[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
A, Total deficit scores at day 1: Higher total deficit scores represent a greater decline in performance. The cutoff for significant cognitive injury (5.46 points) at day 1 is 2 SDs above the mean of the control group (2.16 ± 1.65) and is depicted by the horizontal line. Based on this criterion, 22 patients (28%) have significant cognitive decline following CEA, compared with a control group of patients undergoing spine surgery. In the control group, lumbar spine surgeries included 21 (86%) undergoing laminectomy, 3 (12%) undergoing discectomy, and 1 (2%) undergoing microdiscectomy. B, Total deficit scores at day 30: The cutoff for significant cognitive injury (4.65 points) is 2 SDs above the mean of the control group (1.81 ± 1.42) and is depicted by the horizontal line. Based on this criterion, 11 (23%) of 48 patients have significant cognitive decline 1 month after CEA, compared with a control group of patients undergoing spine surgery.

A, Total deficit scores at day 1: Higher total deficit scores represent a greater decline in performance. The cutoff for significant cognitive injury (5.46 points) at day 1 is 2 SDs above the mean of the control group (2.16 ± 1.65) and is depicted by the horizontal line. Based on this criterion, 22 patients (28%) have significant cognitive decline following CEA, compared with a control group of patients undergoing spine surgery. In the control group, lumbar spine surgeries included 21 (86%) undergoing laminectomy, 3 (12%) undergoing discectomy, and 1 (2%) undergoing microdiscectomy. B, Total deficit scores at day 30: The cutoff for significant cognitive injury (4.65 points) is 2 SDs above the mean of the control group (1.81 ± 1.42) and is depicted by the horizontal line. Based on this criterion, 11 (23%) of 48 patients have significant cognitive decline 1 month after CEA, compared with a control group of patients undergoing spine surgery.

Table 1. Demographics and Intraoperative Variables in Carotid Endarterectomy (CEA) and Spine Groups*
Demographics and Intraoperative Variables in Carotid Endarterectomy (CEA) and Spine Groups*
Table 2. Neuropsychological Test Scores*
Neuropsychological Test Scores*
1.
North American Symptomatic Carotid Endarterectomy Trial Collaborators Beneficial effect of carotid endarterectomy in symptomatic patients with high-grade carotid stenosis.  N Engl J Med.1991;325:445-453.Google Scholar
2.
Hobson II  RWWeiss  DGFields  WS  et al Efficacy of carotid endarterectomy for asymptomatic carotid stenosis.  N Engl J Med.1993;328:221-227.Google Scholar
3.
Executive Committee for the Asymptomatic Carotid Atherosclerosis Study Endarterectomy for asymptomatic carotid artery stenosis.  JAMA.1995;273:1421-1428.Google Scholar
4.
European Carotid Surgery Trialists' Collaborative Group MRC European Carotid Surgery Trial: interim results for symptomatic patients with severe (70-99%) or with mild (0-29%) carotid stenosis.  Lancet.1991;337:1235-1243.Google Scholar
5.
Kelly  MPGarron  DCJavid  H Carotid artery disease, carotid endarterectomy, and behavior.  Arch Neurol.1980;37:743-748.Google Scholar
6.
De Leo  DSerraiotto  LPellegrini  CMagni  GFranceschi  LDeriu  GP Outcome from carotid endarterectomy: neuropsychological performances, depressive symptoms and quality of life: 8-month follow-up.  Int J Psychiatry Med.1987;17:317-325.Google Scholar
7.
Bornstein  RABenoit  BGTrites  RL Neuropsychological changes following carotid endarterectomy.  Can J Neurol Sci.1981;8:127-132.Google Scholar
8.
Bennion  RSOwens  MLWilson  SE The effect of unilateral carotid endarterectomy on neuropsychological test performance in 53 patients.  J Thorac Cardiovasc Surg.1985;26:21-26.Google Scholar
9.
Hemmingsen  RMejsholm  BVorstrup  SLester  JEngell  HCBoysen  G Carotid surgery, cognitive function, and cerebral blood flow in patients with transient ischemic attacks.  Ann Neurol.1986;20:13-19.Google Scholar
10.
Antonelli Incalzi  RGemma  ALandi  F  et al Neuropsychologic effects of carotid endarterectomy.  J Clin Exp Neuropsychol.1997;19:785-794.Google Scholar
11.
Casey  JEFerguson  GGKimura  DHachinski  VC Neuropsychological improvement versus practice effect following unilateral carotid endarterectomy in patients without stroke.  J Clin Exp Neuropsychol.1989;11:461-470.Google Scholar
12.
Parker  JCGranberg  BWNichols  WKJones  JGHewett  JE Mental status outcomes following carotid endarterectomy: a six-month analysis.  J Clin Neuropsychol.1983;5:345-353.Google Scholar
13.
Vanninen  EVanninen  RAikia  M  et al Frequency of carotid endarterectomy–related subclinical cerebral complications.  Cerebrovasc Dis.1996;6:272-280.Google Scholar
14.
Heyer  EAdams  DTodd  G  et al Neuropsychometric changes in patients after carotid endarterectomy.  Stroke.1998;29:1110-1115.Google Scholar
15.
Lind  CWimmer  AMagometschnigg  H  et al Effects of carotid endarterectomy on various neuropsychologic parameters: a neuropsychologic longitudinal study [in German].  Langenbecks Arch Chir.1993;378:345-352.Google Scholar
16.
Brinkman  SDBraun  PGanji  SMorrell  RMJacobs  LA Neuropsychological performance one week after carotid endarterectomy reflects intra-operative ischemia.  Stroke.1984;15:497-503.Google Scholar
17.
Cushman  LBrinkman  SDGanji  SJacobs  LA Neuropsychological impairment after carotid endarterectomy correlates with intraoperative ischemia.  Cortex.1984;20:403-412.Google Scholar
18.
Gaunt  MEMartin  PJSmith  JL  et al Clinical relevance of intraoperative embolization detected by transcranial Doppler ultrasonography during carotid endarterectomy: a prospective study of 100 patients.  Br J Surg.1994;81:1435-1439.Google Scholar
19.
Townes  BDDikmen  SSBledsoe  SWHornbein  TFMartin  DCJanesheski  JA Neuropsychological changes in a young, healthy population after controlled hypotensive anesthesia.  Anesth Analg.1986;65:955-959.Google Scholar
20.
Jones  MJ The influence of anesthetic methods on mental function.  Acta Chir Scand Suppl.1989;550:169-175.Google Scholar
21.
Moller  JTCluitmans  PRasmussen  LS  et alfor the International Study of Post-Operative Cognitive Dysfunction investigators Long-term postoperative cognitive dysfunction in the elderly.  Lancet.1998;351:857-861.Google Scholar
22.
Heyer  EJSharma  RWinfree  CJ  et al Severe pain confounds neuropsychological test performance.  J Clin Exp Neuropsychol.2000;22:633-639.Google Scholar
23.
Lezak  MD Neuropsychological Assessment. 3rd ed. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 1995.
24.
Meyers  JMeyers  K Rey Complex Figure Test and Recognition Trial Professional Manual.  Odessa, Fla: Psychological Assessment Resources Inc; 1995.
25.
Downie  WWLeatham  PARhind  VMWright  VBranco  JAAnderson  JA Studies with pain rating scales.  Ann Rheum Dis.1978;37:378-381.Google Scholar
26.
Thal  GDSzabo  MDLopez-Bresnahan  MCrosby  G Exacerbation or unmasking of focal neurologic deficits by sedatives.  Anesthesiology.1996;85:21-25.Google Scholar
27.
Lunn  SCrawley  FHarrison  MJBrown  MMNewman  SP Impact of carotid endarterectomy upon cognitive functioning: a systematic review of the literature.  Cerebrovasc Dis.1999;9:74-81.Google Scholar
28.
Rasmussen  LSChristiansen  MJohnsen  JGronholdt  MLMoller  JT Subtle brain damage cannot be detected by measuring neuron-specific enolase and S-100β protein after carotid endarterectomy.  J Cardiothorac Vasc Anesth.2000;14:166-170.Google Scholar
29.
Sirkka  ASalenius  JPPortin  RNummenmaa  T Quality of life and cognitive performance after carotid endarterectomy during long-term follow-up.  Acta Neurol Scand.1992;85:58-62.Google Scholar
30.
Williams  MMcGee  TF Psychological study of carotid occlusion and endarterectomy.  Arch Neurol.1964;10:293-297.Google Scholar
31.
Owens  MPressman  MEdwards  AE  et al The effect of small infarcts and carotid endarterectomy on postoperative psychologic test performance.  J Surg Res.1980;28:209-216.Google Scholar
Original Contribution
February 2002

A Controlled Prospective Study of Neuropsychological Dysfunction Following Carotid Endarterectomy

Author Affiliations

From the Departments of Anesthesiology (Dr Heyer and Ms Rampersad), Neurological Surgery (Ms Sharma and Drs Winfree, Solomon, McCormick, McMurtry, Quest, Lazar, and Connolly, and Mr Mack), Neurology (Drs Heyer, Stern, and Lazar), the Gertrude H. Sergievsky Center (Dr Stern), Department of Surgery, St Luke's-Roosevelt Hospital Center (Dr Todd), and the Herbert and Florence Irving Center for Clinical Research (Dr Connolly), Columbia University, New York, NY.

Arch Neurol. 2002;59(2):217-222. doi:10.1001/archneur.59.2.217
Abstract

Background  Although subtle cognitive injury as revealed by neuropsychological testing occurs in a substantial number of patients following carotid endarterectomy (CEA), there is controversy about whether this finding is a result of the surgery or the anesthesia.

Objectives  To examine the changes in neuropsychological test performance in patients following CEA vs a control group of patients older than 60 years following spine surgery, so as to determine whether neuropsychological dysfunction after CEA is a result of surgery or anesthesia.

Methods  Patients undergoing CEA (n = 80) and lumbar spine surgery (n = 25) were assessed with a battery of neuropsychological tests preoperatively and on postoperative days 1 and 30. The neuropsychological performance of patients in the control group was used to normalize performance for patients in the CEA group, by calculating z scores using the mean and SD of the change scores in the control group. Significant cognitive dysfunction was defined as performance that exceeded 2 SDs above the mean performance of patients in the control group.

Results  Postoperative days 1 and 30 total deficit scores were significantly worse in the CEA group compared with the controls. When individual test results were examined, the CEA group performed significantly worse than the controls on the Rey Complex Figure test and Halstead-Reitan Trails B on day 1, and on the Rey Complex Figure on day 30. Overall, cognitive dysfunction was seen in 22 patients (28%) in the CEA group on day 1 and in 11 (23%) of 48 patients on day 30.

Conclusions  Subtle cognitive decline following CEA occurs and persists for at least several weeks after surgery. This decline was absent in a control group.

CAROTID endarterectomy (CEA) is an effective means of preventing stroke in appropriately selected patients.1-4 Although the incidence of perioperative stroke is low, subtle cognitive changes as revealed by neuropsychological (NP) testing occur in a high percentage of patients following CEA. Several studies have demonstrated improvement,5-10 others show no change,11,12 and still others demonstrate a decline13-15 in postoperative NP performance. Although decline in NP performance may be related to intraoperative hemispheric cerebral ischemia,16,17 microemboli,18 or subclinical microinfarcts,13 some decline may be due to the effects of general anesthesia,19,20 particularly in older patients.21 Most previous studies have not controlled for the effects of perioperative variables, such as anesthesia, stress, and pain.

To better characterize the cognitive injury that occurs following CEA, we tested patients before and after surgery using a battery of NP tests to evaluate several cognitive domains to reveal subtle cognitive injury. Furthermore, we defined a group of patients undergoing spine surgery (age, >60 years) to serve as an appropriate control group to account for the effects of these perioperative variables.22 This prospective study explores the changes in NP test performance 1 day and 1 month following CEA.

Subjects and methods
Subjects

Eighty-three patients undergoing elective CEA and 25 patients (age, >60 years) undergoing lumbar spine surgery were recruited to participate in this institutional review board–approved study. We included all patients who were able to perform the NP evaluation in English, and excluded all patients with either a postoperative stroke or pain scores greater than 4 at their first follow-up examination. After written informed consent was obtained, patients were assessed with a battery of NP tests at 3 time points: before surgery (n = 108), 1 day after surgery (n = 105), and 30 days after surgery (n = 65). All examinations were performed at least 3 hours after any analgesic or sedative medication had been administered. Preoperative and postoperative magnetic resonance imaging and computed tomographic scanning were not performed. Although intraoperative transcranial Doppler ultrasonography was performed on some patients, it was not used to determine the need for shunting or to count the number of emboli coming from the surgical field.

Anesthesia

No patients were premedicated. All patients received general anesthesia, with routine hemodynamic and temperature monitoring recorded continually every minute. Sedation before induction consisted of fentanyl citrate and midazolam hydrochloride. Anesthetic agents for induction included 1 or more of the following: propofol, etomidate, or thiopental sodium, and succinylcholine chloride, cisatracurium besylate, or rocuronium bromide; for maintenance: isoflurane or sevoflurane with nitrous oxide in oxygen in a 2:1 ratio, and cisatracurium or rocuronium; and for emergence: neostigmine bromide and glycopyrrolate for reversal of neuromuscular blockade, and labetalol hydrochloride or esmolol hydrochloride for hemodynamic control. For patients undergoing CEA, a radial arterial catheter for measuring blood pressure continuously and an 8-channel encephalographic monitor (Neurotrac II; Moberg Medical, Inc, Ambler, Pa) were used. Significant encephalographic change on clamping the carotid artery was defined as a 50% or greater decrease in amplitude in the alpha or beta frequencies and a similar increase in the delta or theta frequencies, or complete loss of all cerebral electrical activity. Anesthesia for patients having spine surgery was essentially identical to that for patients having CEA, except for higher mean dosages offentanyl in patients undergoing spine surgery (Table 1).

Surgery

All laminectomies were performed by a member of the neurosurgical spine service (P.C.M., J.G.M., or D.O.Q.), and all CEAs were performed by members of either the neurovascular service (R.A.S., D.O.Q., or E.S.C.) or the vascular service (G.J.T.). The surgery for CEA consisted of positioning the patient supine with the head in an extended midline position. An incision was made along a skin crease from just below the angle of the mandible to near the midline through skin, subcutaneous tissue, and platysma. The common, internal, and external carotid arteries were exposed and controlled. All patients undergoing CEA received 5000 to 6000 U of heparin sodium bolus. No patients were shunted. In patients operated on by one of us (G.J.T.), a saphenous vein patch was used and protamine sulfate reversal was given selectively.

Patients undergoing spine surgery were placed in the prone position. Following a midline incision, dissection through the paraspinal muscles was accomplished unilaterally in patients undergoing discectomy and bilaterally in those undergoing laminectomy. The microscope was used for visualization of disk removal in 1 patient. Patients were returned to the supine position before extubation. No patients, in either the CEA or control groups, received blood transfusions.

All patients were extubated in the operating room and recovered for 1 to 3 hours in a postoperative care unit. After CEA, patients were transferred to the intensive care unit, where they stayed for 1 night. No patients having lumbar spine surgery required a stay in the intensive care unit. All patients remained in the hospital for 1 to 3 days for postoperative pain scoring and NP testing.

Np evaluation

Patients were assessed with a battery of NP tests. All examinations were administered by the same research assistant (R.S.), trained to administer and score these NP tests under the supervision of neuropsychologists (Y.S. and R.M.L.). Four raw scores were generated from the battery of 3 NP tests, which were chosen to represent a limited range of cognitive domains. Halstead-Reitan Trails parts A and B evaluated visual conceptual and visuomotor tracking by timing how long it took a subject to connect consecutively numbered circles with a single line (part A) and then connect the same number of consecutively numbered and lettered circles by alternating between the 2 sequences (part B). The Controlled Oral Word Association Test evaluated verbal fluency and provided information on the function of the dominant hemisphere ("left"). Patients were asked to generate as many words as they could beginning with 3 target letters (C, F, and L) in 1 minute. Their raw score was the sum of the words produced for all 3 letters. The copy portion of the Rey Complex Figure test was administered to assess perceptual and visuospatial organization and provided information on the function of the nondominant hemisphere ("right"). Patients were asked to copy the Rey Complex Figure to the best of their ability.23 A standardized scoring system24 was used to evaluate the presence of specific design features and the accuracy of their location. Although we would have preferred a more extensive battery, our population of older patients better tolerated this one because of its short administration time.

The degree of pain was assessed at the same time NP testing was performed, using an 11-point Numeric Pain Intensity Scale adapted from a previous study.25 Patients with pain scores greater than 4 were excluded at the first follow-up, because of the confounding effect of pain on NP test performance.22

Statistical measures

For each NP test, the change in performance was calculated by subtracting the baseline score from the postoperative score. A normative data set for these changes was derived from the control population for each NP test. The mean change and SD for the control population were then used to calculate a z score for each test: z score = (change score – mean change scorespine)/SD of change scorespine. To illustrate cognitive decline, a point system was used to transform negative z scores into points for each NP test. Points were assigned as follows: z scores ≥−0.5 = 0 points; <−0.5 to –1.0 = 1 point; <−1.0 to −1.5 = 2 points; <−1.5 to –2.0 = 3 points; <−2.0 to –2.5 = 4 points; <−2.5 to –3.0 = 5 points; and <−3.0 = 6 points. This test deficit score measured how far each patient's performance in the CEA group deviated from the mean performance of the control group. For each patient, the total number of test deficit scores was summed to produce the total deficit score. This total deficit score represents overall performance on the NP battery, while test deficit scores represent performance on individual NP tests. Group differences were compared by using the Satterthwaite modification to the unpaired t test because of differences in the variance of the measurement between groups. The Kolmogorov-Smirnov test was used to test whether the distribution of test and z scores between the control and CEA groups for each NP test was normal.

Results

Demographic and intraoperative variables are shown in Table 1 for all patients. Performance on all the NP tests was normally distributed at baseline for patients in the CEA group, including Trails A when 1 outlier was excluded. Because we expected patients with abnormalities of the central nervous system to perform worse than those without injury, it is not surprising that "symptomatic" CEA patients (those who had suffered a previous stroke or transient ischemic attack [TIA]) performed worse at baseline on all 3 NP examinations compared with the control subjects.26 In contrast, "asymptomatic" CEA patients performed significantly worse only on Trails A at baseline (P = .03). Therefore, we might also expect symptomatic patients to demonstrate a greater decline in NP performance after surgery than asymptomatic patients. However, a subgroup analysis of patients undergoing CEA showed no significant difference in NP outcome between symptomatic and asymptomatic patients. There was no significant relationship between educational level and NP outcome scores. Three patients in the CEA group had postoperative strokes and were excluded from the analysis. One patient had a postoperative myocardial infarction and was not excluded from the analysis. No significant difference in NP performance was seen between those who had a saphenous vein patch and those who did not.

Postoperative day 1 total deficit scores were significantly worse in the CEA group compared with the control patients (Table 2, P = .003). When individual test results were examined (Table 2), the CEA patients performed significantly worse than control patients on the Rey Complex Figure (P = .006) and Trails B (P = .01). There were no significant differences in performance between patients undergoing right and left CEAs on any of the NP tests. Postoperative day 30 total deficit scores (Table 2) were significantly worse in the CEA group (P = .03) compared with the control patients. When individual test results were examined at the 1-month follow-up (Table 2), the CEA group still performed significantly worse on the Rey Complex Figure (P = .01). Of the 37 CEA patients with significant decline on either the Rey Complex Figure or Trails B, only 16% (6 patients) demonstrated a significant decline on both tests, while 84% (31 patients) demonstrated a significant decline on one or the other (16 [43%] on Rey Complex Figure and 15 [41%] on Trails B).

To determine a cutoff for significant cognitive dysfunction following CEA, postoperative day 1 total deficit scores were examined in the control group (n = 25). Total deficit scores higher than 2 SDs (>5.46 points) above the mean of the control group (higher points indicated poorer performance) represented significant cognitive dysfunction (Figure 1, A). Of the 80 CEA patients, 22 (28%) had significant cognitive dysfunction on postoperative day 1. Only 1 control patient had a postoperative day 1 total deficit score higher than 2 SDs above the mean.

Total deficit scores higher than 4.65 points represented significant cognitive dysfunction at day 30 after surgery (Figure 1, B). Of the 48 CEA patients tested at day 30, 11 (23%) demonstrated significant cognitive decline, compared with the controls. Because 32 patients (40%) in the CEA group declined to complete the study at 1 month, we compared baseline performance scores between them and the 48 patients who returned. There was no significant difference in test performance on all 4 individual NP tests at baseline and at 1 day after surgery between these 2 subgroups of patients. The 2 subgroups differed with respect to 2 perioperative variables: the 48 patients who returned had a significantly higher incidence of previous stroke or TIA (P<.02) but a lower incidence of previous myocardial infarction (P<.004), compared with the patients who did not return. Six patients injured at day 30 were also injured at day 1 (P<.03, χ2 test). No control patients had postoperative day 30 total deficit scores higher than 2 SDs above the mean. Despite the loss of 8 control patients at the 1-month follow-up, there was no significant difference in their test performance on all 4 individual NP tests at baseline and 1 day after surgery, compared with the 17 who returned. The control patients who returned were significantly shorter in stature than the patients who did not return (P<.02).

Of the 22 patients in the CEA group with significant cognitive dysfunction 1 day after surgery, 6 (27%) remained injured 1 month later. Ten patients who were injured 1 day after surgery declined their 1-month follow-up examination. Of the 58 patients who did not demonstrate significant cognitive dysfunction 1 day after surgery, 5 (9%) demonstrated significant cognitive dysfunction at 1 month. Nineteen patients who were uninjured 1 day after surgery declined follow-up.

Patients in the CEA group with significant cognitive dysfunction were compared with patients in the group without dysfunction. These 2 subgroups did not significantly differ with respect to perioperative variables (age, sex, educational level, or handedness), medical history (stroke or TIA, CEA, diabetes mellitus, or hypertension), surgical variables (side of surgery, contralateral carotid stenosis, duration of carotid cross-clamping, or duration of surgery), or anesthetic variables (fentanyl or midazolam dosage or intraoperative temperature). However, patients with a history of myocardial infarction were more likely to develop cognitive dysfunction following CEA than those without a history (P<.01).

Comment

Using total deficit scores to measure NP performance, patients who underwent CEA sustained a significant decline on postoperative days 1 and 30, compared with a control group of patients having spine surgery. This decline was measured using a battery of 3 NP tests to evaluate language, attention, and perceptual and visuospatial organization. This persistent decline in performance was not seen in a previous uncontrolled study.14

Numerous investigations spanning several decades have explored whether CEA affects postoperative NP performance.27 One universal limitation of these studies was the absence of an appropriate control group to account for the effects of general anesthesia and surgery on NP performance.19,20 Some studies used nonoperative controls,6,7,12,28,29 others used operative controls,5,7,9,13 and still others used no control group at all.8,14,15,30,31

Our 2 groups were well matched in perioperative variables, medical history, anesthetic variables, and perioperative pain levels. The only difference was that patients undergoing spine surgery required higher doses of fentanyl during surgery and were in more pain after surgery. However, increased doses of fentanyl would bias the data against the control group, causing them to perform worse on the NP tests, and we excluded patients with pain scores higher than 4 from our analysis.22 Therefore, it is likely that factors specific to CEA, such as hemispheric cerebral ischemia or microemboli, may be responsible for the decline in NP performance.

When results of individual tests 1 day after surgery were examined, the CEA group patients performed significantly worse than did controls on the Rey Complex Figure and Trails B tests. A significant decline on both of these tests occurred in only 6 patients (16%). This finding may support the hypothesis that different mechanisms of ischemia, focal vs hemispheric, are responsible for the postoperative cognitive decline following CEA. At 30 days after surgery, the CEA group performed significantly worse than did controls on only the Rey Complex Figure test.

Besides using a control group to determine the effects of anesthesia and surgery, NP performance in a control group was used to account for changes in NP performance that were due to the "practice effect," which arises when patients are repeatedly tested. Using the mean and SD of the change scores in the control group, z scores were calculated to measure how individuals in the CEA group performed compared with patients in the control group. By using a surgical control group, we avoided using less rigorous statistical methods, such as defining deficit based on a percentage decline, used in a previous study.14 In addition, the severity of significant injury on individual NP test results can be quantified using test deficit scores, and overall injury can be quantified using total deficit scores.

For this study, significant cognitive injury was defined as a decline in performance that exceeded 2 SDs above the mean of the spine surgery cohort. Using this criterion, the NP performance of 24 (96%) of the control patients was within the normal (uninjured) range on postoperative day 1. All control patients performed within the normal range 1 month after surgery. Although most patients in the CEA group showed no change or improved NP performance after surgery, 22 (28%) demonstrated a significant decline in NP performance 1 day after surgery, and 11 (23%) of 48 were injured 1 month after surgery. There was also no difference in the incidence of cognitive injury with respect to the side of the CEA, using the Rey Complex Figure test as a nondominant hemisphere task and the oral word test as a dominant hemisphere task.

Although the thrust of this article was to highlight the incidence of patients who developed cognitive deficits, some patients undergoing CEA have experienced improved postoperative NP performance.14 This improvement may have occurred because of relief of flow failure after CEA. Future studies may investigate cerebral blood flow before and after surgery and attempt to correlate relief of flow failure with improved NP performance after surgery.

Although we were concerned about the reduced number of patients at the second follow-up, there were no obvious differences in NP test performance on either baseline or first follow-up examinations among the CEA and control groups. The patients in the CEA group who returned for the second follow-up were more likely to have had a previous stroke or TIA and were less likely to have had a myocardial infarction. Except for shorter stature, the control patients who returned for the second follow-up were no different from the control patients who returned for the first follow-up only.

The battery of NP tests used in this study consistently reveals subtle cognitive decline following CEA, which persists for at least several weeks after surgery. These changes were apparently unrelated to anesthetic or perioperative factors. They represent subtle neurological deficits that arose as a result of the surgery itself, unrelated to overt clinical stroke.

Accepted for publication September 17, 2001.

Author contributions: Study concept and design (Drs Heyer, Winfree, Solomon, Quest, Stern, Lazar, and Connolly); acquisition of data (Drs Heyer, Solomon, Todd, McCormick, and McMurtry and Mss Sharma and Rampersad); analysis and interpretation of data (Drs Heyer, Winfree, Quest, Stern, Lazar, and Connolly, Mr Mack, and Ms Rampersad); drafting of the manuscript (Drs Heyer, McMurtry, and Connolly, Mr Mack, and Mss Sharma and Rampersad); critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: (Drs Heyer, Winfree, Solomon, Todd, McCormick, Quest, Stern, Lazar, and Connolly and Ms Rampersad); statistical expertise (Dr Heyer, Mr Mack, and Ms Rampersad); obtained funding (Drs Heyer and Connolly); administrative, technical, and material support (Drs Heyer, Winfree, Solomon, Todd, McCormick, Lazar, and Connolly and Mss Sharma and Rampersad) study supervision (Drs Heyer, Solomon, Quest, Stern, Lazar, and Connolly and Ms Rampersad).

This study was supported in part by grants from the American Federation for Aging Research (Dr Connolly) and the Charles A. Dana Foundation (Dr Heyer), New York, NY, and by research grants KO8 NS2038 and RO1 NS40409 (Dr Connolly) and 1R01 AG17604-01A2 (Dr Heyer) from the National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, Md.

We are grateful for the excellent statistical support provided by Robert R. Sciacca, Eng ScD.

Corresponding author and reprints: Eric J. Heyer, MD, PhD, Department of Anesthesiology, Columbia University, 630 W 168th St, New York, NY 10032 (e-mail: ejh3@columbia.edu).

References
1.
North American Symptomatic Carotid Endarterectomy Trial Collaborators Beneficial effect of carotid endarterectomy in symptomatic patients with high-grade carotid stenosis.  N Engl J Med.1991;325:445-453.Google Scholar
2.
Hobson II  RWWeiss  DGFields  WS  et al Efficacy of carotid endarterectomy for asymptomatic carotid stenosis.  N Engl J Med.1993;328:221-227.Google Scholar
3.
Executive Committee for the Asymptomatic Carotid Atherosclerosis Study Endarterectomy for asymptomatic carotid artery stenosis.  JAMA.1995;273:1421-1428.Google Scholar
4.
European Carotid Surgery Trialists' Collaborative Group MRC European Carotid Surgery Trial: interim results for symptomatic patients with severe (70-99%) or with mild (0-29%) carotid stenosis.  Lancet.1991;337:1235-1243.Google Scholar
5.
Kelly  MPGarron  DCJavid  H Carotid artery disease, carotid endarterectomy, and behavior.  Arch Neurol.1980;37:743-748.Google Scholar
6.
De Leo  DSerraiotto  LPellegrini  CMagni  GFranceschi  LDeriu  GP Outcome from carotid endarterectomy: neuropsychological performances, depressive symptoms and quality of life: 8-month follow-up.  Int J Psychiatry Med.1987;17:317-325.Google Scholar
7.
Bornstein  RABenoit  BGTrites  RL Neuropsychological changes following carotid endarterectomy.  Can J Neurol Sci.1981;8:127-132.Google Scholar
8.
Bennion  RSOwens  MLWilson  SE The effect of unilateral carotid endarterectomy on neuropsychological test performance in 53 patients.  J Thorac Cardiovasc Surg.1985;26:21-26.Google Scholar
9.
Hemmingsen  RMejsholm  BVorstrup  SLester  JEngell  HCBoysen  G Carotid surgery, cognitive function, and cerebral blood flow in patients with transient ischemic attacks.  Ann Neurol.1986;20:13-19.Google Scholar
10.
Antonelli Incalzi  RGemma  ALandi  F  et al Neuropsychologic effects of carotid endarterectomy.  J Clin Exp Neuropsychol.1997;19:785-794.Google Scholar
11.
Casey  JEFerguson  GGKimura  DHachinski  VC Neuropsychological improvement versus practice effect following unilateral carotid endarterectomy in patients without stroke.  J Clin Exp Neuropsychol.1989;11:461-470.Google Scholar
12.
Parker  JCGranberg  BWNichols  WKJones  JGHewett  JE Mental status outcomes following carotid endarterectomy: a six-month analysis.  J Clin Neuropsychol.1983;5:345-353.Google Scholar
13.
Vanninen  EVanninen  RAikia  M  et al Frequency of carotid endarterectomy–related subclinical cerebral complications.  Cerebrovasc Dis.1996;6:272-280.Google Scholar
14.
Heyer  EAdams  DTodd  G  et al Neuropsychometric changes in patients after carotid endarterectomy.  Stroke.1998;29:1110-1115.Google Scholar
15.
Lind  CWimmer  AMagometschnigg  H  et al Effects of carotid endarterectomy on various neuropsychologic parameters: a neuropsychologic longitudinal study [in German].  Langenbecks Arch Chir.1993;378:345-352.Google Scholar
16.
Brinkman  SDBraun  PGanji  SMorrell  RMJacobs  LA Neuropsychological performance one week after carotid endarterectomy reflects intra-operative ischemia.  Stroke.1984;15:497-503.Google Scholar
17.
Cushman  LBrinkman  SDGanji  SJacobs  LA Neuropsychological impairment after carotid endarterectomy correlates with intraoperative ischemia.  Cortex.1984;20:403-412.Google Scholar
18.
Gaunt  MEMartin  PJSmith  JL  et al Clinical relevance of intraoperative embolization detected by transcranial Doppler ultrasonography during carotid endarterectomy: a prospective study of 100 patients.  Br J Surg.1994;81:1435-1439.Google Scholar
19.
Townes  BDDikmen  SSBledsoe  SWHornbein  TFMartin  DCJanesheski  JA Neuropsychological changes in a young, healthy population after controlled hypotensive anesthesia.  Anesth Analg.1986;65:955-959.Google Scholar
20.
Jones  MJ The influence of anesthetic methods on mental function.  Acta Chir Scand Suppl.1989;550:169-175.Google Scholar
21.
Moller  JTCluitmans  PRasmussen  LS  et alfor the International Study of Post-Operative Cognitive Dysfunction investigators Long-term postoperative cognitive dysfunction in the elderly.  Lancet.1998;351:857-861.Google Scholar
22.
Heyer  EJSharma  RWinfree  CJ  et al Severe pain confounds neuropsychological test performance.  J Clin Exp Neuropsychol.2000;22:633-639.Google Scholar
23.
Lezak  MD Neuropsychological Assessment. 3rd ed. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 1995.
24.
Meyers  JMeyers  K Rey Complex Figure Test and Recognition Trial Professional Manual.  Odessa, Fla: Psychological Assessment Resources Inc; 1995.
25.
Downie  WWLeatham  PARhind  VMWright  VBranco  JAAnderson  JA Studies with pain rating scales.  Ann Rheum Dis.1978;37:378-381.Google Scholar
26.
Thal  GDSzabo  MDLopez-Bresnahan  MCrosby  G Exacerbation or unmasking of focal neurologic deficits by sedatives.  Anesthesiology.1996;85:21-25.Google Scholar
27.
Lunn  SCrawley  FHarrison  MJBrown  MMNewman  SP Impact of carotid endarterectomy upon cognitive functioning: a systematic review of the literature.  Cerebrovasc Dis.1999;9:74-81.Google Scholar
28.
Rasmussen  LSChristiansen  MJohnsen  JGronholdt  MLMoller  JT Subtle brain damage cannot be detected by measuring neuron-specific enolase and S-100β protein after carotid endarterectomy.  J Cardiothorac Vasc Anesth.2000;14:166-170.Google Scholar
29.
Sirkka  ASalenius  JPPortin  RNummenmaa  T Quality of life and cognitive performance after carotid endarterectomy during long-term follow-up.  Acta Neurol Scand.1992;85:58-62.Google Scholar
30.
Williams  MMcGee  TF Psychological study of carotid occlusion and endarterectomy.  Arch Neurol.1964;10:293-297.Google Scholar
31.
Owens  MPressman  MEdwards  AE  et al The effect of small infarcts and carotid endarterectomy on postoperative psychologic test performance.  J Surg Res.1980;28:209-216.Google Scholar
×