test the hypothesis include basic mechanisms, such as processing speed (reaction time), and high level functions, such as intelligence (reasoning). As such, they are suitable for investigating an overall association of cognition with the eye. Further work would be required to identify the underlying mechanisms linking RNFL thickness with specific cognitive domains.

Our findings are consistent with those from several previous studies of people with an established disease. Hinton et al24 described an association between dementia and thinner RNFL. Others have made similar observations in mild, moderate, and severe cognitive impairment in cases series.13,15,19,25-27 A thinner RNFL has been recorded in Parkinson disease28 and Lewy body dementia.13

Although most of the previous data suggesting an association between RNFL thickness and cognition come from case series, 2 studies have identified a cross-sectional association between thinner RNFL and poorer cognitive function in community-based cohorts—one in a geographically and genetically isolated population in the Netherlands,17 and the other in the European Prospective Investigation of Cancer (EPIC) Norfolk cohort in the United Kingdom. In the EPIC cohort of 8623 people, a thinner RNFL was associated with poorer scores from cognitive tests that assessed global function, recognition, learning, episodic memory, and premorbid intelligence. While EPIC Norfolk described a similar association as this study, the cross-sectional associations were of small effect size, with RNFL thickness appearing to be ineffective as a potential screening test for cognitive function.18 In contrast, the association between baseline RNFL and future cognitive decline in this study is stronger. A possible explanation for this is that the RNFL measurements in EPIC were generated using scanning laser ophthalmoscopy, which is a less precise measure than OCT. Another recent community-based study that assessed a cohort of Chinese people linked poorer cognitive function to thinner subfoveal choroidal thickness.29 We were not able to assess this parameter in our study because of differences in scanning technology, but it adds weight to the concept that ophthalmic imaging can detect features that are associated with poorer cognitive function.

Of particular interest and relevance are results from a small, prospective study that examined the longitudinal trends in RNFL thickness in a mixed group of 78 people with normal or mildly impaired cognition over a 2-year period in Shanghai, Peoples’ Republic of China.19 Sixty (77%) retained stable cognitive function, while 18 (23%) developed a cognitive decline and then received a diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment (8 [10%]) or AD (10 [13%]). Using retinal OCT to measure RNFL (as we did), they observed a greater reduction of RNFL thickness among those who showed a cognitive decline than the stable participants (mean [SD] reduction, −11.0 [12.8] μm vs −0.4 [15.7] μm; P = .01).

In our study, we specifically excluded participants who reported neurological, diabetic, and ocular diseases and included only people with good visual acuity because of the well-recognized association these conditions have with RNFL measurements. Consequently, our results are more representative of a premorbid population, further strengthening the principle of an association between a thin RNFL and cognitive decline. Others have reported that markers of ill health, particularly cardiovascular, are risk factors for future cognitive decline; such risk factors include atrial fibrillation, diabetes, heart failure, intermittent claudication, previous stroke, and frailty markers, such as poor exercise tolerance.30-32 We chose not to exclude people with these risk factors.

We identified an incremental association between a progressively thinner baseline RNFL and a future decline that spanned different cognitive domains. Gao et al27 sought to but did not find such a correlation between retinal features and severity of cognitive impairment. One possible explanation is that they used the Mini-Mental State Examination as the index of cognitive impairment; the Mini-Mental State Examination has a strong ceiling effect and is likely to be insensitive to early changes at the upper end of the distribution.24 The association between baseline RNFL and baseline cognitive scores appears to be curvilinear, showing a threshold effect with a greater deficit shown in RNFL quintiles 1 and 2 (Figure 1). The evidence for a curvilinear association between baseline RNFL and future cognitive decline was less convincing, although the number of observations was smaller by a factor of 30.

Some have argued against a retinal involvement in generalized neurodegenerative disease.33-37 Van Koolwijk et al17 proposed that while there may be an association between RNFL thickness and cognitive function, it is not sufficient to explain the variance in cognitive test scores and is not a useful predictor of cognitive ability. The UK Biobank cohort benefits from having numerous participants and consequently has greater statistical power. We recognize that statistical significance is not equivalent to clinical relevance; however, while most previous research has focused on later-stage cognitive impairment and on older participants, our findings suggest the potential of RNFL thickness measurement as a screening test for relatively younger and healthier people. Furthermore, the preponderance of white people of relative socioeconomic prosperity (as demonstrated by the favorable mean Townsend deprivation index [Tables 1 and 3]) suggests that our results are even more applicable to a low-risk group and provide a conservative estimate of the association. More recently, preclinical and translational data revealed that in at least one of the neurodegenerative dementias—frontotemporal dementia caused by progranulin haploinsufficiency—retinal layer changes are associated with a demonstrable pathological substrate.38 Nevertheless, in response to Van Koolwijk et al,17 it would be unlikely for any screening test to be used in isolation. Our study strengthens the argument of an association between neurodegenerative processes that affect the brain and the eye and indicates that OCT measurement of the RNFL is a potential noninvasive, relatively low-cost and time-efficient screening test for early cognitive changes.

There is strong evidence that a thinner RNFL is associated with adverse cognitive function. Our data also suggest that RNFL thinning precedes cognitive decline in many people and predicts cognitive deterioration. The wide availability of OCT technology in ophthalmic and optometric practices may accelerate the general uptake of this potential screening test. However, one must be careful in its interpretation so as to avoid an unnecessary psychological burden for people who may not ultimately experience cognitive decline. Further, attempting to risk-stratify people would be most appropriate if there is a viable treatment or preventative measure available. Additional research is required to define a possible role for these observations in health policies and to determine the relevance at an individual level. It is unclear whether RNFL thinning continues as cognitive decline occurs or whether it is a precursor to cognitive deterioration. While UK Biobank did perform follow-up OCT testing, later retinal measures were not available for analysis. Future research may focus on the association between longitudinal RNFL changes and cognitive function. It may be that RNFL imaging is more useful for certain demographic, racial/ethnic, or medical subgroups. We believe that it is plausible that a thinner RNFL is a marker of a currently ill-defined clinical syndrome, which includes cognitive decline.

Conclusions

The finding that a thinner RNFL is associated with significant future cognitive decline in a large cohort of people aged 40 to 69 years, drawn from communities around the United Kingdom, consolidates the case for regarding retinal anatomical measures as a useful potential screening test for identifying those at risk of future cognitive loss. However, macular retinal measures are now being promoted as a tool for diagnosis and monitoring glaucoma, with measurements focused on the ganglion cell complex (ganglion cell complex = RNFL + the ganglion cell layer and inner plexiform layer ).39 The parallels between glaucoma and cognitive decline therefore suggest that the ganglion cell layer and the inner plexiform layer would be useful targets for a similar analysis. The potential for OCT measurement of retinal layers as a predictor of cognitive decline is particularly attractive because it is rapid, noninvasive, and widely available, with high potential for uptake.

Back to top
Article Information

Accepted for Publication: April 27, 2018.

Corresponding Author: Paul J. Foster, PhD, UCL Institute of Ophthalmology, 11-43 Bath St, London EC1V 9EL, England (p.foster@ucl.ac.uk).

Published Online: June 25, 2018. doi:10.1001/jamaneurol.2018.1578

Author Contributions: Dr Ko had full access to all the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis. Drs Patel and Foster contributed equally to the authorship of this article.

Concept and design: Ko, Gallacher, Khaw, Reisman, Foster, Patel.

Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: Ko, Muthy, Gallacher, Sudlow, Rees, Yang, Keane, Petzold, Reisman, Strouthidis, Foster, Patel.

Drafting of the manuscript: Ko, Muthy, Gallacher, Rees, Yang, Foster, Patel.

Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Ko, Muthy, Gallacher, Sudlow, Rees, Keane, Petzold, Khaw, Reisman, Strouthidis, Foster, Patel.

Statistical analysis: Ko, Gallacher, Rees, Reisman, Foster.

Obtained funding: Ko, Sudlow, Khaw, Foster, Patel.

Administrative, technical, or material support: Ko, Muthy, Sudlow, Rees, Keane, Khaw, Reisman, Foster.

Supervision: Petzold, Reisman, Strouthidis, Foster, Patel.

Conflict of Interest Disclosures: Dr Ko receives grant support from University College of London (UCL). Ms Muthy receives personal fees from UCL. Prof Sudlow is chief scientist at UK Biobank. Prof Rees receives grant support from Wellcome Trust and personal fees from Google DeepMind. Dr Yang and Mr Reisman are employed by Topcon Medical Systems Inc. Dr Keane receives personal fees from Allergan, Topcon, Heidelberg Engineering, Haag-Streit, Novartis, Bayer, Optos, and DeepMind as well as grant support from a Clinician Scientist award (CS-2014-14-023) from the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR). Dr Petzold receives personal fees and grant support from Novartis and is a member of the steering committee of the Optical Coherence Tomography Trial in Multiple Sclerosis (OCTiMS), which is sponsored by Novartis and for which he has not received honoraria Prof Foster receives personal fees from Allergan, Carl Zeiss, Google/DeepMind, and Santen; grant support from Alcon, and support from the Richard Desmond Charitable Trust, via Fight for Sight, London. Dr Patel receives grant support from Topcon Medical Systems Inc. Prof Khaw is supported in part by the Helen Hamlyn Trust. No other disclosures are reported.

Funding/Support: This analysis was supported by the Eranda Foundation via the International Glaucoma Association in the design and conduct of the study. The UCL Overseas Research Scholarship and Graduate Research Scholarship programs provided scholarship support for Dr Ko. Ms Muthy, Drs Strouthidis and Patel and Profs Khaw and Foster received salary support from the NIHR Biomedical Research Centres at Moorfields Eye Hospital NHS Foundation Trust. Dr Foster received support from the Richard Desmond Charitable Trust via Fight for Sight, London. UK Biobank Eye and Vision Consortium is supported by grants from Moorfields Eye Charity, the NIHR Biomedical Research Centre at Moorfields Eye Hospital NHS Foundation Trust, and the UCL Institute of Ophthalmology and the Alcon Research Institute.

Role of the Funder/Sponsor: No funders had a direct role in the collection, management, analysis, or interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; nor in the decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

UK Biobank Eye & Vision Consortium: The UK Biobank Eye & Vision Consortium members are Tariq Aslam, PhD, Manchester University, Sarah A. Barman, PhD, Kingston University, Jenny H. Barrett, PhD, University of Leeds, Paul Bishop, PhD, Manchester University, Peter Blows, BSc, NIHR Biomedical Research Centre, Catey Bunce, DSc, King’s College London, Roxana O. Carare, PhD, University of Southampton, Usha Chakravarthy, FRCOphth, Queens University Belfast, Michelle Chan, FRCOphth, NIHR Biomedical Research Centre, Sharon Y.L. Chua, PhD, NIHR Biomedical Research Centre, David P. Crabb, PhD, UCL, Philippa M. Cumberland, MSc, UCL Great Ormond Street Institute of Child Health, Alexander Day, PhD, NIHR Biomedical Research Centre, Parul Desai, PhD, NIHR Biomedical Research Centre, Bal Dhillon, FRCOphth, University of Edinburgh, Andrew D. Dick, FRCOphth, University of Bristol, Cathy Egan, FRCOphth, NIHR Biomedical Research Centre, Sarah Ennis, PhD, University of Southampton, Paul Foster, PhD, NIHR Biomedical Research Centre, Marcus Fruttiger, PhD, NIHR Biomedical Research Centre, John E.J. Gallacher, PhD, University of Oxford, David F. GARWAY-HEATH FRCOphth- NIHR Biomedical Research Centre, Jane Gibson, PhD, University of Southampton, Dan Gore, FRCOphth, NIHR Biomedical Research Centre, Jeremy A. Guggenheim, PhD, Cardiff University, Chris J. Hammond, FRCOphth, King's College London, Alison Hardcastle, PhD, NIHR Biomedical Research Centre, Simon P. Harding, MD, University of Liverpool, Ruth E. Hogg, PhD, Queens University Belfast, Pirro Hysi, PhD, King's College London, Pearse A. Keane, MD, NIHR Biomedical Research Centre, Sir Peng T. Khaw, PhD, NIHR Biomedical Research Centre, Anthony P. Khawaja, DPhil, NIHR Biomedical Research Centre, Gerassimos Lascaratos, PhD, NIHR Biomedical Research Centre, Andrew J. Lotery, MD, University of Southampton, Tom Macgillivray, PhD, University of Edinburgh, Sarah Mackie, PhD, University of Leeds, Keith Martin, FRCOphth, University of Cambridge, Michelle McGaughey, Queen’s University Belfast, Bernadette McGuinness, PhD, Queen’s University Belfast, Gareth J. McKay, PhD, Queen's University Belfast, Martin McKibbin, FRCOphth, Leeds Teaching HospitalsNHS Trust, Danny Mitry, PhD, NIHR Biomedical Research Centre, Tony Moore, FRCOphth, NIHR Biomedical Research Centre, James E. Morgan, DPhil, Cardiff University, Zaynah A. Muthy, BSc, NIHR Biomedical Research Centre, Eoin O’Sullivan, MD, King's College Hospital NHS Foundation Trust, Chris G. Owen, PhD, University of London, Praveen Patel, FRCOphth, NIHR Biomedical Research Centre, Euan Paterson, BSc, Queens University Belfast, Tunde Peto, PhD, Queen's University Belfast, Axel Petzold, PhD, UCL, Jugnoo S. Rahi, PhD, UCL Great Ormond Street Institute of Child Health, Alicja R. Rudnikca, PhD, University of London, Jay Self, PhD, University of Southampton, Sobha Sivaprasad, FRCOphth, NIHR Biomedical Research Centre, David Steel, FRCOphth, Newcastle University, Irene Stratton, MSc, Gloucestershire HospitalsNHS Foundation Trust, Nicholas Strouthidis, PhD, NIHR Biomedical Research Centre, Cathie Sudlow, DPhil, University of Edinburgh, Dhanes Thomas, FRCOphth, NIHR Biomedical Research Centre, Emanuele Trucco, PhD, University of Dundee, Adnan Tufail, FRCOphth, NIHR Biomedical Research Centre, Veronique Vitart, PhD, University of Edinburgh, Stephen A. Vernon, DM, Nottingham University HospitalsNHS Trust, Ananth C. Viswanathan, FRCOphth, NIHR Biomedical Research Centre, Cathy Williams, PhD, University of Bristol, Katie Williams, PhD, King's College London, Jayne V. Woodside, MRCOphth, PhD, Queen's University Belfast, Max M. Yates, PhD, University of East Anglia, Jennifer Yip, PhD, University of Cambridge, and Yalin Zheng, PhD, University of Liverpool.

Disclaimer: The views expressed in this publication are those of the author(s) and not necessarily those of the National Health Service, the NIHR, or the Department of Health.

Additional Contributions: We thank the UK Department of Health for providing financial support through an award from the NIHR to Moorfields Eye Hospital NHS Foundation Trust and the UCL Institute of Ophthalmology for a Biomedical Research Centre for Ophthalmology. We also thank Kay-Tee Khaw, PhD, University of Cambridge, for providing analytic advice. No individuals were compensated for their contributions.

References
1.
GBD 2015 Neurological Disorders Collaborator Group.  Global, regional, and national burden of neurological disorders during 1990-2015: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2015.  Lancet Neurol. 2017;16(11):877-897. doi:10.1016/S1474-4422(17)30299-5PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
Saint Martin  M, Sforza  E, Barthélémy  JC,  et al; PROOF group study.  Long-lasting active lifestyle and successful cognitive aging in a healthy elderly population: the PROOF cohort.  Rev Neurol (Paris). 2017;173(10):637-644. doi:10.1016/j.neurol.2017.05.009PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
Mormino  EC, Betensky  RA, Hedden  T,  et al.  Synergistic effect of β-amyloid and neurodegeneration on cognitive decline in clinically normal individuals.  JAMA Neurol. 2014;71(11):1379-1385. doi:10.1001/jamaneurol.2014.2031PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Wirth  M, Villeneuve  S, Haase  CM,  et al.  Associations between Alzheimer disease biomarkers, neurodegeneration, and cognition in cognitively normal older people.  JAMA Neurol. 2013;70(12):1512-1519.PubMedGoogle Scholar
5.
Bateman  RJ, Xiong  C, Benzinger  TL,  et al; Dominantly Inherited Alzheimer Network.  Clinical and biomarker changes in dominantly inherited Alzheimer’s disease.  N Engl J Med. 2012;367(9):795-804. doi:10.1056/NEJMoa1202753PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Hebert  LE, Weuve  J, Scherr  PA, Evans  DA.  Alzheimer disease in the United States (2010-2050) estimated using the 2010 census.  Neurology. 2013;80(19):1778-1783. doi:10.1212/WNL.0b013e31828726f5PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Hebert  LE, Scherr  PA, Bienias  JL, Bennett  DA, Evans  DA.  Alzheimer disease in the US population: prevalence estimates using the 2000 census.  Arch Neurol. 2003;60(8):1119-1122. doi:10.1001/archneur.60.8.1119PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Brookmeyer  R, Johnson  E, Ziegler-Graham  K, Arrighi  HM.  Forecasting the global burden of Alzheimer’s disease.  Alzheimers Dement. 2007;3(3):186-191. doi:10.1016/j.jalz.2007.04.381PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Huang  D, Swanson  EA, Lin  CP,  et al.  Optical coherence tomography.  Science. 1991;254(5035):1178-1181. doi:10.1126/science.1957169PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Yang  Q, Reisman  CA, Wang  Z,  et al.  Automated layer segmentation of macular OCT images using dual-scale gradient information.  Opt Express. 2010;18(20):21293-21307. doi:10.1364/OE.18.021293PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
Paquet  C, Boissonnot  M, Roger  F, Dighiero  P, Gil  R, Hugon  J.  Abnormal retinal thickness in patients with mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease.  Neurosci Lett. 2007;420(2):97-99. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2007.02.090PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Weil  RS, Schrag  AE, Warren  JD, Crutch  SJ, Lees  AJ, Morris  HR.  Visual dysfunction in Parkinson’s disease.  Brain. 2016;139(11):2827-2843. doi:10.1093/brain/aww175PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Moreno-Ramos  T, Benito-León  J, Villarejo  A, Bermejo-Pareja  F.  Retinal nerve fiber layer thinning in dementia associated with Parkinson’s disease, dementia with Lewy bodies, and Alzheimer’s disease.  J Alzheimers Dis. 2013;34(3):659-664.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
Cheung  CY, Ong  YT, Hilal  S,  et al.  Retinal ganglion cell analysis using high-definition optical coherence tomography in patients with mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease.  J Alzheimers Dis. 2015;45(1):45-56.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Garcia-Martin  ES, Rojas  B, Ramirez  AI,  et al.  Macular thickness as a potential biomarker of mild Alzheimer’s disease.  Ophthalmology. 2014;121(5):1149-1151.e3. doi:10.1016/j.ophtha.2013.12.023PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Coppola  G, Di Renzo  A, Ziccardi  L,  et al.  Optical coherence tomography in Alzheimer’s Disease: a meta-analysis.  PLoS One. 2015;10(8):e0134750. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0134750PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
van Koolwijk  LME, Despriet  DDG, Van Duijn  CM,  et al.  Association of cognitive functioning with retinal nerve fiber layer thickness.  Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2009;50(10):4576-4580. doi:10.1167/iovs.08-3181PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
Khawaja  AP, Chan  MPY, Yip  JLY,  et al.  Retinal nerve Fiber layer measures and cognitive function in the EPIC-Norfolk cohort study.  Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2016;57(4):1921-1926. doi:10.1167/iovs.16-19067PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
19.
Shi  Z, Wu  Y, Wang  M,  et al.  Greater attenuation of retinal nerve fiber layer thickness in Alzheimer’s disease patients.  J Alzheimers Dis. 2014;40(2):277-283.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Ko  F, Foster  PJ, Strouthidis  NG,  et al; UK Biobank Eye & Vision Consortium.  Associations with retinal pigment epithelium thickness measures in a large cohort: results from the UK Biobank.  Ophthalmology. 2017;124(1):105-117. doi:10.1016/j.ophtha.2016.07.033PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
Patel  PJ, Foster  PJ, Grossi  CM,  et al; UK Biobank Eyes and Vision Consortium.  Spectral-domain optical coherence tomography imaging in 67 321 adults: associations with macular thickness in the UK Biobank study.  Ophthalmology. 2016;123(4):829-840. doi:10.1016/j.ophtha.2015.11.009PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
Cruz-Herranz  A, Balk  LJ, Oberwahrenbrock  T,  et al; IMSVISUAL consortium.  The APOSTEL recommendations for reporting quantitative optical coherence tomography studies.  Neurology. 2016;86(24):2303-2309. doi:10.1212/WNL.0000000000002774PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
23.
Tewarie  P, Balk  L, Costello  F,  et al.  The OSCAR-IB consensus criteria for retinal OCT quality assessment.  PLoS One. 2012;7(4):e34823. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0034823PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Hinton  DR, Sadun  AA, Blanks  JC, Miller  CA.  Optic-nerve degeneration in Alzheimer’s disease.  N Engl J Med. 1986;315(8):485-487. doi:10.1056/NEJM198608213150804PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
Kesler  A, Vakhapova  V, Korczyn  AD, Naftaliev  E, Neudorfer  M.  Retinal thickness in patients with mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease.  Clin Neurol Neurosurg. 2011;113(7):523-526. doi:10.1016/j.clineuro.2011.02.014PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
Whitson  HE, Farsiu  S, Stinnett  S,  et al.  Retinal imaging biomarkers for early diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease.  Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2015;56(7):389. http://iovs.arvojournals.org/article.aspx?articleid=2333793&resultClick=1Google Scholar
27.
Gao  L, Liu  Y, Li  X, Bai  Q, Liu  P.  Abnormal retinal nerve fiber layer thickness and macula lutea in patients with mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease.  Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2015;60(1):162-167. doi:10.1016/j.archger.2014.10.011PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
28.
Inzelberg  R, Ramirez  JA, Nisipeanu  P, Ophir  A.  Retinal nerve fiber layer thinning in Parkinson disease.  Vision Res. 2004;44(24):2793-2797. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2004.06.009PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
29.
Jonas  JB, Wang  YX, Wei  WB, Zhu  LP, Shao  L, Xu  L.  Cognitive function and subfoveal choroidal thickness: the Beijing Eye Study.  Ophthalmology. 2016;123(1):220-222. doi:10.1016/j.ophtha.2015.06.020PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
30.
Tilvis  RS, Kähönen-Väre  MH, Jolkkonen  J, Valvanne  J, Pitkala  KH, Strandberg  TE.  Predictors of cognitive decline and mortality of aged people over a 10-year period.  J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2004;59(3):268-274. doi:10.1093/gerona/59.3.M268PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
31.
Marquis  S, Moore  MM, Howieson  DB,  et al.  Independent predictors of cognitive decline in healthy elderly persons.  Arch Neurol. 2002;59(4):601-606. doi:10.1001/archneur.59.4.601PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
32.
Liew  G, Wong  TY, Mitchell  P, Cheung  N, Wang  JJ.  Retinopathy predicts coronary heart disease mortality.  Heart. 2009;95(5):391-394. doi:10.1136/hrt.2008.146670PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
33.
Curcio  CA, Drucker  DN.  Retinal ganglion cells in Alzheimer’s disease and aging.  Ann Neurol. 1993;33(3):248-257. doi:10.1002/ana.410330305PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
34.
Davies  DC, McCoubrie  P, McDonald  B, Jobst  KA.  Myelinated axon number in the optic nerve is unaffected by Alzheimer’s disease.  Br J Ophthalmol. 1995;79(6):596-600. doi:10.1136/bjo.79.6.596PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
35.
Parisi  V, Restuccia  R, Fattapposta  F, Mina  C, Bucci  MG, Pierelli  F.  Morphological and functional retinal impairment in Alzheimer’s disease patients.  Clin Neurophysiol. 2001;112(10):1860-1867. doi:10.1016/S1388-2457(01)00620-4PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
36.
Kergoat  H, Kergoat  MJ, Justino  L, Chertkow  H, Robillard  A, Bergman  H.  An evaluation of the retinal nerve fiber layer thickness by scanning laser polarimetry in individuals with dementia of the Alzheimer type.  Acta Ophthalmol Scand. 2001;79(2):187-191. doi:10.1034/j.1600-0420.2001.079002187.xPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
37.
Kergoat  H, Kergoat  MJ, Justino  L, Robillard  A, Bergman  H, Chertkow  H.  Normal optic nerve head topography in the early stages of dementia of the Alzheimer type.  Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2001;12(6):359-363. doi:10.1159/000051281PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
38.
Ward  ME, Chen  R, Huang  HY,  et al.  Individuals with progranulin haploinsufficiency exhibit features of neuronal ceroid lipofuscinosis.  Sci Transl Med. 2017;9(385):eaah5642. doi:10.1126/scitranslmed.aah5642PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
39.
Hood  DC, Raza  AS.  On improving the use of OCT imaging for detecting glaucomatous damage.  Br J Ophthalmol. 2014;98(suppl 2):ii1-ii9. doi:10.1136/bjophthalmol-2014-305156PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
×