Background
It remains unknown whether depression and apolipoprotein E genotype are risk factors for incident mild cognitive impairment (MCI).
Objective
To determine whether elderly individuals with depression (measured by the short Geriatric Depression Scale) are at increased risk of developing incident MCI.
Design
Prospective cohort study.
Setting
Primary care clinic.
Participants
A cohort of 840 cognitively normal elderly subjects without depression at recruitment who were followed up prospectively for a median of 3.5 years (range, 0.4-12.8 years). Subjects who developed depression (score of ≥6 on the short Geriatric Depression Scale; depression cohort) were compared with all remaining subjects (referent cohort).
Main Outcome Measures
Incidence of MCI (primary outcome) and incidence of MCI or dementia (composite secondary outcome).
Results
Individuals in the depression cohort were at significantly increased risk of subsequent incident MCI (hazard ratio [HR], 2.2; 95% confidence interval [CI], 1.2-4.1) after adjusting for age (time scale), sex, and education, and considering dementia as a competing outcome. The association was stronger in men but did not vary by severity of depression. We observed a synergistic interaction between apolipoprotein E genotype (ε3/ε4 or ε4/ε4) and depression (joint effect HR, 5.1; 95% CI, 1.9-13.6; test for additive interaction, P = .03). We found a similar association between depression and the subsequent composite outcome of incident MCI or dementia (HR, 2.6; 95% CI, 1.6-4.3).
Conclusions
Cognitively normal elderly individuals who develop depression are at increased risk of subsequent MCI. We found a synergistic interaction between depression and apolipoprotein E genotype.
The cognitive spectrum, ranging from normal aging to dementia, has increasingly become the focus of contemporary research on aging and dementia. Various terms are used to describe this entity, including mild cognitive impairment (MCI).1-4 Although much progress has been made in characterizing the cognitive aspects of MCI, there has been less work focusing on the neuropsychiatric aspects.5-7 In a recent cohort study,8,9 depression (as measured by the Geriatric Depression Scale [GDS]) doubled the risk of progressing from MCI to dementia.8,9 However, it remains unknown whether depression is also a risk factor for MCI. Similarly, apolipoprotein E (APOE) genotype is a risk factor for Alzheimer disease10,11; however, its role in MCI remains unknown. We tested the hypothesis that elderly individuals with depression as measured by the short GDS are at increased risk of developing incident MCI.12,13
Normal elderly persons have been recruited by the Mayo Alzheimer Disease Patient Registry for longitudinal studies of cognitive aging since 1986. This cohort has been described in detail in previous publications.2,14 All subjects were residents of Olmsted County, Minnesota, and were receiving their routine medical care at the Division of Primary Care Medicine of the Mayo Clinic, Rochester, Minn. Potential normal individuals were referred to Alzheimer Disease Patient Registry personnel by primary care physicians. On referral to the Alzheimer Disease Patient Registry, each research participant was thoroughly evaluated by one of several behavioral neurologists (primarily R.C.P., D.S.K., and B.F.B.), who obtained a medical history from the participant and from an informant, completed the short test of mental status examination15 and the Hachinski Ischemic Scale,16 and performed a neurologic examination. Study personnel obtained other data, including the Record of Independent Living,17 the short version of the GDS,12 and additional family history information. Laboratory studies included standard hematologic tests; vitamin B12, folic acid, and sensitive thyroid stimulating hormone levels; and syphilis serologic testing. All participants underwent brain imaging (computed tomography or magnetic resonance imaging).
An extensive neuropsychological battery was administered on enrollment, and a briefer version was administered at subsequent follow-up visits every 12 to 18 months.2 These tests included the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Revised, the Wechsler Memory Scale–Revised, the Auditory Verbal Learning Test, and the Wide-Range Achievement Test–III.18 Additional tests were the Mini-Mental State Examination,19 the Dementia Rating Scale,20 the Free and Cued Selective Reminding Task,21 the Boston Naming Test,22 the Controlled Oral Word Association Test,23 and the category fluency procedures.24 At the completion of the clinical, neuropsychological, and neuroimaging evaluations, a consensus meeting was held involving behavioral neurologists, a geriatrician, neuropsychologists, nurses, and other study personnel who had evaluated the participant. The consensus team also assigned a Clinical Dementia Rating25 score and a Global Deterioration Scale26 score.
Normal cognitive status was defined using the published criteria of the Mayo's Older American Normative Studies (MOANS)27 and required normal functioning in the community and no cognitive impairment. We excluded persons with a neurologic or psychiatric condition judged to interfere with cognitive assessment at the time of recruitment, such as depression. However, a history of depression was not an exclusion criterion. We also excluded persons receiving psychoactive medications in amounts that could compromise cognition. By contrast, persons with comorbid illnesses such as hypertension and coronary artery disease and those receiving medications for these disorders were included. However, if the primary care physician considered these illnesses or their treatments to interfere with cognitive functions, the persons were excluded.
Participants were followed up longitudinally with repeated visits every 12 to 18 months. The follow-up visits included a clinical evaluation and the administration of an abbreviated neuropsychological battery.2 Depression was assessed by using the short version of the GDS (15 items).12,13 Study participants who developed depression (score of ≥6 on the GDS at least once) before reaching one of the outcomes of interest (MCI or dementia) were considered to be in the depression cohort starting on the date of the testing. All remaining participants who never developed depression (as per GDS score) were considered to be in the referent cohort. The depression and referent cohorts were followed up longitudinally to study the incidence of MCI. In addition, for each person in the study we obtained a history of depression preceding enrollment in the study. This historical information was obtained using the medical records–linkage system of the Rochester Epidemiology Project. Details about the system were reported elsewhere.28
Blood was drawn from the study participants after receiving informed consent. DNA was amplified by means of polymerase chain reaction, and APOE genotyping was determined by standard methods,29 as reported in detail elsewhere.14 The genotypes were determined by laboratory technicians who were kept unaware of clinical characteristics (eg, presence of depression or MCI).
The primary outcome was MCI. The adjudicating team diagnosed MCI on the basis of psychometric data collected during the follow-up visits and according to the criteria of Petersen et al2 (memory complaint, normal activities of daily living, normal general cognitive function, abnormal memory for age, and not demented). Because the follow-up visits occurred only every 12 to 18 months, a participant could develop dementia without any evidence of transition through an MCI status. Therefore, we considered as a secondary outcome measure the composite event of incident MCI or dementia. The adjudicating team diagnosed dementia according to Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Revised Third Edition criteria.30
For survival analyses, baseline was the date of enrollment in the study for the referent cohort and the date on which depression was first measured (GDS score ≥6) for the depression cohort. Subjects were censored at death, loss to follow-up, or the end of the study (whichever came first). In analyses for MCI, dementia was considered a competing outcome.31,32 The association between the independent variable (depression measured by GDS) and the outcome of incident MCI was assessed using Cox proportional hazards models adjusted for sex and education (>12 vs ≤12 years). For more stringent age adjustment, age was used as the time scale in the survival analyses.31 However, graphic display of cumulative incidence curves was based on the more conventional time-to-event scale (accounting for competing outcomes). The same type of analysis was conducted for the composite event of incident MCI or dementia (secondary analysis). Stratified analyses were conducted for men and women separately and by level of severity of depression (GDS scores of 6 and 7-15 vs 0-5). Possible interaction effects with APOE genotype were examined using multivariate models to test for both multiplicative and additive interactions. This same method was also used to test for interaction between newly developed depression as measured by the GDS and a history of depression preceding enrollment in the study (preceding baseline). In addition, we compared the distribution of the individual GDS items in depressed subjects who developed MCI or dementia and in those who did not, to identify the most predictive items.
Statistical testing was conducted at the conventional 2-tailed α level of .05. Analyses were conducted using SAS (SAS Institute Inc, Cary, NC) and S-Plus (Insightful Corp, Seattle, Wash) statistical software.
Longitudinal data were available for 840 participants, who were free of both cognitive impairment and depression at the time of enrollment into the study and were followed up for a median of 3.5 years (range, 0.4-12.8 years). A total of 143 participants (17.0%) developed depression during follow-up (depression cohort), while 697 (83.0%) did not (referent cohort). Of the 143 subjects in the depression cohort, 68 (47.6%) had a GDS score of 6, 72 (50.4%) had scores of 7 to 11, and 3 (2.1%) had scores of 12 to 15. The baseline characteristics of the depression and referent cohorts are summarized in Table 1. The depression cohort tended to be older than the referent cohort; however, this imbalance was controlled by using age as the time scale when modeling the survival data.
Of the 143 subjects with depression, 17 (13.3%) developed MCI, whereas of the 697 referent subjects, only 33 (4.9%) developed MCI. Figure 1 shows cumulative incidence curves (using the conventional time-to-event scale and accounting for competing outcomes). The cumulative incidence curves for the depression and referent groups remained divergent throughout the follow-up for both the primary and secondary outcomes. The hazard ratio (HR) for MCI was 2.2 (95% confidence interval [CI], 1.2-4.1), after controlling for age (time scale), education, and sex, and considering dementia as a competing outcome. We also found a significant difference for the secondary outcome of incident MCI or dementia, with frequencies of 22.4% (32/143) for the depression cohort and 7.2% (50/697) for the referent cohort (HR, 2.6; 95% CI, 1.6-4.3).
We conducted analyses for the interaction between newly developed depression and history of depression by using subjects who did not develop depression during the study and did not experience depression before the study as the reference group. Compared with the reference group, subjects who developed depression but did not have a history of depression had an HR of 4.5 (95% CI, 1.9-10.9) for MCI, subjects who did not develop depression but had a history of depression before the study had an HR of 2.9 (95% CI, 1.5-5.8), and subjects with the joint effect of newly developed depression and a history of depression had an HR of 2.6 (95% CI, 1.1-6.3) (data not shown). A test for multiplicative interaction was significant (P = .008; antagonistic interaction), whereas a test for additive interaction was not (P = .3).
Persons with depression (GDS score ≥6) who subsequently developed MCI or dementia complained of “memory problems” more frequently (item 10 of the GDS; P = .04) and declared being “full of energy” less frequently (item 13; P = .04) than those who remained cognitively normal. All other items of the GDS were not significantly different between the 2 groups.
The association between depression and MCI was stronger in men (HR, 4.5; 95% CI, 1.8-11.3) than in women (HR, 1.5; 95% CI, 0.7-3.6); however, the difference was not statistically significant. The cumulative incidence of MCI did not increase with increasing severity of depression (GDS scores of 6 and 7-15 vs 0-5; data not shown). In addition, we found no significant difference in the duration of depressive symptoms between depressed subjects who developed incident MCI or dementia (median, 2.4 years; range, 0.6-8.2 years) and those who did not (median, 2.8 years; range, 0.4-12.3 years; P = .2). The association between depression and MCI increased when APOE genotype was included in the model (ε3/ε4, ε43/ε4, or otherwise). The HR for depression was 2.3 (95% CI, 1.2-4.3); for APOE ε4/ε4, 4.4 (95% CI, 1.0-19.0); and for APOE ε3/ε4, 1.7 (95% CI, 0.9-3.2).
Of 14 subjects with the ε4/ε4 genotype, 2 (14.3%) developed MCI and 1 (7.1%) dementia. Of 188 subjects with the ε3/ε4 genotype, 14 (7.4%) developed MCI and 15 (8.0%) dementia. Of 637 subjects with the ε3/ε3 or other rarer genotypes, 34 (5.3%) developed MCI and 31 (4.9%) dementia. We conducted analyses for interaction using subjects who did not carry either the ε3/ε4 or ε4/ε4 genotype and who did not have depression as the reference group. Compared with the reference group, subjects with depression but no ε3/ε4 or ε4/ε4 had an HR for MCI of 1.9 (95% CI, 0.9-4.1), subjects without depression but with ε3/ε4 or ε4/ε4 had an HR of 1.6 (95% CI, 0.8-3.4), and subjects with the joint effect of depression and ε3/ε4 or ε4/ε4 had an HR of 5.1 (95% CI, 1.9-13.6) (Table 2). A test for additive interaction was significant (P = .03; synergistic interaction), whereas a test for multiplicative interaction was not.
In this prospective cohort study, depression more than doubled the risk of transition from normal cognitive aging to incident MCI after controlling for age (time scale), education, and sex. The risk was similarly increased when the composite outcome of incident MCI or dementia was considered. The association was stronger in men than women, but did not vary by severity of depression. It is surprising that there was no relationship between the severity of depressive symptoms and the risk of MCI. However, most of the subjects who developed depression in this cohort had mild or moderate depression (GDS scores of 6 or 7-11). In addition, the GDS may be inadequate to measure severity because the score increases by the number of items endorsed rather than by the severity of the items.
We found an antagonistic (negative) interaction between depression newly developed during the study and history of depression preceding the study. Subjects who developed depression during the study and also had a history of depression had a smaller risk of MCI than those who developed depression but had no history of depression. This pattern of interaction may suggest a protective effect of antidepressant medications used before recruitment by subjects with a history of depression. Unfortunately, we could not test this hypothesis because we did not collect data on antidepressant medications used either before or during the study.
We also observed a synergistic (positive) interaction between APOE genotype and depression: the joint effect of depression and APOE genotype was significantly greater than the sum of the independent effects of the 2 factors. We propose the following 4 alternative mechanisms that may account for the observed association between depression (GDS score ≥6) and incident MCI (Figure 2).
Causal factor. Depression is in the chain of causality leading to MCI by virtue of its effect on the neuroendocrine axis.33 An important implication of this hypothesis is that an intervention targeting depression may lead to primary prevention of MCI.
Shared risk factor or confounding. Depression is noncausally associated with MCI because a third factor leads to the genesis of both MCI and depression. The third factor (confounder) could be genetic, environmental, or both.
Reverse causality. First, a person who is experiencing some degree of cognitive decline may develop depression as a reaction to the symptoms. Second, depressive symptoms may be an early noncognitive manifestation of dementia. Third, depression may “unmask” the clinical manifestations of MCI in individuals with limited cognitive reserve.34
Interaction. Depression leads to cognitive decline only in the presence of a genetic susceptibility factor or another risk factor. We found evidence of a synergistic interaction between APOE genotype and depression.
These 4 mechanisms (and other possible mechanisms) are not mutually exclusive and may act in combination.
The individual items of the GDS that were significantly more endorsed by depressed subjects destined to later develop MCI or dementia were items 10 (memory) and 13 (energy). These 2 items refer to premonitory symptoms of later cognitive decline or illness in general rather than to mood symptoms per se.
Few prospective studies have investigated the association between depression and dementia, and the results of these studies have been inconsistent.35-39 To our knowledge, ours is one of the first studies to investigate the association between depression and incident MCI as measured by the criteria of Petersen et al.2 Our secondary outcome of incident MCI or dementia is comparable to the outcome used in the Religious Orders Study35 and in the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging.36 In those studies, subjects with depression were more likely than referent subjects to become demented or to experience cognitive decline.35,36
There are several strengths to our study. First, the longitudinal follow-up of a large cohort of elderly individuals free of both cognitive impairment and depression on enrollment enabled us to observe the association between the independent variable (depression) and the primary outcome of incident MCI. Second, the frequency of follow-up visits was high (every 12-18 months). This allowed us to detect depression and MCI or dementia near the time of their onset (incident events). Third, we adjusted for the difference in age at baseline between depressed and referent subjects by using age as the time scale in survival analyses.31 Traditional time-to-event survival analyses were also conducted for comparison and showed similar results (sensitivity analyses; data not shown).31 Fourth, available genotyping data allowed for the exploration of a possible interaction between APOE genotype and depression.
Our study also had limitations. First, we did not use a structured interview in the assessment of depression, and we did not collect information about treatments for depression. We measured depression using the short version of the GDS (15 items) rather than the original long version (30 items).9,12,13 However, the short version has been validated and shown to be comparable to the long version.13 Second, because of our stringent eligibility criteria, the subjects included in our sample may have been healthier than a probability (random) sample of elderly individuals residing in the community. This may have reduced the external validity of our study. A future population-based study can rectify this weakness. Third, despite the high frequency of follow-up visits, we may have missed individuals who transitioned from normal cognition to dementia between 2 visits. To account for these incomplete assessments, we assumed that all subjects who developed dementia went through an MCI stage, and we used a composite secondary outcome measure of MCI or dementia.
Our study suggests that cognitively normal elderly individuals who develop depression are at increased risk of subsequent MCI. In addition, there may be a synergistic interaction between APOE genotype and depression.
Correspondence: Yonas E. Geda, MD, Department of Psychiatry, Mayo Clinic, 4500 San Pablo Rd, Jacksonville, FL 32224 (geda.yonas@mayo.edu).
Accepted for Publication: November 1, 2005.
Author Contributions:Study concept and design: Geda, Petersen, and Rocca. Acquisition of data: Geda, Knopman, Jicha, Smith, Boeve, and Ivnik. Analysis and interpretation of data: Geda, Knopman, Smith, Negash, Boeve, Petersen, Pankratz, and Rocca. Drafting of the manuscript: Geda, Knopman, Petersen, and Rocca. Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Geda, Knopman, Mrazek, Jicha, Smith, Negash, Boeve, Ivnik, Petersen, Pankratz, and Rocca. Statistical analysis: Pankratz, Geda, and Rocca. Obtained funding: Geda and Petersen. Administrative, technical, and material support: Smith, Boeve, Ivnik, and Petersen. Study supervision: Knopman, Petersen, and Rocca.
Funding/Support: This study was supported by grant K01 MH68351 from the National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, Md, and in part by grants U01 AG06786 (Mayo Alzheimer's Disease Patient Registry) and P50 AG16574 (Mayo Alzheimer's Disease Research Center) from the National Institute on Aging, Bethesda. Dr Rocca was partly funded by grant R01 NS33978 from the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke, Bethesda.
Acknowledgment: We thank Karen Tennison for her secretarial assistance.
1.Reisberg
BFerris
SHde Leon
MJ
et al. Stage-specific behavioral, cognitive, and in vivo changes in community residing subjects with age-associated memory impairment and primary degenerative dementia of the Alzheimer type.
Drug Dev Res 1988;15101- 114
Google ScholarCrossref 2.Petersen
RCSmith
GEWaring
SCIvnik
RJTangalos
EGKokmen
E Mild cognitive impairment: clinical characterization and outcome.
Arch Neurol199956303308
[published correction appears in
Arch Neurol. 1999;56:760]
PubMedGoogle Scholar 3.Crook
TBartus
RTFerris
SHWhitehouse
PCohen
GDGershon
S Age-associated memory impairment: proposed diagnostic criteria and measures of clinical change—report of a National Institute of Mental Health Work Group.
Dev Neuropsychol 1986;2261- 276
Google ScholarCrossref 4.Levy
RWorking Party of the International Psychogeriatric Association in collaboration with the World Health Organization, Aging-associated cognitive decline.
Int Psychogeriatr 1994;663- 68
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 5.Lyketsos
CGLopez
OJones
BFitzpatrick
ALBreitner
JDeKosky
S Prevalence of neuropsychiatric symptoms in dementia and mild cognitive impairment: results from the Cardiovascular Health Study.
JAMA 2002;2881475- 1483
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 6.Geda
YESmith
GEKnopman
DS
et al. De novo genesis of neuropsychiatric symptoms in mild cognitive impairment (MCI).
Int Psychogeriatr 2004;1651- 60
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 7.Hwang
TJMasterman
DLOrtiz
FFairbanks
LACummings
JL Mild cognitive impairment is associated with characteristic neuropsychiatric symptoms.
Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 2004;1817- 21
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 8.Modrego
PJFerrandez
J Depression in patients with mild cognitive impairment increases the risk of developing dementia of Alzheimer type: a prospective cohort study.
Arch Neurol 2004;611290- 1293
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 9.Yesavage
JABrink
TLRose
TL
et al. Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale: a preliminary report.
J Psychiatr Res 1982-83;1737- 49
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 10.National Institute on Aging/Alzheimer's Association Work Group, Apolipoprotein E genotyping in Alzheimer's disease.
Lancet 1996;3471091- 1095
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 11.Devanand
DPPelton
GHZamora
D
et al. Predictive utility of apolipoprotein E genotype for Alzheimer disease in outpatients with mild cognitive impairment.
Arch Neurol 2005;62975- 980
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 13.Sheikh
JIYesavage
JA Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS): recent evidence and development of a shorter version.
Clin Gerontol 1986;5165- 173
Google ScholarCrossref 14.Petersen
RCSmith
GEIvnik
RJ
et al Apolipoprotein E status as a predictor of the development of Alzheimer's disease in memory-impaired individuals.
JAMA199527312741278
[published correction appears in
JAMA. 1995;274:538]
PubMedGoogle Scholar 15.Kokmen
ENaessens
JMOfford
KP A short test of mental status: description and preliminary results.
Mayo Clin Proc 1987;62281- 288
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 16.Rosen
WGTerry
RDFuld
PAKatzman
RPeck
A Pathological verification of ischemic score in differentiation of dementias.
Ann Neurol 1980;7486- 488
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 17.Weintraub
S The record of independent living. An informant-completed measure of activities of daily living and behavior in elderly patients with cognitive impairment.
Am J Alzheimers Care Relat Disord 1986;235- 39
Google ScholarCrossref 18.Lezak
MD Neuropsychological Assessment. 3rd ed. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 1995
19.Folstein
MFFolstein
SEMcHugh
PR “Mini-mental state”: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician.
J Psychiatr Res 1975;12189- 198
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 20.Mattis
S Dementia Rating Scale: Professional Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources Inc; 1988
22.Kaplan
EGoodglass
HWeintraub
S The Boston Naming Test. Philadelphia, Pa: Lea & Febiger; 1983
23.Benton
ASivan
ABHamsher
KdeSVarney
NRSpreen
O Contributions to Neuropsychological Assessment: A Clinical Manual. 2nd ed. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 1994
24.Monsch
AUBondi
MWButters
N
et al. A comparison of category and letter fluency in Alzheimer's disease and Huntington's disease.
Neuropsychology 1994;825- 30
Google ScholarCrossref 26.Reisberg
BFerris
SHde Leon
MJCrook
T The Global Deterioration Scale for assessment of primary degenerative dementia.
Am J Psychiatry 1982;1391136- 1139
PubMedGoogle Scholar 27.Ivnik
RJMalec
JFSmith
GE
et al. Mayo's Older Americans Normative Studies: WAIS–R norms for ages 56 to 97.
Clin Neuropsychol 1992;6(suppl)1- 30
Google ScholarCrossref 29.Hixson
JEVernier
DT Restriction isotyping of human apolipoprotein E by gene amplification and cleavage with
HhAl.
J Lipid Res 1990;31545- 548
PubMedGoogle Scholar 30.American Psychiatric Association Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Revised Third Edition. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press; 1987
31.Therneau
TMGrambsch
PM Modeling Survival Data: Extending the Cox Model. New York, NY: Springer-Verlag; 2000
34.Jorm
AFvan Duijn
CMChandra
V
et al. EURODEM Risk Factors Research Group, Psychiatric history and related exposures as risk factors for Alzheimer's disease: a collaborative re-analysis of case-control studies.
Int J Epidemiol 1991;20(Suppl 2)S43- S47
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 35.Wilson
RSBarnes
LLMendes de Leon
CF
et al. Depressive symptoms, cognitive decline, and risk of AD in older persons.
Neurology 2002;59364- 370
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 36.Dal Forno
GPalermo
MTDonoghue
JEKaragiozis
HZonderman
ABKawas
CH Depressive symptoms, sex, and risk for Alzheimer's disease.
Ann Neurol 2005;57381- 387
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 37.Bassuk
SSBerkman
LFWypij
D Depressive symptomatology and incident cognitive decline in an elderly community sample.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 1998;551073- 1081
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 38.Chen
PGanguli
MMulsant
BHDeKosky
ST The temporal relationship between depressive symptoms and dementia: a community-based prospective study.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 1999;56261- 266
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 39.Devanand
DPSano
MTang
MX
et al. Depressed mood and the incidence of Alzheimer's disease in the elderly living in the community.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 1996;53175- 182
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref