Background
Functional magnetic resonance imaging is a noninvasive method of assessing language dominance in a pediatric population.
Objective
To determine the pattern of receptive language lateralization in healthy children.
Design
We used functional magnetic resonance imaging to assess an auditory language task in 11 children (7 girls, 4 boys; mean age, 8.5 years). Participants alternately rested and listened to descriptors of nouns presented auditorily, naming the object described silently. Asymmetry indices ([(left − right)/(left + right)]) were calculated for a priori–determined regions of interest.
Results
The results showed strong activation bilaterally, with greater activation on the left in the superior and middle temporal gyri. Other areas of activation included the cuneus, the left inferior temporal gyrus, the prefrontal area, and the left fusiform and lingual gyri. Regions of interest analysis of individual scans showed additional activation in the left frontal lobe. Asymmetry indices showed strong left lateralization of the inferior frontal gyrus, middle frontal gyrus, and the Wernicke region.
Conclusions
Hemispheric lateralization was clearly demonstrated in 8 children. As in adults, left hemisphere lateralization of receptive language is present at age 8 years.
IN MOST ADULTS, language function is primarily subserved by the left hemisphere, as indicated by various methods of assessment: lesions studies,1 the intracarotid sodium amobarbital procedure (IAP),2 electrocortical stimulation,3 and functional neuroimaging.4,5 However, the question remains as to whether left hemisphere specificity for language is innate or whether dominance develops as language is acquired.
Findings of anatomical asymmetries in the left planum temporale, or auditory association area, of neonates and infants,6,7 in addition to functional asymmetries in infants favoring the left hemisphere,8-10 suggest that a left hemispheric specialization for language is present at birth. On the other hand, language has been found to develop to within the normal range on standardized measures in those with unilateral injury to the left hemisphere before the age of 6 months.11 These findings lend support to the equipotentiality hypothesis that proposes the equal capacity of either hemisphere to subserve language function. Moreover, infants and children do not suffer the same neurological consequences as do adults with analogous lesions of the left hemisphere,12,13 suggesting the developing brain's functional plasticity. However, despite evidence of plasticity, other investigators have found language deficits after early left hemisphere insult.14
In the absence of brain injury, there are 2 possible explanations for the development of left hemisphere language dominance: (1) Language is solely supported by the left hemisphere from birth; or (2) Language is supported by both hemispheres at an early age but becomes increasingly consolidated in the left hemisphere as language competency increases. The critical window of language development and neural plasticity has been debated to some extent, with some investigators proposing that it extends to puberty15,16 and others asserting a more conservative age of 5 or 6 years.17
Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) has been shown to be an effective means of assessing laterality, with numerous advantages over the more invasive IAP procedure in terms of its safety, noninvasiveness, replicability, and ability to apply to a nonclinical population.18-21 Review of the adult imaging literature in the area of auditory language paradigms reveals several consistent findings: (1) The presentation of auditory lexical stimuli, when contrasted with the absence of sounds, activates the superior temporal gyrus (STG) bilaterally, extending beyond the primary auditory cortex. Activation is consistently more extensive on the left, suggesting a specificity for language processing in this area22-25; and (2) Semantic processing of auditory stimuli, such as listening to speech or performing a semantic decision task, appears to activate a network, including most consistently although not exclusively, the left inferior frontal gyrus (IFG),22,26-29 the left middle temporal gyrus (MTG),22,26,30,31 and the left temporoparietal area.23,26-28,31
Hertz-Pannier et al20 demonstrated the efficacy of fMRI to identify language cortices in the frontal lobes of children and adolescents with epilepsy using a word generation task, and more recently, Gaillard et al32 provided similar evidence with healthy children using a reading task. There have been a small number of fMRI studies of receptive language conducted with children. In a study of six 6- to 10-year-old children (mean age, 8 years), Ulualp et al33 used a passive story listening task that activated frontal and temporal areas, consistent with adult studies. They did not find a pattern of lateralization in primary and secondary auditory cortices. However, their paradigm did not require an active response and perhaps did not sufficiently provoke activity in the language cortex. Studies from Booth and colleagues34,35 provide inconsistent conclusions. In their first study of auditory comprehension, they found evidence for bilateral activation. Six children (aged 9-12 years) listened to sentences ranging in complexity, after which they were required to identify elements of the sentence. The authors found bilateral activation of the STG and IFG regions but conjectured that bilateral activation may have been a product of task complexity, which has been shown to increase activation in homologous areas of the right hemisphere.36 However, in a second, similar study, in which an alternate method of calculating asymmetry indices was used, left lateralization was found.35 Given these differences in paradigms and in analysis, it is difficult to draw conclusions about language representation in children of that age.
Our study was designed to assess language laterality in normally developing 8-year-old children using fMRI and an auditory language task. The auditory paradigm implemented was expected to produce activation patterns of the IFG, MTG, and STG. The task was designed to require a response, thus ensuring a degree of higher-order processing.
Participants were 11 children (7 female, 4 male). They ranged in age from 7.3 to 9.6 years (mean [SD], 8.5 [0.9] years). Participants were recruited from the community via posted advertisements. They were paid for their participation. All participants had normal neurologic examination results, normal structural MRI results, and were healthy. All were right-handed as assessed by a modified version of the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory,37 using 7 of the 10 original items most suitable for children. All children had a handedness score of 100, indicating strong preference for the right hand. If a participant had a diagnosis of learning or attentional difficulties, or English was not his or her first language, he or she was excluded from the study. Testing administered to 10 of the children subsequent to their scan indicated that, on average, these children performed in the high average-to-superior range on standardized measures of expressive naming, reading, and cognition. Children received a tour of the MRI facilities prior to scanning. This study received prior approval by the National Institutes of Neurological Disorders and Stroke institutional review board. Written parental consent and child assent were obtained.
Stimuli for the Auditory Response Naming task consisted of auditorily presented several-word phrases.27 The participant silently named the object the phrase described. For example, the correct response to the phrase "long yellow fruit" was "banana." Covert responses were used to minimize motion artifacts. Descriptions of age-appropriate items were generated from the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test,38 the One-Word Expressive Picture Vocabulary Test,39 and the Boston Naming Test.40 Seven clues were presented in each 32-second epoch (interstimulus interval of 0.5 seconds). The individual had approximately 4 seconds to listen and to respond to the stimulus. Task difficulty was adjusted based on age so that participants would respond accurately approximately 85% of the time. The naming task alternated with a control task, in which the background noise of the scanner was present; the participant was instructed to rest during these periods. There were 6 cycles of the rest and task conditions for total task duration of 6 minutes 24 seconds. The same speaker delivered the stimuli binaurally over the scanner intercom into headphones worn by the participant. The headphones attenuated the noise of the scanner. A test run in which a sentence was read to the participant and the participant was asked to repeat it was conducted to ensure that the participant could adequately hear the stimuli.
Images were collected with a 1.5-T General Electric Signa scanner (General Electric, Milwaukee, Wis) equipped with a birdcage radio frequency coil. The participant's head was stabilized with a forehead strap and foam padding. Functional images were acquired with a single-shot, blipped, gradient-echo echo-planar sequence (EPI) (echo time [TE] = 40 milliseconds, field of view [FOV] = 22 cm × 22 cm, acquisition matrix = 64 × 64). Subsequent to the collection of the functional images, anatomical images were acquired using a 3-dimensional fast spin-echo gradient sequence with an inversion impulse (TE = 3.5 milliseconds, repetition time [TR] = 10.1 seconds, TI = 600 milliseconds, flip angle = 20°, slice thickness = 5 mm, FOV = 24 cm × 24 cm, acquisition matrix = 256 × 256, voxel size = 3.4375 mm × 3.4375 mm × 5 mm). For both the functional and structural images, the whole brain was imaged with the collection of 20 contiguous axial images parallel to the anterior-posterior commissure plane. Total scanning time was 18 minutes.
The data were processed and analyzed using the general linear model41 with statistical parametric mapping 99 software (Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, London, England). Prior to statistical analysis, all images were normalized to an EPI template conforming to the Talairach and Tournoux42 convention and then smoothed (full width at half maximum = 8 mm3). To minimize false-positive and false-negative results, 2 different statistical analyses were performed: a fixed-effect design and a more stringent conjunction analysis.43t Tests were used for all statistical contrasts of the rest condition to the experimental condition. The fixed analysis combined all scans of the same condition within a group. Individual voxels were significantly activated if they survived a height threshold of P<.001 (corrected for multiple comparisons) and an extent threshold of 10 voxels. For the conjunction analysis, individual voxels were significantly activated only if each subject activated the identical voxel at or above a height threshold of P<.05 (corrected). Thus, voxels not activated in every subject were effectively eliminated.
Analysis of data at the individual level can be a source of additional information. Analysis of individual participant data was conducted using a region of interest approach. A priori regions of interest (for each hemisphere) were drawn on raw EPI images: IFG, middle frontal gyrus (MFG), the Wernicke area, and MTG. Analyses of individual regions were conducted using semiautomated image-analyzing software that subtracted signal change between control and task conditions on a voxel-by-voxel basis. A voxel was significantly activated if it survived a threshold value of t>3.0. 44 The total number of activated voxels in each region was automatically calculated.
Lateralization of each region was determined with the calculation of an asymmetry index (AI). The AI for each region was calculated using the formula: AI = [left − right / left + right], with left and right representing the number of activated voxels in the respective hemisphere. A positive AI (>+0.2) indicated left-hemisphere dominance and a negative AI (<–0.2) indicated right-hemisphere dominance. Any value falling between –0.2 and +0.2 was considered to be indicative of bilateral activation.45 Two other criteria were also met to consider an AI valid: (1) At least 4 voxels were activated in a particular region; and (2) There was at least a 3-voxel difference between homologous regions.44
Statistical parametric mapping group results
When compared with the rest task, Auditory Responsive Naming strongly activated the following regions bilaterally but with greater activation on the left: MTG (Brodmann area [BA] 22 and 21), STG (BA22), inferior occipital gyrus (IOG) (BA18), and the cuneus (BA18). There was also activation in the cerebellum bilaterally, the left inferior temporal gyrus (ITG) (BA37), the left fusiform gyrus (BA18), the left lingual gyrus, and the left MFG (BA9). Some activation was found in the frontal medialis region (BA10) bilaterally and the right superior frontal gyrus (SFG) (BA10). Table 1 presents the z score for each area of activation as well as the number of voxels activated in the respective area. Figure 1 shows the group activation map.
The conjunction analysis in Figure 2 showed strongly lateralized and highly significant activation in the left MTG and STG, which largely replicated the fixed-effect analysis. Smaller homologous regions in the right hemisphere were also activated (Table 2).
Regions of interest results
On a subject-by-subject basis, laterality based on regions of interest was most efficiently determined at a threshold of t = 3 (Table 3). Mean asymmetry indices per region revealed moderate left lateralization in each region and are presented in Figure 3. The MFG and Wernicke area were most strongly lateralized to the left, with asymmetry indices of 0.43 and 0.41, respectively. Weaker lateralized activation was found in the MTG (mean, 0.20).
This fMRI study was designed to examine language laterality in children. Results of the study revealed highly lateralized activation in the left MTG and STG. These findings strongly indicate left hemisphere language dominance for auditory comprehension in children between the ages of 7 and 9 years. Group analysis showing bilateral activation of the STG, including the primary auditory cortex, is consistent with other studies of auditory stimuli. Activation was asymmetrical, favoring the left hemisphere, which reflects the lexical component of the stimuli.24,46 As anticipated, our results showed strong activation in the MTG. The MTG has been consistently associated with semantic processing30,31,47,48 although it has also been implicated in phonological processing.24,28,49,50
In addition to the hypothesized areas of activation, the fixed-effect analysis also revealed significant activation of the left cuneus, left ITG, and bilateral prefrontal areas. Findings suggest that the cuneate region is used in creating a visual image.27,34,51,52 The paradigm used in our study may be conducive to this process since each stimulus describes a concrete noun of which one can easily generate a mental image. Activation of the ITG and prefrontal areas (BA10) are also consistent with previous studies that found these areas to be implicated in semantic processing, object recognition, and working memory, respectively.22,34,46,50,53,54
Examination of the mean asymmetry indices per region showed all regions to be lateralized to the left hemisphere, providing additional support for the presence of strong lateralization in children as young as 7 years. Standard deviations are large, indicating substantial subject variability in the amount of voxels activated in each region and in asymmetry indices. Because of the extent of anatomical variability in language cortices, the group-analysis method of analyzing data is insufficient and at times inaccurate.55 Analysis on a case-by-case basis can provide meaningful information. Overall, 8 of the 11 participants showed clear evidence of left hemisphere language dominance. Two of the remaining children expressed bilateral activation, and one child's scan was nondiagnostic. With the use of additional scans and alternate language paradigms, more conclusive evidence of hemispheric dominance was provided for 2 participants.
The group analyses did not produce any significant activation in the IFG as predicted; however, in the individual analyses, IFG and/or adjacent MFG activation was observed in most participants. There are several technical issues that pertain to the acquisition and analysis of child-specific data that may contribute to these conflicting findings.32 Many of these technical issues derive from anatomical differences between children and adults. For example, children's brains are smaller,56 partly due to the process of myelination and synaptic pruning, which is not complete until adolescence.57 Myelination proceeds from posterior (occipital) to anterior (frontal) brain regions; thus, the frontal lobes may be particularly susceptible to distortion since they are less developed than the temporal lobes in the child brain.58 Furthermore, there is substantial individual variability in the location of language cortices.59 Distortion created when normalizing a child-size brain into a standard stereotactic space based on an adult-size brain, in conjunction with normal individual variability, may eliminate consistent areas of frontal lobe activation across individuals. Thus, an adult-based model may not be optimal (although will be used until a pediatric atlas becomes available). Finally, we speculate that variability in location of frontal lobe activation may reflect the use of different word-finding strategies among children.
Two fMRI studies of auditory comprehension in children have found predominantly bilateral activation.33,34 Consistent with these studies, analysis of our data at the individual level showed similar areas of activation in the temporal cortex, the IFG, and the prefrontal area. However, our study differed substantially in terms of lateralization. We found strong lateralization in the left hemisphere for the Auditory Responsive Naming paradigm. There may be a number of reasons for this difference. First, our sample size was larger than the other child studies and the age range more restricted. Second, our task differed from the others in that it was not overly difficult nor was it a passive task, as it required covert responses. Furthermore, each study used a different auditory language comprehension paradigm. Finally, Booth et al34,35 and Ulualp et al33 used alternative methods from those used in our study to calculate laterality. Different analyses may affect outcome. For example, the earlier study by Booth et al34 calculated correlations between percentage of voxels activated in each hemispheric region but when similar data were examined with an analysis of variance using percentage of activated voxels in hemispheres and regions as independent variables, left lateralization was found.35 The selection of the formula implemented in our study was based on its correspondence to Wada testing.19
In comparison with an analogous study conducted with 24 adults,60 there were no differences found between the number of activated voxels within regions or in asymmetry indices. In other words, on an identical listening comprehension task, children and adults show highly similar patterns in the extent of activation and degree of lateralization. In conclusion, there are several limitations of this study necessary to address. First, we used a small sample of children; therefore, broad conclusions are difficult to make. Second, language and cognitive testing indicated that these children performed in the high average to superior range; thus, it is possible that highly lateralized language in children of this age is partially a product of their substantial language and cognitive capacities. Finally, because we did not obtain behavioral data, we have no concrete evidence that the children were actually performing the task as instructed. However, the consistency of activation patterns across participants suggests task compliance.
This study used fMRI to demonstrate left hemispheric language dominance of auditory comprehension in normally developing 8-year-old children. At this age, children show a lateralized pattern highly similar to that of adults. Our data provide preliminary neuroimaging evidence in support of those anatomic, evoked response potential, and early unilateral injury findings that suggest early left hemisphere language lateralization.
Accepted for publication January 31, 2002.
Author contributions: Study concept and design (Dr Grandin, Ms Braniecki, and Drs Elliott and Gaillard); acquisition of data (Mss Balsamo and Braniecki and Drs Xu, Grandin, Petrella, and Gaillard); analysis and interpretation of data (Ms Balsamo and Drs Grandin, Elliott, and Gaillard); drafting of the manuscript (Ms Balsamo and Dr Gaillard); critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content (Drs Xu, Grandin, and Petrella, Elliott, and Gaillard and Ms Braniecki); statistical expertise (Drs Xu and Gaillard); obtained funding (Dr Gaillard); administrative, technical, and material support (Drs Xu, Grandin, Petrella, and Gaillard and Ms Braniecki); study supervision (Drs Elliott and Gaillard).
This study was supported by grant K08-NS1663 from the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (NINDS), Bethesda, Md; the Epilepsy Research Branch, NINDS; and a grant from the Board of Lady Visitors, Children's National Medical Center, Washington, DC.
We thank William Theodore, MD, for his continued support and for making the Epilepsy Research Branch resources available. We also thank Suzanne Reigle, BA, for her help in preparing the manuscript.
Corresponding author and reprints: William D. Gaillard, MD, Department of Neurology, Children's National Medical Center, 111 Michigan Ave NW, Washington, DC 20010 (e-mail: gaillardw@ninds.nih.gov).
2.Rasmussen
TMilner
B The role of early left-brain injury in determining lateralization of cerebral speech functions.
Ann N Y Acad Sci.1977;299:355-369.
Google Scholar 3.Ojemann
GA Surgical therapy for medically intractable epilepsy.
J Neurosurg.1987;66:489-499.
Google Scholar 4.Pujol
JDeus
JLosilla
JMCapdevila
A Cerebral lateralization of language in normal left-handed people studied by functional MRI.
Neurology.1999;52:1038-1043.
Google Scholar 5.Springer
JABinder
JRHammeke
TA
et al Language dominance in neurologically normal and epilepsy subjects: a functional MRI study.
Brain.1999;122:2033-2046.
Google Scholar 6.Preis
SJancke
LSchmitz-Hillebrecht
JSteinmetz
H Child age and planum temporale asymmetry.
Brain Cogn.1999;40:441-452.
Google Scholar 7.Wada
JAClarke
RHamm
A Cerebral hemispheric asymmetry in humans: cortical speech zones in 100 adults and 100 infant brains.
Arch Neurol.1975;32:239-246.
Google Scholar 8.Dehaene-Lambertz
G Cerebral specialization for speech and non-speech stimuli in infants.
J Cogn Neurosci.2000;12:449-460.
Google Scholar 9.Molfese
DLMolfese
VJ Cortical responses of preterm infants to phonetic and nonphonetic speech stimuli.
Dev Psychol.1980;16:574-581.
Google Scholar 10.Witelson
SFPallie
W Left hemisphere specialization for language in the newborn: neuroanatomical evidence of asymmetry.
Brain.1973;96:641-646.
Google Scholar 11.Muter
VTaylor
SVargha-Khadem
F A longitudinal study of early intellectual development in hemiplegic children.
Neuropsychologia.1997;35:289-298.
Google Scholar 12.Aram
DM Language sequelae of unilateral brain lesions in children.
In: Plum
F, ed.
Language, Communication and the Brain. New York, NY: Raven Press; 1998.
Google Scholar 13.Hecaen
H Acquired aphasia in children: revisited.
Neuropsychologia.1983;21:581-587.
Google Scholar 14.Woods
BTCarey
S Language deficits after apparent clinical recovery from childhood aphasia.
Ann Neurol.1979;6:405-409.
Google Scholar 15.Lenneberg
EH Biological Foundations of Language. New York, NY: Wiley; 1967.
16.Vargha-Khadem
FCarr
LJIsaacs
EBrett
EAdams
CMishkin
M Onset of speech after left hemispherectomy in a nine-year-old boy.
Brain.1997;120:159-182.
Google Scholar 17.Krashen
SD Lateralization, language learning, and the critical period: some new evidence.
Language Learning.1973;23:63-74.
Google Scholar 18.Benson
RRFitzGerald
DBLeSueur
LL
et al Language dominance determined by whole brain functional MRI in patients with brain lesions.
Neurology.1999;52:798-809.
Google Scholar 19.Binder
JRSwanson
SJHammeke
TA
et al Determination of language dominance using functional MRI: a comparison with the Wada test.
Neurology.1996;46:978-984.
Google Scholar 20.Hertz-Pannier
LGaillard
WDMott
SH
et al Noninvasive assessment of language dominance in children and adolescents with functional MRI: a preliminary study.
Neurology.1997;48:1003-1012.
Google Scholar 21.Lehéricy
SCohen
LBazin
B
et al Functional MR evaluation of temporal and frontal language dominance compared with the Wada test.
Neurology.2000;54:1625-1633.
Google Scholar 22.Chee
MWLO'Craven
KMBergida
RRosen
BRSavoy
RL Auditory and visual word processing studies with fMRI.
Hum Brain Mapp.1999;7:15-28.
Google Scholar 23.Demonet
JFChollet
FRamsay
S
et al The anatomy of phonological and semantic processing in normal subjects.
Brain.1992;115:1753-1768.
Google Scholar 24.Mazoyer
BMTzourio
NFrak
V
et al The cortical representation of speech.
J Cogn Neurosci.1993;5:467-479.
Google Scholar 25.Wise
RChollet
FHadar
UFriston
KHoffner
EFrackowiak
R Distribution of cortical neural networks involved in word comprehension and word retrieval.
Brain.1991;114:1803-1817.
Google Scholar 26.Binder
JRRao
SMHammeke
A
et al Lateralized human brain language systems demonstrated by task subtraction functional magnetic resonance imaging.
Arch Neurol.1995;52:593-601.
Google Scholar 27.Bookheimer
SYZeffiro
TABlaxton
TAGaillard
WDMalow
BTheodore
WH Regional cerebral blood flow during auditory responsive naming: evidence for cross-modality neural activation.
Neuroreport.1998;9:2409-2413.
Google Scholar 28.Zatorre
RJEvans
ACMeyer
EGjedde
A Lateralization of phonetic and pitch discrimination in speech processing.
Science.1992;256:846-884.
Google Scholar 29.Poldrack
RAWagner
ADPrull
MWDesmond
JEGlover
GHGabrieli
JDE Functional specialization for semantic and phonological processing in the left inferior prefrontal cortex.
Neuroimage.1999;10:15-35.
Google Scholar 30.Schlosser
MJAoyagi
NFulbright
RKGore
JCMcCarthy
G Functional MRI studies of auditory comprehension.
Hum Brain Mapp.1998;6:1-13.
Google Scholar 31.Warburton
EWise
RJSPrice
CJ
et al Noun and verb retrieval by normal subjects: studies with PET.
Brain.1996;119:159-179.
Google Scholar 32.Gaillard
WDGrandin
CBXu
B Developmental aspects of pediatric fMRI: considerations for image acquisition, analysis, and interpretation.
Neuroimage.2001;13:239-249.
Google Scholar 33.Ulualp
SOBiswal
BBYetkin
ZKidder
TM Functional magnetic resonance imaging of auditory cortex in children.
Laryngoscope.1998;108:1782-1786.
Google Scholar 34.Booth
JRMacWhinney
BThulborn
KRSacco
KVoyvodic
JFeldman
HM Functional organization of activation patterns in children: whole brain fMRI imaging during three different cognitive tasks.
Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatr.1999;23:669-682.
Google Scholar 35.Booth
JRMacWhinney
BThulborn
KRSacco
KVoyvodic
JTFeldman
HM Developmental and lesion effects in brain activation during sentence comprehension and mental rotation.
Dev Neuropsychol.2000;18:139-169.
Google Scholar 36.Just
MACarpenter
PAKeller
TAEddy
WFThulborn
KR Brain activation modulated by sentence comprehension.
Science.1996;274:114-116.
Google Scholar 37.Oldfield
RC The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory.
Neuropsychologia.1971;9:97-113.
Google Scholar 38.Dunn
LMDunn
LM Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised. Circle Pines, Minn: American Guidance Center; 1981.
39.Gardner
MF Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised. Novato, Calif: Academic Therapy Publications; 1990.
40.Kaplan
EFGoodglass
HWeintraub
S The Boston Naming Test. 2nd ed. Philadelphia, Pa: Lea & Febiger; 1983.
41.Friston
KJFrith
CDLiddle
PFFrakowiak
RS Comparing functional (PET) images: the assessment of clinical change.
J Cereb Blood Flow Metab.1991;11:690-699.
Google Scholar 42.Talairach
JTournoux
P Co-Planar Sterotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain. New York, NY: Thieme Medical; 1988.
43.Friston
KJHolmes
APPrice
CJBüchel
CWorsley
KJ Multisubject fMRI studies and conjunction analyses.
Neuroimage.1999;10:385-396.
Google Scholar 44.Gaillard
WDPugliese
MGrandin,
CB
et al Cortical localization of reading in normal children: an fMRI language study.
Neurology.2001;57:47-54.
Google Scholar 45.Springer
JABinder
JRHammeke
TA
et al Language dominance in neurologically normal and epilepsy subjects: a functional MRI study.
Brain.1999;122:2033-2046.
Google Scholar 46.Muller
R-ARothermel
RDBehen
MEMuzik
OMangner
TJChugani
HT Receptive and expressive language activations for sentences: a PET study.
Neuroreport.1997;8:3767-3770.
Google Scholar 47.Howard
DPatterson
KWise
R
et al The cortical localization of the lexicons.
Brain.1992;115:1769-1782.
Google Scholar 48.Malow
BABlaxton
TASato
S
et al Cortical stimulation elicits regional distinctions in auditory and visual naming.
Epilepsia.1996;37:245-252.
Google Scholar 49.Pugh
KRShaywitz
BAShaywitz
SE
et al Cerebral organization of component processes in reading.
Brain.1996;119:1221-1238.
Google Scholar 50.Shaywitz
BAPugh
KRConstable
T
et al Localization of semantic processing using functional magnetic resonance imaging.
Hum Brain Mapp.1998;2:149-158.
Google Scholar 51.Kosslyn
SMThompson
WLKim
IJAlpert
NM Topographcial representations of mental images in primary visual cortex.
Nature.1995;378:496-498.
Google Scholar 52.Pugliese
MGaillard
WDBasso
G
et al Functional brain mapping of visual mental imagery in children.
Neuroimage.1999;9(suppl):S352.
Google Scholar 53.Braver
TSCohen
JSNystrom
LEJonides
JSmith
EENoll
DC A parametric study of prefrontal cortex involvement in human working memory.
Neuroimage.1997;5:49-62.
Google Scholar 54.Underleider
LGHaxby
JV ‘What' and ‘where' in the human brain.
Curr Opini Neurobiol.1994;4:157-165.
Google Scholar 55.Steinmetz
H Seitz RJ Functional anatomy of language processing: neuroimaging and the problem of individual variability.
Neuropsychologia.1991;29:1149-1161.
Google Scholar 56.Filipek
PARichelme
CKennedy
DNCaviness
VS The young adult human brain: an MRI-based morphometric analysis.
Cereb Cortex.1994;4:344-360.
Google Scholar 57.Giedd
JNBlumenthal
JJeffries
NO
et al Brain development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal MRI study.
Nat Neurosci.1999;2:861-863.
Google Scholar 58.Yakovlev
PILecours
AR The myelogenetic cycles of regional maturation of the brain.
In: Minkowski
A, ed.
Regional Development of the Brain in Early Life. Oxford, England: Blackwell; 1967:3-70.
Google Scholar 59.Ojemann
GAOjemann
JLettich
EBerger
M Cortical language localization in left, dominant hemisphere: an electrical stimulation mapping investigation in 117 patients.
J Neurosurg.1989;71:316-326.
Google Scholar 60.Grandin
CBGaillard
WDWhitnah
JR
et al Gender related differences in activated brain areas for language processing: an fMRI study.
Neuroimage.1998;7(suppl):S159.
Google Scholar