Background
Parkinson disease (PD) is commonly characterized by cognitive deterioration, but it is still unclear whether PD is associated with semantic impairments.
Objective
To evaluate semantic knowledge of concepts in patients with idiopathic PD, addressing concrete and abstract concepts, conceptual attributes, and conceptual relations.
Methods
Twelve patients with preserved cognitive status, 12 patients with mildly deteriorated cognitive status, and 12 control subjects were studied. The cognitive status of patients and controls was determined using detailed cognitive testing. Patients were participants in a university-based movement disorder program, and their PD diagnoses were clinically confirmed during long-term follow-up. The 2 patient groups were similar in age, level of education, disease duration, and parkinsonian disability. Patients were required to produce verbal descriptions of concrete and abstract concepts, to give ratings of the importance of concept attributes, and to assess and construct conceptual hierarchies. The description tasks included guiding questions, which were used if the spontaneous productions of the patients lacked any essentials expected in the answers.
Results
Patients with mild cognitive deterioration performed less well than the other groups in defining concrete and abstract concepts (P<.001 for both). External guidance did not help them markedly improve their performance. They also had difficulties in tasks calling for knowledge of the importance of given attributes to the concepts and in tasks demanding evaluation of hierarchical semantic relations between concepts (P<.001 for both).
Conclusion
Semantic disruption is implied in idiopathic PD in association with incipient cognitive impairment.
SEMANTIC knowledge consists of conceptual meanings, such as knowledge of conceptual attributes and relations between concept domains.1,2 In network models,3-5 concepts are represented in distributed activation patterns, which are primarily linked to structural and perceptual or functional properties of an object. Semantic knowledge has been studied using tasks of word finding (eg, naming and verbal fluency) and concept meaning (eg, vocabulary, attribute ranking, and category knowledge) or tasks of semantic context effects (eg, semantic priming).6 Impairments of conceptual representations have mostly been tested using verbal or pictorial tasks, but concrete rather than abstract concepts have been addressed.
Semantic knowledge is disrupted in Alzheimer disease (AD), a brain disease typically leading to cognitive deterioration and dementia,7-9 but, to our knowledge, there is no systematic study on semantic knowledge in Parkinson disease (PD). Primarily related to nigrostriatal dopamine deficiency, PD is often characterized by slowly progressing cognitive deterioration, which can be relatively mild or can reach the severity of dementia.10,11 There is some evidence suggesting that semantic deficits might also be associated with cognitive deterioration in PD. Parkinsonian patients have demonstrated impaired performance on a variety of tasks apparently calling for semantic processing: concept formation,12,13 verbal fluency,14-17 syntax or sentence comprehension,18-20 written sentences,21 naming,22,23 or Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale24 similarities.11 These findings might reflect incipient semantic impairment in PD.
The purpose of the present study was to explore whether parkinsonian patients exhibit semantic deficits. The tasks we used focused on conscious understanding of the meanings of concepts and their relations. In the description tasks of concrete and abstract concepts, spontaneous and guided productions were scored separately. In the tasks of semantic relations, recognition and more active construction were studied. We compared 2 groups of parkinsonian patients—those with preserved cognitive performance and those with early, mild stages of cognitive deterioration—and control subjects.
We studied 24 patients with a long history of idiopathic PD and 12 controls. The patients were members of a regular medical follow-up study in the Department of Neurology, University of Turku, Turku, Finland. Inclusion criteria for patients were idiopathic PD, a positive response to levodopa therapy, age 50 to 79 years, normal visual acuity, and cognitive status suitable for either the "cognitively preserved" or the "cognitively mildly deteriorated" group (see the "Screening Tests" subsection). Patients with dementia,25 serious systemic or psychiatric disorders, or neurologic disorders other than idiopathic PD were excluded. On the Mini-Mental State Examination,26 which was not used as a criterion test, 3 deteriorated patients scored less than 24 points. At the time of the study, patients underwent a computed tomographic scan to rule out structural changes other than mild cortical or ventricular atrophy. Cerebral atrophy was not used as a criterion or predictor of performance, and only 6 patients (5 preserved and 1 deteriorated) had normal computed tomographic findings. There were 6 men and 6 women in each patient group. The 2 patient groups were matched on parkinsonian disability,27 disease duration, and antiparkinsonian treatments. On the Hoehn and Yahr scale,28 the patients scored on grades II or III (mean, 2.3 for each group). Patients were treated with levodopa, dopamine agonist, and selegiline hydrochloride in various combinations; 4 patients in the preserved group and 2 in the deteriorated group were also taking anticholinergic agents. The doses and combinations of treatments were individually optimally adjusted. The control group (7 men and 5 women) did not have neurologic or psychiatric diseases. The 3 groups were matched on age, level of education, and Beck Depression Inventory score29 (Table 1). All subjects gave informed consent to participate in this study, which was approved by the Joint Ethics Committee of Turku University and Turku University Hospital.
Allocation of patients into 2 groups was based on a neuropsychologic screening battery30 comprising 8 criterion tests measuring verbal, visuomotor, and memory performances: the similarities (abstract reasoning), Digit Span (short-term memory and attention), Digit Symbol (visuomotor speed), and Block Design (visuoconstructive skill) subtests of the Finnish version of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale24; the Benton Visual Retention Test31; immediate recall of 30 paired word associates32; and immediate recall and naming time of 20 objects.33 If the patient scored below −1.5 SDs on any screening test compared with our earlier reference values,34 he/she received 1 deterioration point; if below −2.0 SDs, 2 points; and if below −3.0 SDs, 3 points. Thus, the maximum deterioration score was 24 points.30 Patients were allocated to the cognitively preserved or the mildly deteriorated groups according to the cutoff value of 4 deterioration points, which was the highest score of the present controls. In addition, the delayed recall of 20 objects and 30 paired word associates was asked about after 1 hour.
The cognitively mildly deteriorated group performed more poorly than the cognitively preserved and control groups on all tests except naming time, immediate recall, and delayed recall of 20 objects and Digit Span (Table 2).
Post hoc comparison with ad patients
The cognitive status of the mildly deteriorated parkinsonian group was compared with the results of 14 patients with AD9 (5 men and 9 women; mean [SD] age, 69.1 [5.3] years; P = .90; mean [SD] education, 8.6 [4.1] years; P = .12; mean [SD] Mini-Mental State Examination score, 21.9 [3.1]; P = .02; and mean [SD] deterioration score, 12.5 [3.9]; P = .005). Patients with PD and mild cognitive deterioration performed better than those with AD on episodic memory tests: immediate (P = .003) and delayed (P = .002) recall of 20 objects, immediate (P = .007) and delayed (P = .003) recall of 30 paired word associates, and the Benton Visual Retention Test (P = .009). On the 4 Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale subtests, the performances of the 2 groups did not differ. Thus, the cognitive impairment profiles were different in the PD and AD groups.
All the tasks of semantic knowledge, their selection criteria, and their validity have been presented in detail earlier.9,35 Semantic knowledge was investigated using verbal tasks because it was considered important to study not only concepts referring to concretely observable objects but also more abstract concepts referring to events, relations, and abstract objects. Pilot studies9 were conducted to select concepts familiar to all. Performance in these tasks correlated with performance in more traditional semantic memory tasks such as confrontation naming, category fluency, and reasoning.35 The responses were first elicited through active, spontaneous production. If the subject failed, then guiding questions were used and, finally, forced choices were applied. Thus, the expression of semantic knowledge could be achieved even through passive recognition. This was done to ensure that any semantic knowledge of concepts that subjects still possessed would be revealed in the test situation.
Description of concrete concepts
We defined concrete concepts as concepts that refer to tangible, observable, picturable, and imaginable objects. Each concrete concept belonged to a single, obvious superordinate category (eg, harp → musical instrument). In addition, each object had at least 2 obvious structural attributes (eg, harp → big, has a number of strings) and 1 obvious functional attribute (eg, harp → is played by plucking). We expected subjects to produce the superordinate category of each object to which the concept referred (a total of 15), altogether 44 structural features of the objects to which the concepts referred, and altogether 19 functional features of the objects to which the concepts referred. Thus, the complete semantic knowledge of the concrete concepts consisted of 15 + 44 + 19 = 78 separately scored items.
The subject was encouraged to express freely as much knowledge of the concept as possible in a spontaneous manner, without any further cues or guidance from the experimenter. For each correct feature mentioned the subject obtained 3 points. Thus, the maximum score was 3 × 78 = 234 points. If the subject did not produce some of the expected features of the concept, the experimenter cued the production of the correct answers by asking specific questions about the superordinate category, the structural features, or the functional features of the concept. Each correct answer produced after guiding scored 2 points. If the subject still did not produce some of the expected features, the experimenter presented the subject with 2 alternative answers from which he/she had to choose the one he/she considered to be correct. Each correct choice scored 1 point.
Definition of abstract concepts
Subjects were asked to produce verbal definitions of the given concepts. Abstract concepts were defined as concepts that refer to rather intangible, not readily observable or picturable entities, such as events, relations, and abstract (nonmaterial) objects (such as crime, news, and bachelor). If the spontaneous definition given by the subject lacked some of the essentials of the definition, at first general then more specific forced-choice questions were presented. Correct spontaneous definitions were scored as 3 points, right answers to general questions as 2 points, and correct answers to specific questions as 1 point. The maximum score in this task was 45 points.
In this task, understanding the importance of given attributes to the meaning of concepts was required. Subjects were given a list of 15 concepts with 4 attributes each. The attributes appeared in print in random order below the sentences. The task was presented to the subject as a sentence completion task: he/she was requested to complete the sentences with the given attributes in such a manner that the sentences made sense to the subject. One attribute was always an essential property of the concept in question (eg, an airplane always includes wings). Another attribute was a typical but not absolutely essential feature of the concept in question (an airplane often includes a motor). A third attribute was one that is atypical but nevertheless feasible for the concept to possess, at least sometimes (an airplane seldom includes a hijacker). The fourth attribute named a feature that cannot consistently be applied to the concept in question (an airplane never includes a runway). Each sentence correctly completed scored 1 point; thus, the maximum score was 60 points.
The purpose of these tasks was to evaluate the understanding of the hierarchical relations within conceptual domains. All 8 tasks consisted of a hierarchical, spatial configuration of 10 cards. A superordinate concept was at the top, followed by 3 subcategories, each with 2 instances at the bottom (Figure 1).
The first 4 tasks were error recognition tasks in which the cards were placed in hierarchical order but 5 or 6 cards were misplaced in such a manner that the hierarchy as a whole did not make sense. The subject had to identify and remove the misplaced cards. Correct recognitions were recorded (maximum, 22 points). The remaining 4 hierarchies were construction tasks. Subjects had to assemble a hierarchy from 10 written cards. A line configuration showing the empty conceptual scheme was provided on paper for each hierarchy. The maximum score for the recognition and construction tasks was 22 + 40 = 62 points.
Study groups were compared using analysis of variance and Duncan grouping (P<.05) with parametric variables and Kruskal-Wallis with nonparametric variables. In post hoc comparisons, the t test was used.
The results on all semantic tasks are presented in Table 3. For the description of concrete concepts, the reliability between the scores of 2 independent raters was as high as 93.4%. The final scores were agreed on after discussion. Parkinsonian patients with mild cognitive deterioration performed less well than the other groups in producing superordinate categories and structural and functional attributes. They needed significantly more guiding questions and still did not make the correct choices on the forced-choice tasks.
For the definition of abstract concepts, the reliability between 2 independent raters was 76.7%, and the final scores were based on discussion. The mildly deteriorated group performed more poorly than the other groups. Similarly, the deteriorated group needed more questions and made incorrect choices between the given alternatives more often than the others.
The attribute ranking task was easy for the controls and the preserved group: their total scores of the maximum correct score were 91% and 89%, respectively; however, the mildly deteriorated group achieved only 66% of the maximum score. Total raw scores were lower in mildly deteriorated patients compared with the others.
On the tasks of conceptual hierarchies, the controls and the preserved group obtained 97% of the maximum score, whereas the mean of the mildly deteriorated group remained as low as 61%. Even recognition of misplaced concepts embedded in the concept configurations was difficult for mildly deteriorated patients, although they found the tasks interesting and were motivated to try their best. The total scores of only 2 patients with mild cognitive deterioration were within the range of the other groups. One patient with mild cognitive deterioration was unable to recognize or construct any of the hierarchy items; this patient was not included in the analyses of these tasks.
Results of the present study show that patients with PD and mild cognitive deterioration were clearly impaired in the tasks of defining concrete and abstract concepts and in understanding conceptual attributes and conceptual hierarchies. Even incipient cognitive deterioration was characterized by semantic memory disturbances, which could not be restored by guiding questions. The results provide new evidence of impaired semantic knowledge in PD.
In previous studies21,36-38 of semantic processes in PD, the study groups have been heterogeneous concerning the cognitive status of patients because often only the Mini-Mental State Examination was used for screening. We compared 2 carefully matched PD groups that had similar motor disability but exemplified 2 distinct levels of cognitive performance. One group exhibited preserved verbal, visuomotor, and memory functioning, whereas the other group showed early signs of cognitive deterioration but not dementia. The deterioration was obvious in some but not all criterion tasks, thus showing selectivity of cognitive impairment. In particular, abstract reasoning and aspects of episodic memory as well as visuomotor performances were impaired. Most subjects performed within the "normal" range on the Mini-Mental State Examination.
Another source of previous inconsistent or scanty findings of semantic impairment in PD might be due to the heterogeneity of the methods used. We used new methods and controlled the impact of other than semantic deficits as much as possible. Our tasks were applied without time limits to minimize the effect of cognitive slowing. Subjects did not need to produce specific, predetermined words or sentences to express their semantic knowledge. Circumlocutions and nonverbal expressions such as gestures were accepted where appropriate (eg, in showing the form or the size of an object) to avoid the confounding effect of linguistic problems as much as possible. Subjects were always first familiarized with the idea of the tasks by the administration of illustrative examples. In concrete and abstract tasks, mildly deteriorated patients produced significantly poorer definitions than the other groups. They had to resort to the guiding questions and forced-choice tasks, and they selected the incorrect alternative more often than the others. This finding conflicts with previous suggestions that patients with PD can improve their memory performance under cuing conditions, especially when cues have been explicitly presented.36-38 Furthermore, patients with mild cognitive deterioration had difficulty in the ranking tasks of conceptual attributes. Similarly, error recognition and more active construction of conceptual hierarchies caused problems for these patients. The findings hint at obvious conceptual-semantic impairments.
Impaired semantic knowledge has been found to characterize AD,7-9 and there is evidence39 that at least in some patients with PD diagnosed as having dementia, coexisting AD has been shown at autopsy. We minimized the possible comorbidity of AD by including only patients whose PD diagnoses were clinically confirmed during long-term dopaminergic treatments and who were not demented. Indeed, the cognitively deteriorated PD group showed a markedly different overall impairment profile compared with patients with AD studied earlier.9 They exhibited verbal, visuomotor, and semantic deficits similarly to the patients with PD. In contrast, patients with AD were clearly more impaired in episodic memory despite their slight advantage in education. The PD group performed better than the AD group in immediate and especially in delayed memory, which is consistent with earlier results in PD.40 Thus, it is not likely that concomitant AD explains the semantic disruption found in patients with PD and mild cognitive deterioration.
It is well known that besides lesions in the nigrostriatal dopamine neurons, other subcortical structures as well as anterior cortical regions might be affected and associated with cognitive dysfunction in PD.41,42 Our 2 patient groups had similar parkinsonian disability and other disease-related variables, and only their cognitive status was different. Hence, nigrostriatal damage primarily underlying motor symptoms in PD cannot on its own contribute to conceptual deficits in cognitively deteriorated patients, a result that agrees with previous findings.43,44 Our results do not conflict with the view that subtle cognitive deficits, implying disturbances in semantic prosessing in PD, might reflect frontal lobe dysfunction, which in turn is known to be associated with problem-solving deficits.13,41,42 However, if defective executive functions and, hence, an inability to use self-directed search strategies could explain our results of impaired semantic knowledge, then external guidance should have helped the patients. This did not happen, however. Therefore, frontal lobe abnormalities might not be a sufficient explanation for the impairment of semantic knowledge in PD.
Some ideas have been presented on the question of what the system that forms the neural basis of concepts is believed to be like. A model of semantic memory proposes a distributed network in which concepts are represented across a wide range of attributes in different patterns of activation.3,5 Consistent with this model, it has been argued that the basis for a concept consists of a collection of simultaneously activated sensory and motor representations that have a high probability of being triggered by the same (verbal or nonverbal) stimulus. It is proposed4,43 that the neural representations for such conceptual-semantic networks reside in the primary sensory areas of the cortex and in higher-order cortical areas. Therefore, impairment of semantic knowledge in our cognitively deteriorated group can be interpreted as showing that a verbal stimulus results in deficient activation of semantic representations. Because the performances remained impaired even after cuing or forced choice, it seems that the strength of the activation is probably not the only reason for the failure to access relevant semantic knowledge from the memory stores. To summarize, our results suggest that semantic networks are disrupted, ie, patients have a storage deficit of semantic knowledge. Nevertheless, this disruption might be associated with even mild cognitive deterioration and progressive lesions not only in PD but also in other brain diseases.6,9,35,45 Functional imaging of the brain during semantic tasks is necessary to evaluate the pathology underlying the semantic processing deficit in PD.
Accepted for publication January 24, 2000.
This study was supported by projects 36106 and 45704 from the Academy of Finland and the Jenny and Antti Wihuri Fund.
We thank Anne Kaljonen, MSc, and Kimmo Hypén, MA, for statistical assistance.
Reprints: Raija Portin, PhL, Department of Neurology, University of Turku, FIN-20520 Turku, Finland (e-mail: raija.portin@tyks.fi).
1.Collins
AMLoftus
EF A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing.
Psychol Rev. 1975;82407- 428
Google ScholarCrossref 2.Tulving
E Multiple memory systems and consciousness.
Hum Neurobiol. 1987;667- 80
Google Scholar 3.Allport
DA Distributed memory, modular subsystems and dysphasia. Newman
SKEpstein
Reds.
Current Perspectives in Dysphasia. Edinburgh, Scotland Churchill Livingstone1985;207- 244
Google Scholar 5.Shallice
T From Neuropsychology to Mental Structure. Cambridge, England Cambridge University Press1982;
7.Chan
ASButters
NPaulsen
JSSalmon
DPSwenson
MRMaloney
L An assessment of the semantic network in patients with Alzheimer's disease.
J Cogn Neurosci. 1993;5254- 261
Google ScholarCrossref 8.Chertkow
HBub
DBergman
HBruemmer
AMerling
ARothfleisch
J Increased semantic priming in patients with dementia of Alzheimer's type.
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1994;16608- 622
Google ScholarCrossref 9.Laatu
SPortin
RRevonsuo
ATuisku
SRinne
J Knowledge of concept meanings in Alzheimer's disease.
Cortex. 1997;3327- 45
Google ScholarCrossref 10.Brown
RGMarsden
CD "Subcortical dementia": the neuropsychological evidence.
Neuroscience. 1988;25363- 387
Google ScholarCrossref 11.Portin
RRaininko
RRinne
UK Neuropsychological disturbances and cerebral atrophy determined by computerized tomography in parkinsonian patients with long-term levodopa treatment.
Adv Neurol. 1984;40219- 227
Google Scholar 12.Bowen
FPKamienny
RSBurns
MMYahr
MD Parkinsonism: effects of levodopa treatment on concept formation.
Neurology. 1975;25701- 704
Google ScholarCrossref 13.Lees
AJSmith
E Cognitive deficits in the early stages of Parkinson's disease.
Brain. 1983;106257- 270
Google ScholarCrossref 14.Bayles
KATrosset
MWTomoeda
CK Generative naming in Parkinson's disease patients.
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1993;15547- 562
Google ScholarCrossref 15.Beatty
WWMonson
NGoodkin
DE Access to semantic memory in Parkinson's disease and multiple sclerosis.
J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol. 1989;2153- 162
Google ScholarCrossref 16.Raskin
SASliwinski
MBorod
JC Clustering strategies on tasks of verbal fluency in Parkinson's disease.
Neuropsychologia. 1992;3095- 99
Google ScholarCrossref 17.Tröster
AIFields
JATesta
JA
et al. Cortical and subcortical influences on clustering and switching in the performance of verbal fluency tasks.
Neuropsychologia. 1998;36295- 304
Google ScholarCrossref 18.Grossman
MCarvell
SGollomp
S
et al. Cognitive and physiological substrates of impaired sentence processing in Parkinson's disease.
J Cogn Neurosci. 1993;5480- 498
Google ScholarCrossref 19.Lieberman
PFriedman
JFeldman
LS Syntax comprehension deficits in Parkinson's disease.
J Nerv Ment Dis. 1990;178360- 365
Google ScholarCrossref 20.Natsopoulos
DMentenopoulos
GBostantzopoulou
SKatsarou
ZGrouios
GLogothetis
J Understanding of relational time terms before and after in parkinsonian patients.
Brain Lang. 1991;40444- 458
Google ScholarCrossref 21.Small
JALyons
KKemper
S Grammatical abilities in Parkinson's disease: evidence from written sentences.
Neuropsychologia. 1997;351571- 1576
Google ScholarCrossref 22.Beatty
WWMonson
N Lexical processing in Parkinson's disease and multiple sclerosis.
J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol. 1989;2145- 152
Google ScholarCrossref 23.Matison
RMayeux
RRosen
JFahn
S "Tip-of-the-tongue" phenomenon in Parkinson's disease.
Neurology. 1982;32567- 570
Google ScholarCrossref 24.Wechsler
D Manual for the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. New York, NY The Psychological Corporation1955;
25.American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Third Edition. Washington, DC American Psychiatric Association1980;
26.Folstein
MFFolstein
SEMcHugh
PR "Mini-mental state": a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician.
J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12189- 198
Google ScholarCrossref 27.Duvoisin
RC The evaluation of extrapyramidal disease. De Ajuriaguerra
JGauthier
Geds.
Monoamines, Noyaux Gris Centreux et Syndrome de Parkinson. Geneva, Switzerland Cerr et Cie SA1971;313- 325
Google Scholar 29.Beck
ATWard
CHMendelson
MMock
JErbaugh
JK An inventory for measuring depression.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1961;4561- 571
Google ScholarCrossref 30.Revonsuo
APortin
RKoivikko
LRinne
JORinne
UK Slowing of information processing in Parkinson's disease.
Brain Cogn. 1993;2187- 110
Google ScholarCrossref 31.Benton
AL The Revised Visual Retention Test. New York, NY The Psychological Corporation1963;
32.Portin
RRinne
UK The effect of deprenyl (selegiline) on cognition and emotion in parkinsonian patients undergoing long-term levodopa treatment.
Acta Neurol Scand. 1983;95135- 144
Google ScholarCrossref 33.Portin
RSaarijärvi
SJoukamaa
MSalokangas
RKR Education, gender and cognitive performance in a 62-year-old normal population: results from the TURVA project.
Psychol Med. 1995;251295- 1298
Google ScholarCrossref 34.Portin
RRinne
UK Neuropsychological responses of parkinsonian patients to long-term levodopa treatment. Rinne
UKKlingler
MStamm
Geds.
Parkinson's Disease Current Progress, Problems and Management. New York, NY Elsevier Science Inc1980;271- 304
Google Scholar 35.Laatu
SHämäläinen
PRevonsuo
APortin
RRuutiainen
J Semantic memory deficit in multiple sclerosis: impaired understanding of conceptual meanings.
J Neurol Sci. 1999;162152- 161
Google ScholarCrossref 36.Faglioni
PBotti
CScarpa
MFerrari
VSaetti
MC Learning and forgetting processes in Parkinson's disease: a model-based approach to disentangling storage, retention and retrieval contributions.
Neuropsychologia. 1997;35767- 779
Google ScholarCrossref 37.Van Spaendonck
KPMBerger
HJCHorstink
MWIMBorm
GFCools
AR Memory performance under varying cueing conditions in patients with Parkinson's disease.
Neuropsychologia. 1996;341159- 1164
Google ScholarCrossref 38.Buytenhuijs
ELBerger
HJCVan Spaendonck
KPMHorstink
WIMBorm
GFCools
AR Memory and learning strategies in patients with Parkinson's disease.
Neuropsychologia. 1994;32335- 342
Google ScholarCrossref 40.Sullivan
EVSagar
HJ Double dissociation of short-term memory and long-term memory for nonverbal material in Parkinson's disease and global amnesia.
Brain. 1991;114893- 906
Google ScholarCrossref 41.Owen
AMJames
PLeigh
PN
et al. Fronto-striatal cognitive deficits at different stages of Parkinson's disease.
Brain. 1992;1151727- 1751
Google ScholarCrossref 42.Taylor
AESaint-Cyr
JALang
AE Frontal-lobe dysfunction in Parkinson's disease.
Brain. 1986;109845- 883
Google ScholarCrossref 43.Damasio
AR Time-locked multiregional retroactivation: a systems-level proposal for the neural substrates of recall and recognition.
Cognition. 1989;3325- 62
Google ScholarCrossref 44.Cooper
JASagar
HJDoherty
SMJordan
NTidswell
PSullivan
EV Different effects of dopaminergic and anticholinergic therapies on cognitive and motor function in Parkinson's disease.
Brain. 1992;1151701- 1725
Google ScholarCrossref