[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Figure 1. 
Maximum-likelihood estimated models of cognitive test score change for the Boston Naming Test, New York University (NYU) Story Delayed Recall (Verbatim), Category Verbal Fluency Test, and Luria Mental Rotation Test. PcD indicates Pick disease; AD, Alzheimer disease. The 4 lines within each group, from high to low, correspond to different years of education (20, 16, 12, and 8 years, respectively). For variables affected by age at disease onset (Boston Naming and NYU Delayed Story Recall), models are plotted against age with separate lines for different onset age by decade (50s through 80s). Best-fitting least-squares lines for normal control subjects (NCS) (n = 60) are superimposed for variables plotted by age (Boston Naming, NYU Delayed Story Recall). Patients with PcD declined significantly faster than patients with AD on the Boston Naming Test (P = .04) and Category Verbal Fluency Test (P<.001), but not on the NYU Delayed Story Recall Test and Luria Mental Rotation Test.

Maximum-likelihood estimated models of cognitive test score change for the Boston Naming Test, New York University (NYU) Story Delayed Recall (Verbatim), Category Verbal Fluency Test, and Luria Mental Rotation Test. PcD indicates Pick disease; AD, Alzheimer disease. The 4 lines within each group, from high to low, correspond to different years of education (20, 16, 12, and 8 years, respectively). For variables affected by age at disease onset (Boston Naming and NYU Delayed Story Recall), models are plotted against age with separate lines for different onset age by decade (50s through 80s). Best-fitting least-squares lines for normal control subjects (NCS) (n = 60) are superimposed for variables plotted by age (Boston Naming, NYU Delayed Story Recall). Patients with PcD declined significantly faster than patients with AD on the Boston Naming Test (P = .04) and Category Verbal Fluency Test (P<.001), but not on the NYU Delayed Story Recall Test and Luria Mental Rotation Test.

Figure 2. 
Maximum-likelihood estimated models of change for the Blessed Dementia Scale and Activities of Daily Living Scale. PcD indicates Pick disease; AD, Alzheimer disease. Within each group, separate lines correspond to different educational levels and onset ages as explained for Figure 1, except that higher lines correspond to fewer years of education (because the Blessed Dementia Scale and Activities of Daily Living Scale scores measure severity of illness). On both measures, the patients with PcD worsened significantly faster than the patients with AD (P = .03 and P = .02, respectively).

Maximum-likelihood estimated models of change for the Blessed Dementia Scale and Activities of Daily Living Scale. PcD indicates Pick disease; AD, Alzheimer disease. Within each group, separate lines correspond to different educational levels and onset ages as explained for Figure 1, except that higher lines correspond to fewer years of education (because the Blessed Dementia Scale and Activities of Daily Living Scale scores measure severity of illness). On both measures, the patients with PcD worsened significantly faster than the patients with AD (P = .03 and P = .02, respectively).

Figure 3. 
Maximum-likelihood estimated models of change for the Category Verbal Fluency Test in cases of neuropathologically confirmed Pick disease (PcD; n = 10) and Alzheimer disease (AD; n = 14). The PcD group declined significantly (P = .01) more sharply than the AD group across duration of illness.

Maximum-likelihood estimated models of change for the Category Verbal Fluency Test in cases of neuropathologically confirmed Pick disease (PcD; n = 10) and Alzheimer disease (AD; n = 14). The PcD group declined significantly (P = .01) more sharply than the AD group across duration of illness.

Table 1. 
Demographic Features of PcD, AD, and NCS Groups*
Demographic Features of PcD, AD, and NCS Groups*
Table 2. 
Caregiver Report of Initial Symptoms in Patients With PcD and AD*
Caregiver Report of Initial Symptoms in Patients With PcD and AD*
Table 3. 
Cognitive Test Scores at Initial Examination for PcD, AD, and NCS Groups*
Cognitive Test Scores at Initial Examination for PcD, AD, and NCS Groups*
1.
Pick  A Über die Beziehungen der senilen Hirnatrophie zur Aphasie.  Prager Med Wochenschr. 1892;17165- 167Google Scholar
2.
Pick  A Zur Symptomatologie der linksseitigen Schlafenlappenatrophie.  Monatsschr Psychiatr Neurol. 1904;16378- 388Google ScholarCrossref
3.
Pick  A Über einen weiteren Symptomencomplex im Rahmen der Dementia senilis, bedingt durch umschriebene, stärkere Hirnatrophie.  Monatsschr Psychiatr Neurol. 1906;1997- 108Google ScholarCrossref
4.
Alzheimer  A Über eigenartige Krankheitsfälle des späteren Alters.  Z Ges Neurol Psychiatr. 1911;4356- 385Google ScholarCrossref
5.
Gans  A Betrachtungen über Art und Ausbreitung des krankhaften Prozesses in einem Fall von Pickscher Atrophie des Stirnhirns.  Z Ges Neurol Psychiatr. 1923;8010- 20Google ScholarCrossref
6.
Onari  KSpatz  H Anatomische Beiträge zur Lehre von der Pickschen umschiebenen Grosshirnrindenatrophie.  Z Ges Neurol Psychiatr. 1926;101470- 511Google ScholarCrossref
7.
The Lund and Manchester Groups, Clinical and neuropathological criteria for frontotemporal dementia.  J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1994;57416- 418Google ScholarCrossref
8.
Levy  MLMiller  BLCummings  JL Frontal and frontotemporal dementia. Growdon  JHRossor  MNeds. The Dementias. Boston, Mass Butterworth-Heinemann1998;45- 65Google Scholar
9.
Tissot  RConstantidinidis  JRichard  J Pick's disease. Fredericks  JAMed. Handbook of Clinical Neurology. Amsterdam, the Netherlands Elsevier1985;2233- 246Google Scholar
10.
McKhan  GDrachman  DFolstein  MKatzman  RPrice  DStadlan  EM Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer's Disease.  Neurology. 1984;34939- 944Google ScholarCrossref
11.
Litvan  IAgid  YSastrj  N  et al.  What are the obstacles for an accurate clinical diagnosis of Pick's disease?  Neurology. 1997;4962- 69Google ScholarCrossref
12.
Knopman  DSChristiansen  KJSchut  LJ The spectrum of imaging and neuropsychological findings in Pick's disease.  Neurology. 1989;39362- 368Google ScholarCrossref
13.
Wechsler  AFVerity  MARosenschein  SFried  IScheiber  AB Pick's disease: a clinical, computed tomography, histologic study with Golgi impregnation observations.  Arch Neurol. 1982;39287- 290Google ScholarCrossref
14.
Graff-Radford  NRDamasio  ARHyman  BT  et al.  Progressive aphasia in patients with Pick's disease: a neuropsychological, radiological, and anatomic study.  Neurology. 1990;40620- 626Google ScholarCrossref
15.
Mendez  MFSelwood  AAngeline  RMFrey  WH Pick's disease versus Alzheimer's disease: a comparison of clinical characteristics.  Neurology. 1993;43289- 292Google ScholarCrossref
16.
American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition.  Washington, DC American Psychiatric Association1994;
17.
World Health Organization, The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioral Disorders: Diagnostic Criteria for Research.  Geneva, Switzerland World Health Organization1993;
18.
Blessed  GTomlinson  BERoth  M The association between quantitative measures of dementia and senile change in the gray matter of elderly subjects.  Br J Psychiatry. 1968;114797- 811Google ScholarCrossref
19.
Weintraub  S The record of independent living: an informant-completed measure of activities of daily living and behavior in elderly patients with cognitive impairment.  Am J Alzheimer Care. 1986;135- 39Google ScholarCrossref
20.
Locascio  JJGrowdon  JHCorkin  S Cognitive test performance in detecting, staging, and tracking Alzheimer's disease.  Arch Neurol. 1995;521087- 1099Google ScholarCrossref
21.
Randt  CTBrown  EROsborn  DP A memory test for longitudinal measurement of mild to moderate deficits.  Clin Neuropsychol. 1980;2184- 194Google Scholar
22.
Kimura  D Right temporal-lobe damage.  Arch Neurol. 1963;8264- 271Google ScholarCrossref
23.
Benton  AL The Revised Visual Retention Test.  New York, NY Psychological Corp1974;
24.
Kaplan  EGoodglass  HWeintraub  S Boston Naming Test.  Philadelphia, Pa Lea & Febiger1983;
25.
Huff  FJCorkin  SGrowdon  J Semantic impairment and anomia in Alzheimer's disease.  Brain Lang. 1986;28235- 249Google ScholarCrossref
26.
Lezak  MD Neuropsychological Assessment. 2nd ed. New York, NY Oxford University Press1983;
27.
Stroop  JR Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions.  J Exp Psychol. 1935;18643- 662Google ScholarCrossref
28.
Raven  CJ Guide to Using the Coloured Progressive Matrices.  London, England HK Lewis1965;
29.
Golden  CJHammeke  TAPurish  AD Manual of the Luria-Nebraska Neuropsychological Battery.  Los Angeles, Calif Western Psychological Services1980;
30.
Money  J A Standard Road Map Test of Direction Test.  San Rafael, Calif Academic Therapy Publications1976;
31.
Wechsler  D Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Revised.  New York, NY Psychological Corp1981;
32.
Not Available, The multtest procedure. Technical Report P-229.  Cary, NC SAS Institute Inc1992;369- 405
33.
Not Available, The mixed procedure. Technical Report P-229.  Cary, NC SAS Institute Inc1992;287- 366
34.
Snowden  JSNeary  DMann  DMA Fronto-Temporal Lobar Degeneration: Fronto-Temporal Dementia, Progressive Aphasia, Semantic Dementia.  Edinburgh, Scotland Churchill-Livingstone1996;
35.
Neary  DSnowden  JSGustafson  L  et al.  Frontotemporal lobar degeneration.  Neurology. 1998;511546- 1554Google ScholarCrossref
36.
Heston  LLWhite  JHMastri  AR Pick's disease: clinical genetics and natural history.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1987;44409- 411Google ScholarCrossref
37.
Cummings  JLDuchen  LW Kluver-Bucy syndrome in Pick disease: clinical and pathologic correlation.  Neurology. 1981;311415- 1422Google ScholarCrossref
38.
Heston  LLMastri  AR Age at onset of Pick's and Alzheimer's dementia: implication for diagnosis and research.  J Gerontol. 1982;37422- 424Google ScholarCrossref
39.
Braak  HBraak  E Neuropathological staging of Alzheimer-related changes.  Acta Neuropathol (Berl). 1991;82239- 259Google ScholarCrossref
40.
Arriagada  PVGrowdon  JHHedley-Whyte  ETHyman  BT Neurofibrillary tangles but not senile plaques parallel the duration and severity of Alzheimer's disease.  Neurology. 1992;42631- 639Google ScholarCrossref
41.
Hof  PRBierer  LMPerl  DP Evidence for early vulnerability of the medial and inferior aspects of the temporal lobe in an 82-year-old patient with preclinical signs of dementia: regional and laminar distribution of neurofibrillary tangles and senile plaques.  Arch Neurol. 1992;49946- 953Google ScholarCrossref
42.
Spatz  H Über die Bedeutung der Basalen Rinde: auf Grund von Beobachtungen bei Pickscher Krankheit und bei gedeckten Hirnverletzungen.  Z Ges Neurol Psychiatr. 1937;158208- 232Google ScholarCrossref
43.
Sano  MDevanand  DPRichards  M  et al.  A standardized technique for establishing onset and duration of symptoms of Alzheimer's disease.  Arch Neurol. 1995;52961- 966Google ScholarCrossref
44.
Mega  SMCummings  JLRoss  WFiorello  TGornbein  J The spectrum of behavioral changes in Alzheimer's disease.  Neurology. 1996;46130- 135Google ScholarCrossref
45.
Desimone  RUngerleider  LG Neural mechanisms of visual processing in monkeys. Goodglass  HDamasio  AReds. Handbook of Neuropsychology. Amsterdam, the Netherlands Elsevier1989;267- 300Google Scholar
46.
De Renzi  E Disorders of Space Exploration and Cognition.  London, England John Wiley & Sons Ltd1982;
47.
Lüers  T Über den Verfall der Sprache bei der Pickschen Krankheit.  Erch Psychiatr. 1947;17894- 131Google ScholarCrossref
48.
Munoz-Garcia  DLudwin  SK Classic and generalized variants of Pick's disease: a clinical pathologic ultrastructural and immunocytochemical comparative study.  Ann Neurol. 1984;16467- 480Google ScholarCrossref
49.
Brion  SMikol  JPsimaras  A Recent findings in Pick's disease. Zimmerman  HMed. Progress in Neuropathology. London, England Grune & Stratton1973;2421- 425Google Scholar
50.
Mendez  MFCherrier  MPerryman  KPachana  NMiller  BCummings  JL Frontotemporal dementias versus Alzheimer's disease: differential cognitive features.  Neurology. 1996;471189- 1194Google ScholarCrossref
51.
Pachana  NABoone  KBMiller  BLCummings  JLBerman  N Comparison of neuropsychological functioning in Alzheimer's disease and frontotemporal dementia.  J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 1996;2505- 510Google ScholarCrossref
52.
Holland  ALMcBurney  DHMossy  JReinmuth  OM The dissolution of language in Pick's disease with neurofibrillary tangles: a case study.  Brain Lang. 1985;2436- 58Google ScholarCrossref
53.
Grober  EBuschke  HKawas  CFuld  P Impaired ranking of semantic attributes in dementia.  Brain Lang. 1985;26276- 286Google ScholarCrossref
54.
Diesefeldt  HFA Verbal fluency in senile dementia: an analysis of search and knowledge.  Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 1985;4231- 239Google ScholarCrossref
55.
Nebes  RBoller  FHolland  A Use of a semantic context by patients with Alzheimer's disease.  Psychol Aging. 1986;1261- 269Google ScholarCrossref
56.
Chertkow  HBub  D Semantic memory loss in dementia of Alzheimer's type.  Brain. 1990;113397- 417Google ScholarCrossref
57.
Hodges  JRSalmon  DPButters  N The nature of the naming deficit in Alzheimer's and Huntington's disease.  Brain. 1991;1141547- 1559Google ScholarCrossref
58.
Binetti  GMagni  ECappa  SFPadovani  ABianchetti  ATrabucchi  M Semantic memory in Alzheimer's disease: an analysis of category fluency.  J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1995;1782- 89Google ScholarCrossref
59.
Corkin  SAmaral  DGGonzales  RGJonshon  KAHyman  BT H.M.'s medial temporal-lobe lesion: findings from MRI.  J Neurosci. 1997;173964- 3979Google Scholar
60.
Stern  CECorkin  SGonzales  RG The hippocampal formation participates in novel picture encoding: evidence from functional MRI.  Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1996;9338660- 8665Google ScholarCrossref
61.
Fletcher  PCFrith  CDGrasby  PMShallice  TFrackowiak  RSDolan  RJ Brain systems for encoding and retrieval of auditory-verbal memory: an in vivo study in humans.  Brain. 1995;118401- 416Google ScholarCrossref
62.
Squire  L Memory and hippocampus: a synthesis from findings with rats, monkey, and humans.  Psychol Rev. 1992;99195- 231Google ScholarCrossref
63.
Masliah  ETerry  RDAlford  M Cortical and subcortical patterns of synaptophysin-like immunoreactivity in Alzheimer's disease.  Am J Pathol. 1991;138235- 246Google Scholar
64.
DeKosky  STScheff  SW Synapse loss in frontal cortex biopsies in Alzheimer's disease: correlation with cognitive severity.  Ann Neurol. 1990;27457- 464Google ScholarCrossref
65.
Gomez-Isla  TPrice  JLMcKeel  DWMorris  JCGrowdon  JHHyman  BT Profound loss in layer II entorhinal cortex neurons occurs in very mild Alzheimer's disease.  J Neurosci. 1996;164491- 4500Google Scholar
Original Contribution
February 2000

Differences Between Pick Disease and Alzheimer Disease in Clinical Appearance and Rate of Cognitive Decline

Author Affiliations

From the Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences and the Clinical Research Center, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge (Drs Binetti, Locascio, and Corkin); Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Boston (Drs Binetti, Locascio, and Growdon); Department of Neuropathology, Laboratory for Molecular Neuropathology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown (Dr Vonsattel); and the Alzheimer's Disease Research Unit, Istituto di Ricovero e Cura a Carattere Scientifico, S Giovanni di Dio, Brescia, Italy (Dr Binetti).

Arch Neurol. 2000;57(2):225-232. doi:10.1001/archneur.57.2.225
Abstract

Objectives  To define the cognitive characteristics of Pick disease (PcD), and to determine which features distinguish PcD from Alzheimer disease (AD), in a cross-sectional and longitudinal study.

Methods  The participants were 44 patients with PcD (10 pathologically verified), 121 patients with AD (14 pathologically verified), and 60 normal control subjects. We obtained information regarding the initial symptom of dementia from each patient's caregiver, estimated global dementia severity by the Blessed Dementia Scale and the Activities of Daily Living Scale, and assessed specific cognitive domains by administering 10 tests of memory, language, visuospatial, and reasoning abilities and selective attention.

Results  Among initial symptoms reported by caregivers, personality change and language impairment were significantly more common in PcD than AD; deficits in memory were common in both groups but more prevalent in AD (P<.001). At initial cognitive testing, the scores of patients with PcD were inferior to those of normal controls on all tests, except on a measure of visuospatial function; the scores of patients with AD were inferior to those of controls on all tests. Patients with PcD were superior to patients with AD on measures of explicit memory (P<.001) and visuospatial function (P = .001) but had greater impairments on the Activities of Daily Living Scale (P<.05). During the course of illness, patients with PcD declined significantly faster than those with AD on language tests and on global measures of dementia severity (P<.05), whereas measures of explicit memory and visuospatial and reasoning abilities worsened equally in both patient groups.

Conclusions  There is a characteristic cognitive profile and course of dementia in PcD. Nonetheless, cognitive test performance does not clearly distinguish PcD from AD.

IN 1892, Arnold Pick, a professor of psychiatry at the University of Prague, provided the first description of the clinical and gross pathological features of a dementing illness that now bears his name. Pick observed a 71-year-old man with a history of dementia lasting 3 years. The associated features included prominent aphasia with many paraphasic errors. The autopsy disclosed prominent atrophy of the frontal and temporal lobes.1 Pick subsequently described other cases of dementia with cerebral atrophy localized to the temporal lobes2 and to the parietal and frontal lobes.3 Neither Pick nor his pathologist collaborator Chiari commented on the histological nature of the atrophy they observed, but several years later, Alzheimer described the histological characteristics of the disease as "spongy cortical wasting, ballooned cells, and argentophilic globes in neuronal cytoplasm."4(p383) Neurofibrillary tangles and senile plaques were notably absent. Gans5 and Onari and Spatz6 proposed the name Pick disease (PcD) for this neuropathological entity. According to a new proposed classification of focal cortical atrophies, PcD is now considered a subtype of a broad category of frontotemporal degeneration (FTD).7,8

From a clinical point of view, PcD is difficult to differentiate from other forms of progressive dementia and is often confused with Alzheimer disease (AD).9-12 There are relatively few comprehensive studies that report the clinical and behavioral features of PcD. What studies exist report either single cases or small numbers of cases12-14 or, when based on a large sample, rely on analyses of data collected retrospectively.15 In taking a fresh look at PcD, we based our study on a relatively large number of patients with PcD who were examined longitudinally over time with the same comprehensive set of cognitive tests. The goals of the study were (1) to document the early cognitive features of PcD; (2) to describe the rate of cognitive decline in PcD; and (3) to distinguish PcD from AD in terms of clinical characteristics, including initial symptoms and course of dementia progression.

Subjects and methods
Subjects

This study was based on data collected from 34 clinically diagnosed and an additional 10 neuropathologically diagnosed patients with PcD (Table 1), all of whom were examined between January 1, 1985, and December 31, 1996, in the Memory Disorders Unit (MDU) of the Massachusetts General Hospital, Boston. These 44 cases represented 2% of all patients with dementia examined in the MDU during this period. During the same time, the prevalence of AD was 63%. These percentages are comparable with those recorded in the MDU Brain Bank: of 605 autopsies conducted between 1985 and 1996, 54% had AD and 3% had PcD or other variants of lobar atrophy. The clinical diagnosis of PcD was made in individuals with progressive dementia, prominent language impairments, and subtle personality changes; these features meet current criteria of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition,16 and International Classification of Diseases, 10th Revision (ICD-10).17 Three of the 44 patients with PcD in the sample had an antemortem diagnosis of AD but a neuropathological diagnosis of PcD; their behavioral data are included in the PcD group. All patients with an antemortem diagnosis of PcD who underwent autopsy had PcD neuropathologically; none had AD. All of the patients with autopsy-confirmed AD had a clinical diagnosis of AD; none had a clinical diagnosis of PcD. The neuropathological examination in all 10 cases of PcD demonstrated lobar atrophy with ballooned neurons and numerous Pick bodies in the damaged brain regions.

Methods

The course of dementia was assessed in all 44 patients with PcD by the Blessed Dementia Scale (BDS)18 and Activities of Daily Living Scale (ADL)19 scores. In addition to the BDS and ADL, 33 of the 44 completed, at least once, a set of standardized cognitive tests that examined memory, language capacities, visuospatial skills, abstract reasoning abilities, and selective attention20-31; 19 (6 studied by autopsy and 13 living) had 2 or more examinations (with a mean of 1.1 years between the first 2 sessions), 7 had 3 or more, 2 had 4 or more, and 1 had 6. They were followed up for a mean of 1.6 years (SD, 1.0 year; range, 0.5-4.4 years). The demographic characteristics of this subgroup of 33 did not differ significantly from those of the total group of 44 (10 studied by autopsy, 23 living; 21 men, 12 women; mean age, 66.2 ± 9.1 years; education, 14.3 ± 3.3 years; age at onset of disease, 63.4 ± 9.4 years; unless otherwise indicated, data are given as mean ± SD). The remaining 11 patients with PcD were unable to complete the full set of cognitive tests.

We compared the patients with PcD with a pool of 123 patients with AD20 (Table 1). These 123 were all of the patients examined in the MDU at Massachusetts General Hospital between 1985 and 1993 who met these criteria: a clinical diagnosis of probable AD according to the National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke–Alzheimer's Disease and Related Disorders Association guidelines10 and completion of the same cognitive tests as those administered to the patients with PcD.20 Of the 123 patients with AD, 34 died and an autopsy was performed in 16. The clinical diagnosis of AD was confirmed neuropathologically in 14 patients; 2 patients had a neuropathological diagnosis of PcD, and their clinical data were removed from the AD group and incorporated into the PcD group for this study. Another patient with an antemortem diagnosis of AD had autopsy-proved PcD. This subject had been examined in the MDU but was not part of the original cohort of 123 patients with AD. All 121 of the remaining patients with AD had at least 2 testing sessions (with a mean of 1 year between the first 2 sessions); 49 had 3 or more testings, 19 had 4 or more, 5 had 5 or more, and 3 had 6. They were followed up for a mean of 1.8 years (SD, 1.1 years; range, 0.5 to 5.3 years).

The normal control subjects (NCSs) included 60 older subjects living in the community (Table 1). They were generally spouses or caregivers of the patients, and some were volunteers from the Geriatric Education Center of the Harvard Medical School Division on Aging, Boston, Mass. None had past or current histories of neurologic or psychiatric disorders. They underwent the same clinical and cognitive evaluations as did the patients with PcD and AD. Of the 60 NCSs, 11 had 2 or more testing sessions with an average of 2.5 years between the first 2 testings. The NCSs were followed up for a mean of 0.7 year (SD, 1.5 years; most had only 1 testing; otherwise, range was 0.5-6.3 years).

Statistical analysis

The continuous demographic variables and the scores of the first cognitive assessment, including BDS and ADL scores, for the 3 groups were compared by means of analysis of variance, analysis of covariance, and Tukey post hoc test. The χ2 and Fisher exact tests were used for qualitative variables (ie, sex and initial symptoms). The significance tests for the Fisher exact tests of initial symptoms were corrected for multiple testing with a resampling method.32 Tukey post hoc tests corrected for multiple group comparisons on demographics.

To delineate differences between the PcD and AD groups, we modeled change in cognitive test performance, BDS, and ADL vs duration of illness and age. The change models were derived with the use of mixed random and fixed coefficient linear regression models.33 Maximum likelihood estimation techniques determined population models for which our data had the highest probability of occurrence. These methods had the advantage of including all available clinical data in the analyses, regardless of the varying numbers of test dates per subject and unequal test intervals. For all cognitive tests, higher scores indicated better performance; whereas, for the BDS and ADL, higher scores indicated greater disease severity. Age at onset of illness, education level, and sex were entered into the models as covariates to determine whether they were significantly related to the dependent variables. We determined whether relations between test performance and duration of illness (or age) were linear or curvilinear by testing quadratic terms for duration of illness (or age).

Results
Demographic findings

The PcD, AD, and NCS groups did not differ significantly in mean age at first evaluation (Table 1). The patients with PcD did not differ significantly from either of the other groups in mean education, but the NCS group had a higher mean education level than the AD group (P = .02). The proportion of men in the PcD group was significantly greater than in the AD group (P = .02). The reported age at disease onset was significantly lower in the PcD than the AD group (P=.02), but the 2 groups did not differ in mean interval from disease onset to the initial clinical examination in the MDU. To ensure that the total PcD sample represented a homogeneous group of patients, we compared the demographic variables and cognitive test scores at initial evaluation of the 10 patients with PcD who underwent autopsy with those of the 34 living patients with PcD. The 2 groups of patients with PcD did not differ significantly on any demographic variable (respective values for the autopsy vs living groups: sex, 6 men and 4 women vs 20 men and 14 women; mean age at disease onset, 64.1 ± 7.9 vs 63.5 ± 10.0 years; education, 13.8 ± 3.5 vs 14.3 ± 3.6 years; mean age at first evaluation, 66.8 ± 6.4 vs 67.0 ± 10.0 years), mean duration of the illness at first testing (2.7 ± 1.9 vs 2.9 ± 1.6 years), or cognitive test score at initial examination.

First symptoms of disease

As part of the initial neurologic examination, the caregiver of each patient with PcD and AD was asked, "When did the illness begin?" and "What were the first symptoms of the illness?" Caregivers selected the initial symptoms during a structured interview from a menu that included impairment in 11 domains (Table 2). Of the 11 symptoms queried, patients with PcD and AD differed significantly on 3: memory (episodic memory), speech (word finding), and personality problems (defined by the presence of 1 or more of the following symptoms: apathy, agitation, depression, irritability, appetite change, disinhibition, and euphoria). Memory loss was the most common initial symptom in patients with PcD and AD but was significantly more prevalent in AD than in PcD (P<.001). In contrast, speech and personality change as the first symptom were more prevalent in PcD (P = .03 and P = .07, respectively).

Initial behavioral testing

For the cognitive tests administered at first clinical examination, analyses covaried age, educational level, and duration of illness. The mean scores for NCSs were significantly superior to those for the patients with AD on all tests and were significantly superior to those for the patients with PcD on all tests except the Luria Mental Rotation Test (Table 3). As a group, patients with PcD performed better than patients with AD on tests of explicit memory (New York University [NYU] Delayed Story Recall: F = 11.47; P<.001) and visuospatial skill (Luria Mental Rotation Test: F = 11.17; P = .001). Mean scores on all other cognitive tests did not differ between the PcD and AD groups. The mean BDS scores at first evaluation for the PcD and AD groups did not differ, whereas the PcD group had a higher mean ADL score (P = .049) than the AD group, indicating greater functional impairment in PcD (Table 1). Although these differences were statistically significant, they were not large enough to provide discriminant utility. Using logistic regression analysis with jacknife classification, we examined initial test scores for NYU Delayed Story Recall, Luria Mental Rotation, and ADL in separate analyses. In each case, optimal cutoffs provided only 44% to 67% sensitivity for PcD, with 60% to 69% specificity.

Rate of progression

The rate of deterioration was significantly greater for patients with PcD than AD on the Boston Naming Test, Category Verbal Fluency Test, BDS, and ADL (Figure 1 and Figure 2). The rates of decline for the PcD and AD groups did not differ significantly on the Raven's Progressive Matrices Test, the Stroop Color Naming Test, Geometric (easy and hard) Figure 1 Copy and Recall, Benton Visual Retention Test, NYU Stories Immediate and Delayed Recall, Money Road Map Test, Luria Mental Rotation Test, and Picture Arrangement Test.

For all 3 groups, there was a slightly curvilinear decline in the Boston Naming Test score. The best-fitting model (Figure 1) showed that patients with PcD had a steeper curvilinear change over time than patients with AD (P = .04). Category Verbal Fluency Test scores declined linearly; the model showed a significant interaction (P<.001) between duration of disease and group in that the slope of decline was significantly greater for patients with PcD (Figure 1). Mean BDS and ADL scores for patients with PcD increased (ie, worsened) significantly faster than those for patients with AD. For both measures, there were significant group differences in curvilinear change (P = .03 and P = .02, respectively), but not in the level of performance (Figure 2). Patients with PcD had a higher level of performance than patients with AD on the NYU Delayed Story Recall Test (P = .004; for patients studied by autopsy only, P = .008), and the Luria Mental Rotation Test (P = .07), but the slope of decline for the 2 patient groups did not differ (Figure 1). For NYU Delayed Story Recall, the change was curvilinear, whereas for Luria Mental Rotation the effects were linear. Additional analyses indicated that these results on rate of cognitive progression that distinguish PcD from AD did not interact significantly with whether patients were living or dead. Even within the relatively small number of cases studied by autopsy, the rate of decline on the Category Verbal Fluency Test was significantly greater in PcD than in AD (Figure 3).

Comment

This report on PcD is the first, to our knowledge, in which the cognitive aspects of the disease were examined in a comprehensive cross-sectional and longitudinal study. The patients with PcD, patients with AD, and NCSs received the same systematic, standardized assessment, permitting a rigorous observation of the differences among the 3 groups. The majority of our patients with PcD and AD are still alive, and in some the clinical diagnosis may not be confirmed pathologically. To enhance confidence in our findings, we compared initial cognitive test scores and subsequent rates of progression for living and autopsy-studied patients with PcD and AD, and found no evidence in either patient group that the data for the pathologically confirmed cases differed from the clinically diagnosed and still-living cases. A separate analysis of cognitive test scores restricted only to autopsy-studied cases reinforced the conclusions derived from the entire subject sample: at first examination, patients with AD were significantly more impaired on NYU Delayed Story Recall than patients with PcD, whereas the rate of decline on Category Verbal Fluency was significantly greater in patients with PcD than in patients with AD.

Not all cases of dementia with frontal and temporal atrophy contain Pick bodies. It is controversial whether Pick bodies must be present for PcD to be diagnosed or, conversely, whether the absence of Pick bodies excludes a diagnosis of PcD. From a neuropathological point of view, the requirement for Pick bodies is seductive but probably too restrictive, in that it would preclude the antemortem diagnosis of PcD. In this regard, Pick's original observations were clinical and did not rely on microscopic examination of the brain. To minimize nosologic confusion, the broad diagnostic term FTD has been proposed to encompass PcD, and it contains other focal lobar atrophies including those that result in the syndromes of primary progressive aphasia, semantic dementia, frontal lobe dementia, frontal lobe dementia with amyotrophic sclerosis, frontotemporal dementia with Parkinson disease linked to chromosome 17, and corticobasal degeneration.7,8,34 This classification scheme emphasizes the belief that the topography of the atrophy is more responsible for the clinical manifestations than is the underlying histopathologic condition. Although criteria have been proposed to diagnose FTD,7,35 these conditions are still difficult to distinguish from themselves and from other dementias. None of the 44 cases in the present series conformed to the phenomenological descriptions of these other FTD conditions. We use the specific term PcD when it seems diagnostically accurate rather than the broad generic term FTD because of the historical precedent for PcD, because we began collecting our series of patients many years before the FTD classification was proposed, and because all of our cases studied by autopsy did in fact have the gross and microscopic findings of PcD. The analyses of the demographic characteristics of the PcD cohort reinforce previous reports of an increased prevalence in men36 and an earlier disease onset than AD.12,37,38

First symptoms

To gain information regarding the nature of the initial symptom(s) and the estimated date of disease onset, we queried the patient's caregiver. Memory, speech, and personality changes were the 3 reported symptoms that differentiated patients with PcD from those with AD. Our data confirm that memory impairment, specifically affecting explicit memory, is the chief hallmark of AD and is much more prevalent in AD than in PcD. This result is consistent with neuropathological findings that medial temporal-lobe structures are damaged early in the course of AD.39-41 Language disturbances, mainly word-finding difficulty, and personality changes are significantly more common in PcD than in AD. Personality changes may stem from diffuse damage to the amygdala, in contrast to the selective nuclear damage in AD.37 Anomia may reflect the underlying atrophy and neuronal death in the temporal and frontal lobes that characterize PcD pathologically.9,15,37,42 Although the differences in frequency of initial symptoms are statistically significant, overlaps preclude diagnostic specificity, and it remains a challenge to obtain reliable and diagnostically discriminant information regarding the onset of cognitive43 and behavioral44 symptoms in dementia.

Initial evaluation

On initial cognitive testing, the mean scores for patients with PcD were inferior to those of the NCSs, except for the Luria Mental Rotation Test, whereas the mean scores for the patients with AD were inferior to those for the NCSs in all cognitive domains. The Luria Mental Rotation Test requires the mental manipulation of spatial information. The processing of spatial information depends on parietal lobe and occipital lobe functioning,45 with the right hemisphere playing a predominant role.46 The preservation of these cognitive capacities may reflect the relative preservation of the posterior cortical areas in PcD.3,4,47-50 Damage to frontal lobes may account for the observation that executive functions are relatively more impaired in PcD than memory capacities, and the resultant suggestion that this pattern may aid in differentiating PcD from AD.51

Patients with PcD are often misdiagnosed as having AD. To determine whether patients with PcD displayed a cognitive profile that distinguished them from patients with AD, we compared the cognitive assessment of the 2 groups, adjusting for duration of disease, education, and age. At initial examination, mean dementia severity, as estimated by the BDS scores, was equal in both groups. Although some significant differences were found between the PcD and AD groups on individual tests, these differences were not sufficiently numerous or large to define a pathognomonic PcD cognitive profile. Specifically, patients with PcD had spared visuospatial abilities and a relative but significant preservation of the capacity to recall verbal material compared with patients with AD. Among the cases studied by autopsy, patients with AD were significantly (P = .008) impaired in verbal recall compared with patients with PcD. Surprisingly, the PcD and AD groups did not differ at initial examination on 2 tests of language abilities, the Boston Naming Test and Category Verbal Fluency Test, or on a measure of selective attention, the Stroop Color Naming Test. Overall, we conclude that the cross-sectional analysis of a single set of cognitive and ADL measures is not sufficient to disclose all of the potential differences between patients with PcD and those with AD.

Rate of progression

A longitudinal study contains more information than a cross-sectional one, so we analyzed and compared the progression of cognitive test, BDS, and ADL scores for patients with PcD and AD. Patients with PcD worsened over time significantly faster than patients with AD on measures of language, global cognitive impairment, and ADL. The 2 patient groups did not differ in rate of decline on all other cognitive tests. Because initial Boston Naming Test and Category Verbal Fluency Test scores were equivalent, the more rapid decline in scores in the PcD group compared with the AD group likely reflects the biological characteristics of the disease and not a diagnostic selection bias. The accelerated decline in PcD remained even after adjusting for education and age at disease onset. This finding was also evident in the cases studied by autopsy, where the rate of decline in verbal fluency was significantly greater in the PcD group than in the AD group. Progressive language deterioration is a common feature in single case reports of PcD; anomia, neologisms, phonemic paraphasias, echolalia, and verbal stereotypy have all been reported.13,14,52 Our data reinforce the diagnostic importance of aphasia in PcD and emphasize rapid deterioration in expressive language as a marker of PcD. With the Boston Naming and Category Verbal Fluency Tests, we assessed only 2 aspects of language function. Inability to name objects and retrieve words from the lexicon is also common in AD,20,25,53-58 and further research is needed to explore differences between these 2 groups of dementia.

Patients with PcD showed less impairment than patients with AD on tests of explicit memory at initial examination and during the disease course, but the rate of progression was similar, both in the autopsy-studied cases and in the total sample including living patients. Explicit memory depends to a great extent on medial temporal-lobe structures,59 which participate in the initial encoding and storage of information.60 Medial temporal-lobe structures, including the entorhinal cortex and hippocampus, are damaged early in the course of AD,39-41 but these structures are also affected in PcD.42 Cortical pathologic changes may also contribute to the progressive memory deficits in both diseases. Memories are likely stored outside the hippocampal formation in cortical regions.61,62 Accumulation of neurofibrillary tangles,40 decreases in synapses,63,64 and loss of neurons65 in the cortex underlie disease progression in AD, while widespread cortical atrophy with frontal and temporal lobe accentuation are prominent features of PcD. In both diseases, ongoing neuronal degeneration in cortical regions could produce progressive deficits in explicit memory. The similar rate of decline for the 2 groups on delayed recall of the NYU Stories and Geometric Figures suggests that the specific nature of the histopathologic changes are less important determinants of progression rate than are the loci of the lesions.

In conclusion, the observations reported in this study document cognitive and ADL differences in the appearance and course of PcD compared with AD. These cross-sectional and longitudinal cognitive test data provide a clinical index of the underlying pathologic changes in the brain. Despite distinctive differences, however, substantial overlaps between the test performance of patients with PcD and AD limit the diagnostic value of the cognitive testing. To enhance diagnostic precision for PcD, it will be necessary to couple cognitive abnormalities with distinctive biological markers, such as gene abnormalities or characteristic patterns of impaired cerebral metabolism.

Accepted for publication June 24, 1999.

This research was supported by grants AG05134 (Drs Locascio, Vonsattel, and Growdon) and AG06605 (Dr Corkin) from the National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, Md, and by a grant from the Sacro Cuore Fatebenefratelli Institute, Brescia, Italy (Dr Binetti). The Massachusetts Institute of Technology Clinical Research Center, Cambridge, Mass, is supported by National Institutes of Health grant RR 00088. The Older Americans Independence Center of the Harvard Medical School Division on Aging, Boston, Mass, is supported by National Institutes of Health grant AG08812.

Reprints: John H. Growdon, MD, WACC 830, Massachusetts General Hospital, Boston, MA 02114 (e-mail: growdon@helix.mgh.harvard.edu).

References
1.
Pick  A Über die Beziehungen der senilen Hirnatrophie zur Aphasie.  Prager Med Wochenschr. 1892;17165- 167Google Scholar
2.
Pick  A Zur Symptomatologie der linksseitigen Schlafenlappenatrophie.  Monatsschr Psychiatr Neurol. 1904;16378- 388Google ScholarCrossref
3.
Pick  A Über einen weiteren Symptomencomplex im Rahmen der Dementia senilis, bedingt durch umschriebene, stärkere Hirnatrophie.  Monatsschr Psychiatr Neurol. 1906;1997- 108Google ScholarCrossref
4.
Alzheimer  A Über eigenartige Krankheitsfälle des späteren Alters.  Z Ges Neurol Psychiatr. 1911;4356- 385Google ScholarCrossref
5.
Gans  A Betrachtungen über Art und Ausbreitung des krankhaften Prozesses in einem Fall von Pickscher Atrophie des Stirnhirns.  Z Ges Neurol Psychiatr. 1923;8010- 20Google ScholarCrossref
6.
Onari  KSpatz  H Anatomische Beiträge zur Lehre von der Pickschen umschiebenen Grosshirnrindenatrophie.  Z Ges Neurol Psychiatr. 1926;101470- 511Google ScholarCrossref
7.
The Lund and Manchester Groups, Clinical and neuropathological criteria for frontotemporal dementia.  J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1994;57416- 418Google ScholarCrossref
8.
Levy  MLMiller  BLCummings  JL Frontal and frontotemporal dementia. Growdon  JHRossor  MNeds. The Dementias. Boston, Mass Butterworth-Heinemann1998;45- 65Google Scholar
9.
Tissot  RConstantidinidis  JRichard  J Pick's disease. Fredericks  JAMed. Handbook of Clinical Neurology. Amsterdam, the Netherlands Elsevier1985;2233- 246Google Scholar
10.
McKhan  GDrachman  DFolstein  MKatzman  RPrice  DStadlan  EM Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer's Disease.  Neurology. 1984;34939- 944Google ScholarCrossref
11.
Litvan  IAgid  YSastrj  N  et al.  What are the obstacles for an accurate clinical diagnosis of Pick's disease?  Neurology. 1997;4962- 69Google ScholarCrossref
12.
Knopman  DSChristiansen  KJSchut  LJ The spectrum of imaging and neuropsychological findings in Pick's disease.  Neurology. 1989;39362- 368Google ScholarCrossref
13.
Wechsler  AFVerity  MARosenschein  SFried  IScheiber  AB Pick's disease: a clinical, computed tomography, histologic study with Golgi impregnation observations.  Arch Neurol. 1982;39287- 290Google ScholarCrossref
14.
Graff-Radford  NRDamasio  ARHyman  BT  et al.  Progressive aphasia in patients with Pick's disease: a neuropsychological, radiological, and anatomic study.  Neurology. 1990;40620- 626Google ScholarCrossref
15.
Mendez  MFSelwood  AAngeline  RMFrey  WH Pick's disease versus Alzheimer's disease: a comparison of clinical characteristics.  Neurology. 1993;43289- 292Google ScholarCrossref
16.
American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition.  Washington, DC American Psychiatric Association1994;
17.
World Health Organization, The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioral Disorders: Diagnostic Criteria for Research.  Geneva, Switzerland World Health Organization1993;
18.
Blessed  GTomlinson  BERoth  M The association between quantitative measures of dementia and senile change in the gray matter of elderly subjects.  Br J Psychiatry. 1968;114797- 811Google ScholarCrossref
19.
Weintraub  S The record of independent living: an informant-completed measure of activities of daily living and behavior in elderly patients with cognitive impairment.  Am J Alzheimer Care. 1986;135- 39Google ScholarCrossref
20.
Locascio  JJGrowdon  JHCorkin  S Cognitive test performance in detecting, staging, and tracking Alzheimer's disease.  Arch Neurol. 1995;521087- 1099Google ScholarCrossref
21.
Randt  CTBrown  EROsborn  DP A memory test for longitudinal measurement of mild to moderate deficits.  Clin Neuropsychol. 1980;2184- 194Google Scholar
22.
Kimura  D Right temporal-lobe damage.  Arch Neurol. 1963;8264- 271Google ScholarCrossref
23.
Benton  AL The Revised Visual Retention Test.  New York, NY Psychological Corp1974;
24.
Kaplan  EGoodglass  HWeintraub  S Boston Naming Test.  Philadelphia, Pa Lea & Febiger1983;
25.
Huff  FJCorkin  SGrowdon  J Semantic impairment and anomia in Alzheimer's disease.  Brain Lang. 1986;28235- 249Google ScholarCrossref
26.
Lezak  MD Neuropsychological Assessment. 2nd ed. New York, NY Oxford University Press1983;
27.
Stroop  JR Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions.  J Exp Psychol. 1935;18643- 662Google ScholarCrossref
28.
Raven  CJ Guide to Using the Coloured Progressive Matrices.  London, England HK Lewis1965;
29.
Golden  CJHammeke  TAPurish  AD Manual of the Luria-Nebraska Neuropsychological Battery.  Los Angeles, Calif Western Psychological Services1980;
30.
Money  J A Standard Road Map Test of Direction Test.  San Rafael, Calif Academic Therapy Publications1976;
31.
Wechsler  D Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Revised.  New York, NY Psychological Corp1981;
32.
Not Available, The multtest procedure. Technical Report P-229.  Cary, NC SAS Institute Inc1992;369- 405
33.
Not Available, The mixed procedure. Technical Report P-229.  Cary, NC SAS Institute Inc1992;287- 366
34.
Snowden  JSNeary  DMann  DMA Fronto-Temporal Lobar Degeneration: Fronto-Temporal Dementia, Progressive Aphasia, Semantic Dementia.  Edinburgh, Scotland Churchill-Livingstone1996;
35.
Neary  DSnowden  JSGustafson  L  et al.  Frontotemporal lobar degeneration.  Neurology. 1998;511546- 1554Google ScholarCrossref
36.
Heston  LLWhite  JHMastri  AR Pick's disease: clinical genetics and natural history.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1987;44409- 411Google ScholarCrossref
37.
Cummings  JLDuchen  LW Kluver-Bucy syndrome in Pick disease: clinical and pathologic correlation.  Neurology. 1981;311415- 1422Google ScholarCrossref
38.
Heston  LLMastri  AR Age at onset of Pick's and Alzheimer's dementia: implication for diagnosis and research.  J Gerontol. 1982;37422- 424Google ScholarCrossref
39.
Braak  HBraak  E Neuropathological staging of Alzheimer-related changes.  Acta Neuropathol (Berl). 1991;82239- 259Google ScholarCrossref
40.
Arriagada  PVGrowdon  JHHedley-Whyte  ETHyman  BT Neurofibrillary tangles but not senile plaques parallel the duration and severity of Alzheimer's disease.  Neurology. 1992;42631- 639Google ScholarCrossref
41.
Hof  PRBierer  LMPerl  DP Evidence for early vulnerability of the medial and inferior aspects of the temporal lobe in an 82-year-old patient with preclinical signs of dementia: regional and laminar distribution of neurofibrillary tangles and senile plaques.  Arch Neurol. 1992;49946- 953Google ScholarCrossref
42.
Spatz  H Über die Bedeutung der Basalen Rinde: auf Grund von Beobachtungen bei Pickscher Krankheit und bei gedeckten Hirnverletzungen.  Z Ges Neurol Psychiatr. 1937;158208- 232Google ScholarCrossref
43.
Sano  MDevanand  DPRichards  M  et al.  A standardized technique for establishing onset and duration of symptoms of Alzheimer's disease.  Arch Neurol. 1995;52961- 966Google ScholarCrossref
44.
Mega  SMCummings  JLRoss  WFiorello  TGornbein  J The spectrum of behavioral changes in Alzheimer's disease.  Neurology. 1996;46130- 135Google ScholarCrossref
45.
Desimone  RUngerleider  LG Neural mechanisms of visual processing in monkeys. Goodglass  HDamasio  AReds. Handbook of Neuropsychology. Amsterdam, the Netherlands Elsevier1989;267- 300Google Scholar
46.
De Renzi  E Disorders of Space Exploration and Cognition.  London, England John Wiley & Sons Ltd1982;
47.
Lüers  T Über den Verfall der Sprache bei der Pickschen Krankheit.  Erch Psychiatr. 1947;17894- 131Google ScholarCrossref
48.
Munoz-Garcia  DLudwin  SK Classic and generalized variants of Pick's disease: a clinical pathologic ultrastructural and immunocytochemical comparative study.  Ann Neurol. 1984;16467- 480Google ScholarCrossref
49.
Brion  SMikol  JPsimaras  A Recent findings in Pick's disease. Zimmerman  HMed. Progress in Neuropathology. London, England Grune & Stratton1973;2421- 425Google Scholar
50.
Mendez  MFCherrier  MPerryman  KPachana  NMiller  BCummings  JL Frontotemporal dementias versus Alzheimer's disease: differential cognitive features.  Neurology. 1996;471189- 1194Google ScholarCrossref
51.
Pachana  NABoone  KBMiller  BLCummings  JLBerman  N Comparison of neuropsychological functioning in Alzheimer's disease and frontotemporal dementia.  J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 1996;2505- 510Google ScholarCrossref
52.
Holland  ALMcBurney  DHMossy  JReinmuth  OM The dissolution of language in Pick's disease with neurofibrillary tangles: a case study.  Brain Lang. 1985;2436- 58Google ScholarCrossref
53.
Grober  EBuschke  HKawas  CFuld  P Impaired ranking of semantic attributes in dementia.  Brain Lang. 1985;26276- 286Google ScholarCrossref
54.
Diesefeldt  HFA Verbal fluency in senile dementia: an analysis of search and knowledge.  Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 1985;4231- 239Google ScholarCrossref
55.
Nebes  RBoller  FHolland  A Use of a semantic context by patients with Alzheimer's disease.  Psychol Aging. 1986;1261- 269Google ScholarCrossref
56.
Chertkow  HBub  D Semantic memory loss in dementia of Alzheimer's type.  Brain. 1990;113397- 417Google ScholarCrossref
57.
Hodges  JRSalmon  DPButters  N The nature of the naming deficit in Alzheimer's and Huntington's disease.  Brain. 1991;1141547- 1559Google ScholarCrossref
58.
Binetti  GMagni  ECappa  SFPadovani  ABianchetti  ATrabucchi  M Semantic memory in Alzheimer's disease: an analysis of category fluency.  J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1995;1782- 89Google ScholarCrossref
59.
Corkin  SAmaral  DGGonzales  RGJonshon  KAHyman  BT H.M.'s medial temporal-lobe lesion: findings from MRI.  J Neurosci. 1997;173964- 3979Google Scholar
60.
Stern  CECorkin  SGonzales  RG The hippocampal formation participates in novel picture encoding: evidence from functional MRI.  Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1996;9338660- 8665Google ScholarCrossref
61.
Fletcher  PCFrith  CDGrasby  PMShallice  TFrackowiak  RSDolan  RJ Brain systems for encoding and retrieval of auditory-verbal memory: an in vivo study in humans.  Brain. 1995;118401- 416Google ScholarCrossref
62.
Squire  L Memory and hippocampus: a synthesis from findings with rats, monkey, and humans.  Psychol Rev. 1992;99195- 231Google ScholarCrossref
63.
Masliah  ETerry  RDAlford  M Cortical and subcortical patterns of synaptophysin-like immunoreactivity in Alzheimer's disease.  Am J Pathol. 1991;138235- 246Google Scholar
64.
DeKosky  STScheff  SW Synapse loss in frontal cortex biopsies in Alzheimer's disease: correlation with cognitive severity.  Ann Neurol. 1990;27457- 464Google ScholarCrossref
65.
Gomez-Isla  TPrice  JLMcKeel  DWMorris  JCGrowdon  JHHyman  BT Profound loss in layer II entorhinal cortex neurons occurs in very mild Alzheimer's disease.  J Neurosci. 1996;164491- 4500Google Scholar
×