Key PointsQuestion
Which demographic, genetic, and neuroimaging factors are associated with cognitive and brain resilience to pathological tau in patients with Alzheimer disease?
Findings
In this multicenter, cross-sectional, longitudinal study of 260 cognitively impaired amyloid-β–positive participants, young age and female sex were associated with greater brain resilience, whereas higher educational level and cortical thickness were associated with greater cognitive resilience.
Meaning
Cognitive and brain resilience may be associated with differential mechanisms, which may help explain interindividual differences in how well patients tolerate pathological tau.
Importance
Better understanding is needed of the degree to which individuals tolerate Alzheimer disease (AD)–like pathological tau with respect to brain structure (brain resilience) and cognition (cognitive resilience).
Objective
To examine the demographic (age, sex, and educational level), genetic (APOE-ε4 status), and neuroimaging (white matter hyperintensities and cortical thickness) factors associated with interindividual differences in brain and cognitive resilience to tau positron emission tomography (PET) load and to changes in global cognition over time.
Design, Setting, an Participants
In this cross-sectional, longitudinal study, tau PET was performed from June 1, 2014, to November 30, 2017, and global cognition monitored for a mean [SD] interval of 2.0 [1.8] years at 3 dementia centers in South Korea, Sweden, and the United States. The study included amyloid-β–positive participants with mild cognitive impairment or AD dementia. Data analysis was performed from October 26, 2018, to December 11, 2019.
Exposures
Standard dementia screening, cognitive testing, brain magnetic resonance imaging, amyloid-β PET and cerebrospinal fluid analysis, and flortaucipir (tau) labeled with fluor-18 (18F) PET.
Main Outcomes and Measures
Separate linear regression models were performed between whole cortex [18F]flortaucipir uptake and cortical thickness, and standardized residuals were used to obtain a measure of brain resilience. The same procedure was performed for whole cortex [18F]flortaucipir uptake vs Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) as a measure of cognitive resilience. Bivariate and multivariable linear regression models were conducted with age, sex, educational level, APOE-ε4 status, white matter hyperintensity volumes, and cortical thickness as independent variables and brain and cognitive resilience measures as dependent variables. Linear mixed models were performed to examine whether changes in MMSE scores over time differed as a function of a combined brain and cognitive resilience variable.
Results
A total of 260 participants (145 [55.8%] female; mean [SD] age, 69.2 [9.5] years; mean [SD] MMSE score, 21.9 [5.5]) were included in the study. In multivariable models, women (standardized β = −0.15, P = .02) and young patients (standardized β = −0.20, P = .006) had greater brain resilience to pathological tau. Higher educational level (standardized β = 0.23, P < .001) and global cortical thickness (standardized β = 0.23, P < .001) were associated with greater cognitive resilience to pathological tau. Linear mixed models indicated a significant interaction of brain resilience × cognitive resilience × time on MMSE (β [SE] = −0.235 [0.111], P = .03), with steepest slopes for individuals with both low brain and cognitive resilience.
Conclusions and Relevance
Results of this study suggest that women and young patients with AD have relative preservation of brain structure when exposed to neocortical pathological tau. Interindividual differences in resilience to pathological tau may be important to disease progression because participants with both low brain and cognitive resilience had the most rapid cognitive decline over time.
Positron emission tomography (PET), fluid biomarker, and neuropathological studies1-5 have consistently demonstrated an association between increased pathological tau and decreased cognitive function and brain atrophy across the Alzheimer disease (AD) spectrum. However, the human brain is characterized by remarkable interindividual differences in coping with pathological insults because comparable amounts of pathological burden can result in variable levels of cognitive impairment or neurodegeneration.6 The degree of structural and cognitive loss relative to the pathological burden defines ones resilience, which is considered to be an aggregate term for multiple reserve-related concepts, such as cognitive reserve,7 brain reserve,8 or brain maintenance.9 Resilience can be further divided into brain resilience (BR) (better or worse than expected structural properties of the brain based on the pathological burden) and cognitive resilience (CR) (higher or lower than expected cognitive performance based on the pathological burden).10,11 To date, it is largely unknown which factors contribute to resilience to pathological tau, whether this differs between CR and BR, and whether the level of resilience is associated with rates of longitudinal cognitive decline.
Understanding why some individuals are more resilient to pathological tau than others may provide information for the development of resilience-enhancing therapies and help refine the prognosis in individuals with AD. We therefore measured the total burden of insoluble tau aggregates using flortaucipir labeled with fluor-18 (18F) PET in amyloid-β–positive persons with mild cognitive impairment (MCI due to AD) or AD dementia. We then computed individual resilience scores based on the degree of cortical thickness (BR) or cognition (CR) relative to the total tau burden. Finally, we tested whether demographic (age, sex, and educational level), genetic (APOE-ε4), and imaging markers (cortical thickness and white matter hyperintensities [WMHs]) are associated with between-person variability in CR and BR to pathological tau.12-15 On the basis of an emerging literature highlighting female-specific risks for developing AD,12,16-19 we were particularly interested in potential sex differences in resilience to pathological tau.
This cross-sectional, longitudinal study included 260 patients from the Memory Disorder Clinic of Gangnam Severance Hospital (Seoul, South Korea), the Swedish BioFINDER study at Lund University (Lund, Sweden), and the University of California, San Francisco (UCSF) AD Research Center (San Francisco, California) who underwent [18F]flortaucipir PET from June 1, 2014, to November 30, 2017. All patients tested positive for amyloid-β by PET and/or cerebrospinal fluid analysis (details were reported previously20), 83 were clinically diagnosed with MCI (referred to as MCI due to AD),21 and 177 were diagnosed with AD dementia.22 All underwent medical history and neurologic examination, magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), and neuropsychological testing. Data analysis was performed from October 26, 2018, to December 11, 2019. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants, and local institutional review boards (UCSF, University of California, Berkeley, Lawrence Berkeley National Laboratory, Lund University, Skåne University Hospital, the Swedish Medical Products Agency, and Gangnam Severance Hospital) for human research approved the study. This study followed the Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) reporting guideline.23
Acquisition of PET and MRI Data
The PET images were acquired using a Biograph micro-CT PET/CT scanner (Siemens Medical Solutions) in the Memory Disorder Clinic of Gangnam Severance Hospital, Discovery 690 PET scanner (GE Medical Systems) in the BioFINDER study, and a Biograph 6 Truepoint PET/CT scanner (Siemens Medical Solutions) for UCSF patients. The PET data were locally reconstructed into 4- × 5-minute frames for the 80- to 100-minute interval after injection.15,24,25 The MRIs were acquired on a 3.0-T Discovery MR750 scanner (GE Medical Systems) in the Memory Disorder Clinic of Gangnam Severance Hospital, 3.0-T Tim Trio or Skyra scanner (Siemens Medical Solutions) in the BioFINDER study, and a 3.0-T Tim Trio or Prisma scanner (Siemens Medical Solutions) at UCSF.
T1-Weighted MRI Processing
The MRI data were centrally processed (at Lund University) using previously reported procedures.20 In brief, cortical reconstruction and volumetric segmentation were performed with the FreeSurfer software, version 6.0 image analysis pipelines.26 The magnetization prepared–rapid gradient echo (MP-RAGE) images underwent correction for intensity homogeneity,21 removal of nonbrain tissue,22 and segmentation into gray matter and white matter with intensity gradient and connectivity among voxels.24 Cortical thickness was measured as the distance from the gray matter–white matter boundary to the corresponding pial surface.25 Reconstructed data sets were visually inspected for accuracy, and segmentation errors were corrected. Cortical thickness was determined across the whole cortex for the primary analyses and in frontal, temporal, parietal, and occipital regions of interest for secondary analyses (eTable 1 in the Supplement).
[18F]Flortaucipir PET Processing
PET images were first resampled to obtain the same image size (128 × 128 × 63 matrix) and voxel dimensions (2.0 × 2.0 × 2.0 mm) across centers. Next, PET images were centrally processed (at Lund University) using previously reported procedures.20 [18F]Flortaucipir images were motion corrected using the Analysis of Functional NeuroImages (AFNI) 3dvolreg data set, time averaged, and rigidly coregistered to the skull-stripped MRI. Voxelwise standardized uptake value ratio (SUVR) images were created using inferior cerebellar gray matter as the reference region.27 FreeSurfer software, version 6.0 parcellation of the T1-weighted MRI scan was applied to the PET data transformed to individuals’ native T1 space to extract mean regional SUVRs. We calculated mean [18F]flortaucipir SUVR across the whole cortex for the primary analyses and in frontal, temporal, parietal, and occipital regions of interest for secondary analyses (eTable 1 in the Supplement).
Fluid-Attenuated Inversion Recovery MRI Processing
T2-weighted fluid-attenuated inversion recovery (FLAIR) images were available for 259 of the 260 study participants. We estimated WMH volumes using a segmentation method described elsewhere.28 In brief, this method builds a bayesian probabilistic data model based on a gaussian mixture model with an evolving number of components. Because of distribution skewness, data were log transformed before statistical analysis.
Across the 3 centers, Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) and comparable tests for delayed episodic memory and category fluency were administered. We used data from local cognitively normal individuals as reference to create z scores for delayed episodic memory and category fluency. Furthermore, retrospective and prospective longitudinal MMSE scores were used to model changes in global cognition over time. We acquired 664 data points from 246 patients; 182 had at least 2 time points, with a median of 3 (range, 2-8). The mean (SD) interval between the first and last MMSEs was 2.0 (1.8) years.
We performed (separate) linear regression models between whole-cortex [18F]flortaucipir uptake and cortical thickness (eFigure 1 in the Supplement) and used the standardized residuals as a measure of BR (ie, lower than expected cortical thickness based on [18F]flortaucipir SUVR reflects low BR).29,30 The same procedure was performed using whole-cortex [18F]flortaucipir uptake vs MMSE (CRMMSE) (eFigure 1 in the Supplement), delayed episodic memory recall (CRMEMORY), and category fluency (CRFLUENCY) scores to obtain measures of CR (ie, a lower than expected cognitive score based on [18F]flortaucipir SUVR reflects low CR). Next, bivariate and multivariable linear regression models were performed with age, sex, educational level (as tertiles within each center because of cohort differences), APOE-ε4 status, WMHs (adjusted for intracranial volume), and cortical thickness (surface area weighted; in CR models only) as independent variables and BR and CR measures as dependent variables. The WMH volumes were used as variables and not included in the BR measure because the presence and directionality of an association between pathological tau and WMH volume are not clear.31 In addition to the original 2-step approach (ie, obtainment of residuals from the correlation between [18F]flortaucipir and thickness or cognition followed by bivariable and multivariate regression models), we modeled all variables together. In this linear regression model (simultaneous model), thickness or cognition was the dependent variable with [18F]flortaucipir SUVR and all variables as independent variables. In addition, we grouped patients into BR and CR tertiles and performed bivariate and multivariable multinomial logistic regression models using the same set of variables for BR and CRMMSE. Furthermore, because sex was our primary variable of interest, we tested for interactions between sex and each of the other variables with BR and CRMMSE. In secondary analyses, we examined the regional specificity of the findings by repeating the main analysis but this time using [18F]flortaucipir uptake, cortical thickness, and WMH volumes within 4 regions of interest (ie, frontal, parietal, temporal, and occipital) as measures of BR and CRMMSE. Finally, we examined clinical progression using MMSE score as the outcome variable in linear mixed models, including continuous measures of CR and BR, time, CR × time, BR × time, and BR × CR × time, adjusting for age, sex, and educational level. The model contained random intercept and slopes. For visualization purposes, we created a 4-level CR-BR variable (high CR and BR, high CR and low BR, low CR and high BR, and low CR and BR). The significance level was set at 2-sided P < .05. We used R, version 3.5.1 (The R Project for Statistical Computing) for the statistical analyses.
A total of 260 participants (145 [55.8%] female; mean [SD] age, 69.2 [9.5] years; mean [SD] MMSE score, 21.9 [5.5]) were included in the study. The characteristics of the participants are presented in Table 1 and eTable 2 in the Supplement. The mean (SD) whole cortex [18F]flortaucipir SUVR did not differ between women and men (1.57 [0.40] vs 1.49 [0.39]; F = 2.595; P = .11).
Bivariate models showed that female sex (standardized β [stβ] = −0.186; P = .003), younger age (stβ = −0.301; P < .001), and lower global WMH volumes (stβ = −0.282; P < .001) were associated with greater BR (Table 2 and Figure 1A and B). In the multivariable model, the associations with age (stβ = −0.202; P = .006) and sex (stβ = −0.147; P = .02) remained significant, but the association with WMH volumes did not (stβ = −0.140; P = .06). The simultaneous BR model yielded results comparable to those of the 2-step BR model (eTable 3 in the Supplement). In the 2-step and simultaneous BR models, the associations of age (stβ = −0.249; P = .001 in the 2-step model; stβ = −0.331; P < .001 in the simultaneous model) and sex (stβ = 0.125; P = .052 in the 2-step model; stβ = −0.139; P = .03 in the simultaneous model) with BR remained significant after additional adjustment for center. The multinomial logistic regression models were consistent with the linear regression approach (eTable 4 in the Supplement). No significant interactions were found between sex and any of the other variables on BR (eTable 5 in the Supplement). Regional analyses (ie, BR based on [18F]flortaucipir uptake vs cortical thickness within the 4 major lobes) showed that the associations between age and BR were present in frontal, temporal, and occipital cortexes but not the parietal cortex, whereas the association between sex and BR was only significant in the parietal cortex (Figure 2A). In addition, there was an association between WMH volumes and BR in the temporal cortex (stβ = −0.24; P < .001).
Bivariate models found that younger age (stβ = −0.169; P = .008), higher educational level (stβ = −0.258; P < .001), lower global WMH volumes (stβ = −0.244; P < .001), and greater global cortical thickness (stβ = 0.241; P < .001) were associated with greater CRMMSE (Table 2). In the multivariable model, the associations with higher educational level (stβ = 0.232; P < .001) and cortical thickness (stβ = 0.233; P < .001) remained significant (Figure 1C and D), whereas the association with WMH volumes did not (stβ = −0.139; P = .06). The simultaneous CRMMSE model yielded results comparable to those of the 2-step CRMMSE model (eTable 3 in the Supplement). In both the 2-step and simultaneous BR models, the associations of global cortical thickness (stβ = 0.264; P < .001 in the 2-step model; stβ = 0.220; P < .001 in the simultaneous mode) and educational level (stβ = −0.255; P < .001 in the 2-step model; stβ = 0.233; P = .001 in the simultaneous model) with CRMMSE remained significant after adjustment for center. The multinomial logistic regression CRMMSE models were consistent with the linear regression approach (eTable 6 in the Supplement). We found an interaction between sex and WMH volumes on CRMMSE (β [SE] = 0.571 [0.251]; P = .02), indicating that the associations between WMH volumes and CRMMSE were more pronounced in women than in men. No interactions were found between sex and any of the other variables (eTable 5 in the Supplement).
Regional analyses (ie, CR based on MMSE vs [18F]flortaucipir uptake within the 4 major lobes) found that the associations between educational level and CRMMSE were present across all regions of interest, whereas the associations between cortical thickness and CRMMSE were present in the frontal, parietal, and temporal cortexes and those between WMH volumes and CRMMSE in the occipital and parietal cortexes (Figure 2B).
For delayed episodic memory recall, APOE-ε4–negative participants had greater CRMEMORY in both bivariate (stβ = −0.182) and multivariable (stβ = −0.170) models (Table 2 and Figure 1F). In addition, greater global cortical thickness (stβ = 0.155) was associated with greater CRMEMORY in the multivariable model only (Table 2 and Figure 1 ). For category fluency, bivariate models indicated that APOE-ε4 positivity (stβ = 0.139), lower global WMH volumes (stβ = −0.131), and greater global cortical thickness (stβ = 0.249) were associated with greater CRFLUENCY, but only the association with global cortical thickness (stβ = 0.272) remained significant in the multivariable model (Table 2 and Figure 1G and H).
Longitudinal Cognitive Decline
A significant correlation was found between CR and BR (r = 0.245; P < .001) (Figure 3A). Figure 3B shows the estimated MMSE scores over time. The significant BR × CR × time interaction (β [SE] = −0.235 [0.111]; P = .03 for the 2-step approach;β = −0.378 [0.119]; P = .002 for the simultaneous model) indicates that the cognitive trajectories differed as a function of continuous BR and CR measures. For visualization purposes, we created a 4-level BR and CR measure. Individuals with low CR and BR had the steepest slope (annual β coefficient = −2.55; 95% CI, −2.95 to −2.14), followed by high CR and BR (annual β coefficient = −1.79; 95% CI, −2.43 to −1.14), high CR and low BR (annual β coefficient = −1.47; 95% CI, −1.79 to −1.15), and low CR and high BR (annual β coefficient = −1.47; 95% CI, −2.02 to −0.91) (Figure 3B and eFigure 3 in the Supplement).
In this multicenter study, we examined which demographic, genetic, and neuroimaging factors are associated with BR and CR against pathological tau as measured with [18F]flortaucipir PET. Results from this study suggest that women and young patients with AD have relative preservation of brain structure when exposed to neocortical pathological tau. Interindividual differences in resilience to pathological tau may be important with respect to disease progression because participants with negative BR and CR had the most rapid cognitive decline over time.
Factors Associated With BR
The main finding of this study was the observation of greater BR in women compared with men even after adjusting for age, educational level, WMH volumes, and APOE-ε4 status. In other words, men had lower cortical thickness at similar levels of tau load. Under the assumption that tau aggregates cause neurodegeneration,32 this finding might suggest that female sex is protective against tau-induced cell death. Potential mechanisms include epigenetic changes, such as attenuated alterations in age-related gene expression that involves energy production and an upregulation of the immune system in women compared with men,33,34 as well as sex steroid hormone deficiencies that lead to sex-specific inflammatory responses to neuropathological insult.35 Our results are in line with a series of recent articles indicating that women compared with men had less cognitive impairment at similar levels of pathological tau,18 higher APOE-ε4–mediated cerebrospinal fluid phosphorylated tau levels,17 and greater pathological tau in the entorhinal cortex at similar levels of global amyloid-β burden in cognitively normal individuals.12 Although seemingly counterintuitive, these findings of higher resilience against tau may be congruent with epidemiologic observations of a higher prevalence of AD in women in the general population36 for at least 2 reasons. First, it is important to make the distinction between resistance and resilience against pathological tau.10 Our study found that women were possibly able to better preserve their brain structural properties after exposure to pathological tau, but that does not exclude the possibility that women are more prone to aggregate pathological tau than men. Second, our results fit with the higher life expectancy of women compared with men, especially given that advancing age is a major risk factor for the development of AD.37 Thus, although women may have a more favorable response to pathological tau, this benefit is counteracted by more years of high-risk exposure to pathological AD.
Our finding of greater BR in women could partially be associated with premorbid sex differences in cortical thickness, especially because we investigated brain structure (which is characterized by great interindividual variability in healthy brains) and not pathological molecular findings (which by definition are scarce in healthy brains) as the determinant of BR to pathological tau. Although some disparity exists in the literature, several studies38-41 have found greater thickness of especially the temporal and parietal cortexes in women than in men. Regional analysis of our data indicated that the association of sex with BR was most pronounced in the parietal cortex (Figure 2), but we found in a post hoc analysis that the main association of sex with [18F]flortaucipir uptake and cortical thickness was largely consistent across regions of interest (eFigure 2 in the Supplement). This finding suggests that the association between sex and BR is unlikely to be fully explained by premorbid sex differences in regional brain morphometry. However, longitudinal studies assessing actual change in cortical thickness are needed to confirm whether greater BR in women represents a baseline advantage, an attenuated rate of neurodegeneration compared with men, or a combination of both.
The other factor that contributes to BR was young age. This finding is in accordance with our a priori hypothesis because older patients are more likely to exhibit brain atrophy independent of tau burden (eg, owing to cerebrovascular disease, synaptic loss, or comorbid proteinopathies, such as transactive response DNA binding protein 43 kDa [TDP-43] or α-synuclein).42,43 Furthermore, neuronal repair mechanisms may become less efficient with age,44 which potentially increases the susceptibility to downstream effects of tau aggregates in older participants. Patients with early-onset AD, on the other hand, are characterized by greater baseline tau load and higher rates of tau accumulation rates compared with patients with late-onset AD.15,45,46
Although we did not find an association between educational level and BR, a previous PET study47 indicated that the association between pathological tau and glucose hypometabolism was mitigated by education. This finding could be explained by the use of structural (thickness) vs functional (hypometabolism) outcome measures48 or by differences in disease stage because education is possibly most beneficial in early clinical stages of AD.49 Furthermore, the effect sizes of BR and CR in the present study were small (range, 0.15-0.30),50 although only marginally smaller than those reported for treatment with acetylcholinesterase inhibitors—the current standard of care in MCI due to AD and early AD dementia—for cognitive (Cohen d = 0.29-0.51)51,52 and functional (Cohen d = 0.26)52 outcomes.
Factors Associated With CR
Cognitive resilience was associated with the degree of cortical thickness and educational level, which is in line with previous studies reporting that education13 or highly correlated constructs, such as premorbid IQ,53 help to preserve cognitive function in patients with cortical pathological tau. Furthermore, the negative association of pathological tau with cognition was partially mediated by neurodegeneration.54 In bivariate models, age and WMH volumes were negatively associated with CR. The WMH volumes were also significant in the multivariable model (especially in occipitotemporal regions) (Figure 2), and their associations with CRMMSE were most pronounced in women (eTable 5 in the Supplement). The association of WMH volumes with CR was potentially underestimated in this study because there were no overlapping neuropsychological tests across the 3 centers that specifically captured cognitive functions typically associated with cerebral small vessel disease, such as executive or attentional processes.55
The APOE genotype was differentially associated with BR and CR. For CR, there was a remarkable dissociation because APOE-ε4 positivity was associated with lower CR based on memory performance, whereas absence of an APOE-ε4 allele was associated with lower CR based on a category fluency task. This finding aligns well with the literature because APOE-ε4 carriers have selective vulnerability of the medial temporal lobe and subsequent memory impairment, whereas APOE-ε4–negative patients with AD more often have cortical-predominant atrophy patterns in conjunction with nonamnestic cognitive deficits.56-59 Furthermore, we found no association between APOE-ε4 status and BR. Although APOE-ε4 positivity has been associated with a wide range of morphologic, hypometabolic, and functional alterations in cognitively normal persons,60,61 it is likely that in the clinically and biologically more advanced stage of disease in participants in the present study, neurodegenerative processes overwhelmed the more subtle premorbid association of APOE-ε4 with brain structure.
We found an interaction between CR and BR and change in MMSE scores over time because individuals with low CR and BR progressed faster on the MMSE than individuals with low CR who had high BR. This finding suggests that CR and BR are not only associated with different demographic, genetic, and imaging features, they also provide distinct prognostic information.
Strengths and Limitations
Strengths of the study include the relatively large sample of amyloid-β–positive individuals across the clinical spectrum of AD with imaging, genetic, and demographic data available. The study also has several limitations. First, there were only 3 equivalent cognitive tests available across centers. Although MMSE, delayed recall, and category fluency are important tests, several domains of cognition, such as executive functions or attention, were not sufficiently covered, and the CR scores were based on a single test. Second, educational level differed across the cohorts (mean [SD] years of education: 11 [5] in the Memory Disorder Clinic of Gangnam Severance Hospital, 12 [4] in the BioFINDER study, and 17 [3] in UCSF). This limitation was resolved by creating tertiles within each cohort, but we acknowledge that this approach potentially reduced the sensitivity to detect associations between educational level and BR and/or CR. Third, for the longitudinal analyses, we used an outcome measure (ie, MMSE) that was also used to determine CR. This approach was taken because there were no sufficient longitudinal data points available for the other cognitive tests (ie, delayed episodic memory and category fluency) or another global measure (eg, the clinical dementia rating scale). In addition, the MMSE is a crude measure to capture longitudinal changes in cognition. Fourth, some data were missing that could not be imputed because most relevant variables were already included in our statistical models. Fifth, amyloid-β pathologic findings were assessed using different modalities (PET and CSF analyses) and PET tracers; thus, a continuous measure of amyloid-β could not be entered as a variable in statistical analyses.
In this study, female sex and young age were associated with greater BR against pathological tau, whereas higher educational level and cortical thickness were associated with greater CR. Furthermore, persons who had low CR and BR had the most rapid cognitive decline over time. Thus, CR and BR may be associated with differential mechanisms and may provide complementary prognostic information.
Accepted for Publication: December 19, 2019.
Published Online: February 24, 2020. doi:10.1001/jamaneurol.2019.5154
Open Access: This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the CC-BY License. © 2020 Ossenkoppele R et al. JAMA Neurology.
Corresponding Author: Rik Ossenkoppele, PhD, Clinical Memory Research Unit, Lund University, Simrisbanvagen 14, CA 212 24 Malmö, Sweden (r.ossenkoppele@amsterdamumc.nl); Oskar Hansson, MD, PhD, Clinical Memory Research Unit, Lund University, Simrisbanvagen 14, CA 212 24 Malmö, Sweden (oskar.hansson@med.lu.se).
Author Contributions: Drs Ossenkoppele and Hansson had full access to all the data in the study and take responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.
Concept and design: Ossenkoppele, Hansson.
Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: Ossenkoppele, Lyoo, Jester-Broms, Sudre, Cho, Ryu, Choi, Smith, Strandberg, Palmqvist, Kramer, Boxer, Gorno-Tempini, Miller, La Joie, Rabinovici, Hansson.
Drafting of the manuscript: Ossenkoppele, Hansson.
Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Ossenkoppele, Lyoo, Jester-Broms, Sudre, Cho, Ryu, Choi, Smith, Strandberg, Palmqvist, Kramer, Boxer, Gorno-Tempini, Miller, La Joie, Rabinovici, Hansson.
Statistical analysis: Ossenkoppele, Jester-Broms, Sudre, Strandberg, La Joie.
Obtained funding: Palmqvist, Boxer, Gorno-Tempini, Miller, Rabinovici, Hansson.
Administrative, technical, or material support: Ossenkoppele, Lyoo, Cho, Ryu, Choi, Smith, Strandberg, Palmqvist, Kramer, Boxer, Gorno-Tempini, Miller, La Joie, Hansson.
Supervision: Lyoo, Ryu, Gorno-Tempini, Miller, Hansson.
Conflict of Interest Disclosures: Dr Sudre reported receiving grants from the Alzheimer's Society during the conduct of the study. Dr Boxer reported receiving personal fees and from Arvinas; personal fees from Amgen, Arkuda, AGTC, AbbVie, Asceneuron, Avexis, Ionis, Passage BIO, Lundbeck, UCB, Toyama, Third Rock, Pinteon, Wave, Samumed, and Novartis; grants from Biogen, Cortice, Genentech, Roche, TauRx, and Eli Lilly and Company; and grants and personal fees from Eisai outside the submitted work. Dr Rabinovici reported receiving grants from the National Institutes of Health and Tau Consortium during the conduct of the study; grants from Avid Radiopharmaceuticals and Eli Lilly and Company; grants and personal fees from GE Healthcare; and personal fees from Axon Neurosciences, Eisai, Merck, Roche, and from Genentech outside the submitted work. Dr Hansson reported receiving grants from the Swedish Research Council, Swedish Brain Foundation, Swedish Alzheimer Foundation, and ALF during the conduct of the study and nonfinancial support from AVID Radiopharmaceuticals. No other disclosures were reported.
Funding/Support: Work at Lund University was supported by the European Research Council, the Swedish Research Council, the Marianne and Marcus Wallenberg Foundation, the Knut and Alice Wallenberg Foundation, the Strategic Research Area MultiPark (Multidisciplinary Research in Parkinson’s Disease) at Lund University, the Swedish Brain Foundation, the Swedish Alzheimer Foundation, The Parkinson Foundation of Sweden, The Parkinson Research Foundation, the Skåne University Hospital Foundation, and the Swedish federal government under the ALF agreement. Work at the University of California, San Francisco was supported by grants R01-AG045611, P50-AG023501, P01-AG19724, R01-AG038791, and U54-NS092089 from the National Institute on Aging, grant AARF-16-443577 from the Alzheimer’s Association, the Tau Consortium, and the Michael J. Fox Foundation. Work at Gangnam Severance Hospital was financially supported by grant NRF-2017R1A2B2006694 from the Basic Science Research Program through the National Research Foundation of Korea funded by the Ministry of Science, ICT & Future Planning, and the Korea Health Technology R&D Project through the Korea Health Industry Development Institute funded by grant HI18C1159 from the Ministry of Health & Welfare, Republic of Korea. Doses of flortaucipir labeled with fluro-18 injection were sponsored by Avid Pharmaceuticals.
Role of the Funder/Sponsor: The funding sources had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and the decision to submit the manuscript for publication.
Disclaimer: Gil D. Rabinovici, MD, is Associate Editor, JAMA Neurology, but he was not involved in any of the decisions regarding review of the manuscript or its acceptance.
Additional Contributions: Anita van Loenhoud, PhD, and Diana Bocancea, MSC, Alzheimer Center Amsterdam, Department of Neurology, Amsterdam Neuroscience, Amsterdam University Medical Center, Amsterdam, the Netherlands, provided intellectual input on this article. They were not compensated for their work.
2.Ossenkoppele
R, Smith
R, Ohlsson
T,
et al Associations between tau, Aβ, and cortical thickness with cognition in Alzheimer disease. Neurology. 2019;5;92(6):e601-e612.
3.Jack
CR
Jr, Wiste
HJ, Weigand
SD,
et al. Age-specific and sex-specific prevalence of cerebral β-amyloidosis, tauopathy, and neurodegeneration in cognitively unimpaired individuals aged 50-95 years: a cross-sectional study.
Lancet Neurol. 2017;16(6):435-444. doi:
10.1016/S1474-4422(17)30077-7
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 7.Stern
Y, Arenaza-Urquijo
EM, Bartrés-Faz
D,
et al; Reserve, Resilience and Protective Factors PIA Empirical Definitions and Conceptual Frameworks Workgroup. Whitepaper: defining and investigating cognitive reserve, brain reserve, and brain maintenance.
Alzheimers Dement. 2018;S1552-5260(18)33491-5. doi:
10.1016/j.jalz.2018.07.219
PubMedGoogle Scholar 12.Buckley
RF, Mormino
EC, Rabin
JS,
et al. Sex differences in the association of global amyloid and regional tau deposition measured by positron emission tomography in clinically normal older adults.
JAMA Neurol. 2019;76(5):542-551. doi:
10.1001/jamaneurol.2018.4693
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 15.Schöll
M, Ossenkoppele
R, Strandberg
O,
et al; Swedish BioFINDER study. Distinct 18F-AV-1451 tau PET retention patterns in early- and late-onset Alzheimer’s disease.
Brain. 2017;140(9):2286-2294. doi:
10.1093/brain/awx171
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 17.Hohman
TJ, Dumitrescu
L, Barnes
LL,
et al; Alzheimer’s Disease Genetics Consortium and the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative. Sex-specific association of apolipoprotein E with cerebrospinal fluid levels of tau.
JAMA Neurol. 2018;75(8):989-998. doi:
10.1001/jamaneurol.2018.0821
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 18.Jack
CR
Jr, Therneau
TM, Weigand
SD,
et al. Prevalence of biologically vs clinically defined Alzheimer spectrum entities using the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer's Association Research Framework [published online July 15, 2019]. JAMA Neurol.
19.Ramanan
VK, Castillo
AM, Knopman
DS,
et al. Association of apolipoprotein E ɛ4, educational level, and sex with tau deposition and tau-mediated metabolic dysfunction in older adults.
JAMA Netw Open. 2019;2(10):e1913909. doi:
10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2019.13909
PubMedGoogle Scholar 20.Ossenkoppele
R, Rabinovici
GD, Smith
R,
et al. Discriminative accuracy of [18F]flortaucipir positron emission tomography for Alzheimer disease vs other neurodegenerative disorders.
JAMA. 2018;320(11):1151-1162. doi:
10.1001/jama.2018.12917
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 21.Albert
MS, DeKosky
ST, Dickson
D,
et al. The diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease.
Alzheimers Dement. 2011;7(3):270-279. doi:
10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.008
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 22.McKhann
GM, Knopman
DS, Chertkow
H,
et al. The diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease.
Alzheimers Dement. 2011;7(3):263-269. doi:
10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.005
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 23.von Elm
E, Altman
DG, Egger
M, Pocock
SJ, Gøtzsche
PC, Vandenbroucke
JP; STROBE Initiative. The Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) statement: guidelines for reporting observational studies.
PLoS Med. 2007;4(10):e296. doi:
10.1371/journal.pmed.0040296
PubMedGoogle Scholar 28.Sudre
CH, Cardoso
MJ, Bouvy
WH, Biessels
GJ, Barnes
J, Ourselin
S. Bayesian model selection for pathological neuroimaging data applied to white matter lesion segmentation.
IEEE Trans Med Imaging. 2015;34(10):2079-2102. doi:
10.1109/TMI.2015.2419072
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 46.Pontecorvo
MJ, Devous
MD, Kennedy
I,
et al. A multicentre longitudinal study of flortaucipir (18F) in normal ageing, mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease dementia.
Brain. 2019;142(6):1723-1735. doi:
10.1093/brain/awz090
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 48.Neitzel
J, Franzmeier
N, Rubinski
A, Ewers
M; Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI). Left frontal connectivity attenuates the adverse effect of entorhinal tau pathology on memory.
Neurology. 2019;93(4):e347-e357. doi:
10.1212/WNL.0000000000007822
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 53.Rentz
DM, Mormino
EC, Papp
KV, Betensky
RA, Sperling
RA, Johnson
KA. Cognitive resilience in clinical and preclinical Alzheimer’s disease: the association of amyloid and tau burden on cognitive performance.
Brain Imaging Behav. 2017;11(2):383-390. doi:
10.1007/s11682-016-9640-4
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 55.Alber
J, Alladi
S, Bae
HJ,
et al. White matter hyperintensities in vascular contributions to cognitive impairment and dementia (VCID): knowledge gaps and opportunities.
Alzheimers Dement (N Y). 2019;5:107-117. doi:
10.1016/j.trci.2019.02.001
PubMedGoogle Scholar 56.Wolk
DA, Dickerson
BC; Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative. Apolipoprotein E (APOE) genotype has dissociable effects on memory and attentional-executive network function in Alzheimer’s disease.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010;107(22):10256-10261. doi:
10.1073/pnas.1001412107
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref