Background
The head-impulse test, which is sensitive and specific for detecting severe unilateral peripheral vestibulopathy, is an accepted part of the neurological examination, especially in patients with vertigo and balance disorders.
Objective
To discover if the head-impulse test is just as useful diagnostically when patients are asked to rotate their own heads, the active head-impulse test, rather than when the clinician does so as in the standard passive head-impulse test.
Methods
Clinical observation of compensatory saccades and search coil measurement of compensatory eye rotations, during active and passive horizontal head-impulses in 6 patients with total unilateral vestibular deafferentation.
Results
Clinical observation showed the expected compensatory saccades with rotations toward the side with the lesion with passive head-impulses but not with active head-impulses. Search coil recordings revealed 2 reasons for this. With active head-impulses not only was vestibulo-ocular reflex gain higher, but compensatory saccade latency was shorter resulting in an occult saccade that occurred during, rather than after, head rotation.
Conclusions
Passive head-impulses are necessary to detect a severe unilateral peripheral vestibulopathy; active head-impulses will produce a false-negative result.
The head-impulse test, since we first described it 15 years ago,1 has become an accepted part of the neurological examination.2 It detects severe unilateral loss of semicircular canal (SCC) function clinically and is important for the assessment of dizzy patients in the emergency department and physician’s office.3 In the emergency department the head-impulse test can distinguish between vestibular neuritis and cerebellar infarction in a patient seen with an initial attack of severe acute spontaneous vertigo: the result of the head-impulse test is invariably normal in a patient with a cerebellar infarction but abnormal in a patient with vestibular neuritis. In the physician’s office the head-impulse test can identify a vestibular disorder in the patient with recurrent dizziness.
Could the head-impulse test be as useful with active head-impulses as with passive head-impulses, that is, with the patients themselves rotating their heads rather than the clinician doing so? To address this question we trained 6 patients with total unilateral vestibular deafferentation after surgery for vestibular schwannoma (acoustic neuroma) to make rapid, active head-impulses. We observed their eye movements clinically and then measured the horizontal vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR) with active as well as with passive head-impulses. The velocity profiles of the active head-impulses matched those of the passive head-impulses. We found that not only is the horizontal VOR gain higher with active than with passive head-impulses4 but that also with active head-impulses the compensatory saccade occurs during, rather than after, the head-impulse, thus giving the false impression of a normal horizontal VOR and, consequently, a spuriously negative finding for the head-impulse test in patients with total unilateral loss of SCC function.
Three-dimensional eye and head positions were measured in a magnetic field (CNC Engineering, Seattle, Wash) with dual-axis search coils (Skalar, Delft, the Netherlands). Eye position was measured using a scleral search coil contact lens placed on the left eye. Head position was measured using a search coil mounted in a mouth guard composed of dental impression material (President Putty; Coltène/Whaledent Inc, Mahwah, NJ) in a disposable dental impression tray. Full details of the recording methods have been reported.5,6
We studied the VOR with active and passive head-impulses in 6 subjects with total unilateral vestibular deafferentation (5 men, 1 woman), aged 32 to 71 years, more than 2 years after vestibular schwannoma (acoustic neuroma) surgery. The head-impulse is a low-amplitude (15º-20°), high-acceleration (4000°/s2-6000°/s2) head rotation, in this case horizontal, with a peak velocity of 150º/s to 350º/s. Details of the head-impulse test have been previously published.6,7 A passive head-impulse is delivered by an investigator who stands behind the patient and firmly holds the patient’s head; at unpredictable times the investigator rapidly turns the patient’s head to the left or right by 15º to 20º while the patient tries to keep staring at the target. The target is an earth-fixed laser dot located 94 cm straight ahead of the left eye. Active head-impulses were made by the patients themselves: each was asked to make rapid horizontal head rotations with velocities that fell within a template envelope representing the upper and lower 95% confidence intervals of the velocity of all passive head-impulses. The template was shown on a screen throughout the experiment. As the patient made an active head-impulse its velocity profile was instantaneously displayed on the head velocity template envelope (Figure 1). With this feedback all patients learned to make active head-impulses that fell within the passive head-impulse velocity envelope. Only those head velocities that fell within the envelope were analyzed. The peak velocities and durations of active and passive head-impulses were therefore matched. Written informed consent was obtained from subjects prior to testing, according to the Declaration of Helsinki. The experimental protocol was approved by the human ethics committee of the Central Sydney Area Health Service, Sydney, Australia.
The VOR with active and passive head-impulses was analyzed from the onset of head rotation to final gaze fixation—about 500 milliseconds. The gain of the VOR was calculated by finding the gradient of a line fitted to the mean eye velocity plotted as a function of head velocity in a time frame window from 30 to 75 milliseconds of head rotation onset. Full details of the analysis methods have been previously published.6 Mean ± 2-tailed 95% confidence intervals were computed for head and eye velocity. A 2-tailed t test (P<.05) was performed to determine whether a statistically significant difference existed between head velocities and accelerations and VOR gains with active vs passive head-impulses.
All 6 patients learned to make active head-impulses that matched the passive head-impulses within about 20 attempts. While it was easy to see the compensatory saccade with passive ipsilesional head-impulses—the clinical sign of canal paresis1 —we all had difficulty in doing so when the patient made active head-impulses because compensatory eye movements appeared smooth during most active head-impulses.
Using scleral search coil recordings, it was possible to make a valid comparison of the VOR in response to active vs passive head-impulses because the head velocity profiles in the first 75 milliseconds were the same (Figure 2). The results were that while ipsilesional VOR gain was abnormally low with both active and passive head-impulses, with active head-impulses (mean ± SD, 0.38 ± 0.1), it was almost twice that seen with passive head-impulses (0.18 ± 0.02) (Figure 2). In contrast, contralesional VOR gain was only slightly higher with active head-impulses (0.78 ± 0.08) than with passive head-impulses (0.68 ± 0.06) (Figure 3).
Furthermore, the latency of the first saccade, which corrects the gaze position error accumulated during the ipsilesional head-impulse and forms the clinical sign of canal paresis,1 was shorter with active than with passive ipsilesional head-impulses. With passive ipsilesional head-impulses, the mean latency of the compensatory saccade was 219 ± 102 milliseconds (mean ± SD) so that it invariably started only after the head had stopped rotating (Figure 2). In contrast with active ipsilesional head-impulses, the mean latency of the compensatory saccade was only 87 ± 11 milliseconds, so that typically it would begin while the head was still rotating. Furthermore, since VOR gain was higher with active than with passive ipsilesional head-impulses, the accrued gaze error and therefore the mean amplitude of the compensatory saccade was lower with active (approximately 4º) than with passive head-impulses (approximately 12º). Consequently, with active ipsilesional head-impulses, the eye position response appeared to be almost perfectly compensatory for head rotation (Figure 2)—creating the misleading impression of a near-normal VOR.
The technique and interpretation of the passive head-impulse test is generally straightforward as long as one recalls that SCC afferent neurons have a tonic firing rate at rest—about 40 spikes per second in the squirrel monkey.8 Rotations away from an SCC (ie, in the off-direction) decrease the firing rate of the neuron whereas rotations toward an SCC (in the on-direction) increase its firing rate. With high-acceleration rotations, it is much easier to saturate the firing rate of a vestibular neuron in the off-direction, that is, to silence it, than to saturate its firing rate in the on-direction.9 In a patient with only one functioning labyrinth, rapid head rotations toward the side with the lesion will silence SCC afferents on the intact side and saturate the gain (eye velocity divided by head velocity) of the VOR.6 Consider a patient with left vestibular neuritis: if the patient fixates a distant earth-fixed target while the clinician rapidly rotates the patient’s head to the left, the passive head-impulse test, the patient will make 1 or 2 observable, compensatory, that is, rightward, saccades just after the head-impulse is over. This is the head-impulse sign and it indicates that the gain of the horizontal VOR, which is generated from inhibition of the sole functioning, right, lateral SCC, rather than from excitation of the nonfunctioning left lateral SCC, is severely defective. In other words, the VOR gain is much less than 1.0. In contrast, when the clinician rotates the patient’s head to the right, there will be no compensatory saccades indicating that the gain of the VOR, generated by excitation from the right lateral SCC plus any inhibition from the left lateral SCC, is close to 1.0. Not only can the compensatory saccades be observed clinically, the VOR deficit can be measured oculographically4-7,10-12 not only from lateral but also from individual vertical SCCs.6,13
The present results show that active head-impulses, unlike passive head-impulses, are not an effective clinical way to show severe unilateral loss of vestibular function. They confirm that in unilateral vestibular loss ipsilesional VOR gain is abnormally low with both active4,14 and passive head-impulses5-7 ; however, they also show that the VOR gain is significantly higher with active than with passive head-impulses. Although ipsilesional VOR gain is still low with active head-impulses resulting in a measurable, and potentially observable, gaze-error correcting compensatory saccade, the saccade latency is shorter with active head-impulses than with passive head-impulses so that the saccade is occult in that it occurs during, rather than after, the head-impulse. Because the velocity profile of the compensatory saccade resembles that of the head-impulse itself, it is easy clinically to mistake the compensatory saccade for a normal compensatory VOR and consider the findings of the head-impulse test to be normal. Consequently, for the diagnosis of a unilateral peripheral vestibulopathy, active impulses have 2 disadvantages compared with passive head-impulses: they not only produce a higher VOR gain, but they also generate an earlier compensatory saccade that can mimic a normal VOR.
Why is VOR gain higher and compensatory saccade latency shorter with active than with passive head-impulses? There is no theoretical reason why the VOR gain should be higher and there are no animal data because it has not proven possible to train an animal to make active head-impulses. Single neuron data from vestibular nucleus neurons in alert behaving monkeys show reduced activation with active vs passive head rotations.15 However, in these experiments the animal’s goal was refixation rather than fixation, a task that would benefit from a lower VOR gain. Active head rotations are normally made to refix rather than to fix gaze and that is presumably why for human subjects it feels unnatural to rotate the head rapidly while trying to look straight ahead. Saccadic latency might be shorter with active head-impulses than with passive head-impulses because of a learning effect. Our subjects were trained to match the velocity profile of their own passive head-impulses with an active head rotation. Therefore, our subjects may have learned to predict their own gaze error and to make compensatory express saccades16 almost in time with head rotation. It is clear, therefore, that passive head-impulses rather than active head-impulses are needed to demonstrate a unilateral vestibular deficit.
Correspondence: Ross A. Black, MBiomed E, Department of Neurology, Royal Prince Alfred Hospital, Camperdown, New South Wales, Sydney, 2050, Australia (rossb@icn.usyd.edu.au).
Accepted for Publication: May 5, 2004.
Author Contributions:Study concept and design: Black, Halmagyi, Thurtell, and Curthoys. Acquisition of data: Black, Thurtell, and Todd. Analysis and interpretation of data: Black, Halmagyi, Thurtell, Todd, and Curthoys. Drafting of the manuscript: Black, Halmagyi, Thurtell, and Curthoys. Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Black, Halmagyi, Todd, and Curthoys. Statistical analysis: Black and Curthoys. Obtained funding: Halmagyi and Curthoys. Administrative, technical, and material support: Black, Thurtell, and Todd. Study supervision: Halmagyi and Curthoys.
Funding/Support: This study was supported in part by the National Health and Medical Research Council, Canberra, Australia; the Garnett Passe and Rodney Williams Memorial Foundation, Melbourne, Australia; and the Royal Prince Albert Hospital Neurology Department Trustees, Sydney.
4.Della Santina
CCCremer
PDCarey
JPMinor
LB Comparison of head thrust test with head autorotation test reveals that the vestibulo-ocular reflex is enhanced during voluntary head movements.
Arch Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg 2002;1281044- 1054
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 5.Aw
STHalmagyi
GMHaslwanter
TCurthoys
ISYavor
RATodd
MJ Three-dimensional vector analysis of the human vestibulo-ocular reflex in response to high acceleration head rotations, II: responses in subjects with unilateral vestibular loss and selective semicircular canal occlusion.
J Neurophysiol 1996;764021- 4030
PubMedGoogle Scholar 6.Cremer
PDHalmagyi
GMAw
ST
et al. Semicircular canal plane head impulses detect absent function of individual semicircular canals.
Brain 1998;121699- 716
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 7.Halmagyi
GMCurthoys
ISCremer
PD
et al. The human horizontal vestibulo-ocular reflex in response to high-acceleration stimulation before and after unilateral vestibular neurectomy.
Exp Brain Res 1990;81479- 490
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 8.Goldberg
JMFernandez
C Physiology of peripheral neurons innervating the semicircular canals of the squirrel monkey, III: variations among units in their discharge properties.
J Neurophysiol 1971;34676- 684
PubMedGoogle Scholar 9.Hullar
TEMinor
LB High-frequency dynamics of regularly discharging canal afferents provide a linear signal for angular vestibulo-ocular reflexes.
J Neurophysiol 1999;822000- 2005
PubMedGoogle Scholar 10.Crane
BTDemer
JL Human horizontal vestibulo-ocular reflex initiation: effects of acceleration, target distance, and unilateral deafferentation.
J Neurophysiol 1998;801151- 1166
PubMedGoogle Scholar 11.Tabak
SCollewijn
HBoumans
LJvan der Steen
J Gain and delay of human vestibulo-ocular reflexes to oscillation and steps of the head by a reactive torque helmet, II: vestibular-deficient subjects.
Acta Otolaryngol 1997;117796- 809
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 13.Aw
STFetter
MCremer
PDKarlberg
MHalmagyi
GM Individual semicircular canal function in superior and inferior vestibular neuritis.
Neurology 2001;57768- 774
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 14.Halmagyi
GMBlack
RAThurtell
MJCurthoys
IS The human horizontal vestibulo-ocular reflex in response to active and passive head impulses after unilateral vestibular deafferentation.
Ann N Y Acad Sci 2003;1004325- 336
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 15.Roy
JECullen
KE Vestibulo-ocular reflex signal modulation during voluntary and passive head movements
J Neurophysiol 2002;872337- 2357
PubMedGoogle Scholar 16.Gezeck
SFischer
BTimmer
J Saccadic reaction times: a statistical analysis of multimodal distributions.
Vision Res 1997;372119- 2131
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref