Background
Neuropsychologic testing is often used to infer neuropathologic processes, but clinicopathologic correlations for individual cognitive measures are based on a small number of published studies.
Objective
To examine the usefulness of the age- and education-adjusted Mayo Cognitive Factor Scales (MCFS) obtained at participants' initial assessments for predicting the presence or absence of pathologic Alzheimer disease (AD).
Design
This was a longitudinal study of a cohort of elderly patients with and without cognitive complaints who were followed up until death. Mayo Cognitive Factor Scales age- and education-adjusted standard scores from the participants' initial evaluations were used to calculate classification accuracy statistics for neuropathologic AD diagnosis obtained approximately 6 years after testing. Subjects with non-AD diagnoses or substantial non-AD-related changes were excluded from the study.
Setting
Academic medical center.
Participants
One hundred two participants were evaluated clinically and underwent neuropathologic examination at autopsy. All were part of the Mayo Clinic Alzheimer’s Disease Patient Registry or Alzheimer Disease Research Center.
Results
All Mayo Cognitive Factor Scale scores were significantly correlated with AD criteria. Logistic regression modeling including Mayo Cognitive Factor Scales Verbal Comprehension and Retention indices revealed high positive predictive value with moderate sensitivity and specificity for pathologic AD.
Conclusion
Neuropsychologic test scores at initial evaluations were predictive of pathologic AD.
Neuropsychologic testing is widely used in the clinical evaluation of patients with suspected dementia. Extensive data exists on the relationship between neuropsychologic test results and the clinical diagnoses of neurologic disease,1-10 but fewer studies have explored the predictive value of cognitive measures for autopsy-proved cases11-13 or for specific neurologic diseases.14 The validity of diagnostic neuropsychologic assessment is based on its association with neuropathologic findings.
During the past 13 years, we have performed prospective clinical evaluations that included neuropsychologic testing and have encouraged patients to consent to autopsy at death. More than 400 patients who underwent at least 1 clinical evaluation have been examined post mortem, making it possible for us to address the question: How well do neuropsychologic tests predict pathologic Alzheimer disease (AD)?
Although neuropathologic diagnoses are considered the standard against which all antemortem predictors must be considered, important challenges complicate such analyses. First, the lag between initial neuropsychologic testing and death may be many years; intervening events may invalidate attempts to correlate neuropsychologic results with neuropathologic findings. Second, neuropathologic examination of patients thought to have uncomplicated AD often reveals other abnormal findings (eg, Lewy bodies) that confound the correlation between cognitive changes and AD-related changes. We limited the effect of these 2 issues by focusing on patients with dementia who from initial evaluation to death were free of other illnesses, in particular, obvious brain diseases other than clinical AD. Third, we restricted our analyses to subjects who had either no neurologic disease at autopsy or who had AD-related changes. These restrictions enabled us to explore the relationship between AD and cognitive change without the confounding effects of other neurologic diseases. Our primary objective was to examine the usefulness of the age- and education-adjusted Mayo Cognitive Factor Scales (MCFS)15 obtained at participants' initial assessments for predicting the presence or absence of pathologic AD. Although using data obtained closer to autopsy (eg, final psychometric evaluation) would likely improve the association between cognitive performance and neuropathologic disease, data from patients' initial evaluations were selected as predictors to mirror the clinical situation as to when neuropsychologic evaluations may be of most clinical benefit (eg, enabling early diagnosis).
The Mayo Clinic Institutional Review Board approved this study. All participants were enrolled in the Mayo Clinic Alzheimer’s Disease Patient Registry or Alzheimer Disease Research Center. Participants were included in this study if they received a neuropsychologic assessment, a clinical diagnosis was made ante mortem, and neuropathologic examination was performed at autopsy.
Subjects with and without cognitive complaints were recruited from the Mayo Clinic Community Internal Medicine Department or the Department of Neurology. At the initial evaluation, each subject was seen by a study physician (D.S.K., B.F.B., or R.C.P.) who performed a neurologic examination and obtained a complete history. The neuropsychologic battery of tests was administered. All subjects with cognitive impairment and most healthy subjects underwent magnetic resonance imaging unless contraindicated, in which case they underwent computed tomography. Screening laboratory studies were also performed. At the completion of the assessment, evaluations were reviewed in a consensus conference consisting of neurologists (D.S.K., B.F.B., and R.C.P.), neuropsychologists (G.E.S. and R.J.I.), nurses, and geriatricians. A final clinical diagnosis was made for that visit based on all available information. Final clinical diagnoses of dementia were consistent with Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Revised Third Edition criteria16 and later with Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition17 criteria. Diagnoses of AD fulfilled National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Diseases and Stroke/Alzheimer's Disease and Related Disorders Association (NINCDS/ADRDA) criteria for AD.18 Subjects were seen multiple times and clinical diagnoses were made at each evaluation.
Participants were included in the clinical diagnosis group if a diagnosis of AD or normal was made at the last antemortem consensus meeting. Those having clinical diagnoses other than AD or normal were excluded from analyses. A neuropathologic diagnosis of AD was made in participants who met National Institute on Aging and the Reagan Institute Working Group19 criteria for intermediate or high likelihood of AD at autopsy, and a diagnosis of normal was made if they met National Institute on Aging and the Reagan Institute Working Group criteria for low likelihood of AD at autopsy. Clinical and pathologic groups were formed independent of one another; that is, a subject with a clinical diagnosis of normal may have had a neuropathologic diagnosis of AD. The mean interval between subjects' final clinical evaluation and pathologic diagnosis was less than 6 months. According to National Institute on Aging and the Reagan Institute Working Group criteria, 7 (7.4%) of 94 participants having a clinical diagnosis of normal were deemed to have intermediate or high likelihood for pathologic AD and 3 (3.2%) of 94 participants with clinical AD were found to have a low likelihood for pathologic AD at autopsy. As noted, subjects were excluded from the neuropathologic AD group if they were found to have significant involvement of any neuropathologic disorder other than AD. Data were available for 94 subjects having a clinical diagnosis and 102 subjects having a pathologic diagnosis. The sample size varied across groups because of missing cognitive data.
The MCFS age- and education-adjusted standard scores15 obtained during the initial cognitive evaluation were the primary independent variables used to differentiate between pathologically confirmed AD and normal status. The MCFS indices are derived from confirmatory factor analysis of concurrent administration in a large sample of healthy subjects of well-established cognitive instruments, including the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Revised,20 the Wechsler Memory Scale–Revised,21 and the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test.22 Five MCFS factors best described the cognitive abilities assessed by these instruments: Verbal Comprehension, Perceptual Organization, Attention/Concentration, Learning, and Retention.15 We also included the Mattis Dementia Rating Scale23 as a measure of overall severity of dementia.
Participants underwent, on average, 5 or 6 cognitive evaluations, and clinical diagnoses were made after each assessment. We report descriptive data for the study's primary independent variables for both clinical and pathologic AD. We restricted the predictive statistics by addressing only the MCFS score relationship with pathologic AD; however, this is the criterion standard and, unlike clinical diagnosis, is not influenced by the initial neuropsychologic evaluation.
Descriptive data are given for participants according to their last antemortem clinical diagnosis (normal vs AD) and their neuropathologic diagnosis (normal vs AD). As given in Table 1, age at death, age at testing, interval between testing and death, years of education, and sex composition did not differ for subjects with clinical or pathologic AD relative to their normal comparison groups. As expected, patients with clinical or pathologic AD scored lower on the Mattis Dementia Rating Scale, although their mean scores reveal their relatively mild clinical status during the initial evaluation. Group comparisons on all MCFS scores were also significant, with subjects with AD scoring lower on all variables (Table 2). Interval between initial testing and death did not differ for participants having a diagnosis of AD or normal.
Forward selection logistic regression models using age- and education-adjusted MCFS scores from initial evaluation and interval to predict pathologic AD determined approximately 6 years after the initial testing was significant (χ2 = 27.7, P<.001) and included interval along with the MCFS Verbal Comprehension and Retention factors. Overall accuracy was 74.7%, sensitivity was 75.0%, and specificity was 73.9%. Positive and negative predictive values were 89.5% and 50.0%, respectively.
This study examined the relationship between MCFS scores and AD-related changes by using scores obtained from patients' initial cognitive evaluations to predict a neuropathologic diagnosis of AD. The MCFS scores differed substantially between AD and normal groups determined clinically or pathologically. More important, MCFS scores had moderately strong predictive value for neuropathologic AD (relative to pathologically confirmed normal status) despite an approximate 6-year interval between the initial assessment and death. These findings provide additional support for the moderately strong relationship between cognitive status and AD neuropathologic changes24,25 and for the predictive utility of various MCFS indices.
In addition, the results provide additional evidence for the predictive value of language-based and memory retention measures for identifying persons who have pathologically confirmed AD. The predictive value of a memory retention measure was not surprising because the strong relationship between memory and AD is well documented.4,10,26,27 Initial learning and memory deficits are a hallmark clinical feature of AD and are functionally associated with the early hippocampal lesions observed in the typical progression of neurofibrillary tangle pathology.28
The association of the Verbal Comprehension factor and pathologic AD, however, was more surprising, especially given previous findings that the MCFS Verbal Comprehension index was the least sensitive factor score for clinical AD.4 In retrospect, the Verbal Comprehension predictive power may relate to isocortical disease present in our pathologic AD group that was not present in our pathologic normal group. Even during initial testing (≈6 years before pathologic diagnosis), participants composing our pathologic AD group may have had significant isocortical disease (eg, Braak stage IV or higher), negatively affecting their performance on measures more resistant to neurologic disease (eg, overlearned verbal information). Those confirmed to be pathologically normal would not have had isocortical disease during initial assessment.
Other research has demonstrated a link between performance on memory- and language-based tasks in patients with AD with relatively greater pathologic findings in the temporal lobe than in other brain regions.29 Our selection criteria may have increased the odds that our subjects with AD had relatively greater temporal lobe disease compared with that in other brain regions because we excluded patients with coexisting non-AD-related disorders. This method may have inflated the predictive value of memory and language tests for AD diagnosis.
The exclusion of participants with non-AD-related disorders from our analyses reduced the generalizability of our results to clinical practice. In the clinic, we do not have the luxury of evaluating only patients with circumscribed or isolated neuropathologic findings, because many are found to have an admixture of pathologic changes. Our primary goal, however, was to establish the existence of the MCFS predictive power for the presence of AD-related changes; that is, we sought to establish the MCFS as a sensitive marker rather than a specific marker for AD. It was reasonable to eliminate other neuropathologic diseases from analysis. We also acknowledge that the high base rate of AD in this investigation increased the positive predictive value and diminished the negative predictive value of the MCFS scores.30 Our base rate, however, may reasonably correspond to the base rate of AD in subjects undergoing neuropsychologic evaluation because of memory complaints.
Studying the diagnostic validity of our clinical instruments not only strengthens our trust in measures that prove to have a strong relationship with well-defined, objective criteria5 but corrects misperceptions regarding the diagnostic usefulness of other measures that contribute little to the diagnostic process.7 Ideally, this type of data will help guide clinicians' test selection and improve diagnostic accuracy in clinical practice.
Correspondence: Glenn E. Smith, PhD, Department of Psychiatry and Psychology (W-11), Mayo Clinic, Rochester, MN 55905 (smith.glenn@mayo.edu).
Accepted for Publication: December 21, 2005.
Author Contributions:Study concept and design: Powell, Smith, Knopman, and Petersen. Acquisition of data: Smith, Knopman, Parisi, Boeve, and Ivnik. Analysis and interpretation of data: Powell, Smith, Knopman, Parisi, Boeve, and Petersen. Drafting of the manuscript: Powell, Smith, Knopman, and Ivnik. Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Powell, Smith, Knopman, Parisi, Boeve, Petersen, and Ivnik. Statistical analysis: Powell and Smith. Obtained funding: Petersen. Administrative, technical, and material support: Knopman, Parisi, Boeve, and Ivnik. Study supervision: Smith, Knopman, Boeve, and Ivnik.
Funding/Support: This study was supported by grants P50-AG 16574 and U01-AG 06786 from the National Institute on Aging and by the Robert H. and Clarice Smith and Abigail Van Buren Alzheimer's Disease Research Program of the Mayo Foundation.
Acknowledgment: We thank the staff of the Mayo Abigail Van Buren Alzheimer's Disease Research Center for their work and all the participants for their willingness to support research.
1.Storandt
MBotwinick
JDanziger
WBerg
LHughes
C Psychometric differentiation of mild senile dementia of the Alzheimer type.
Arch Neurol 1984;41497- 499
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 2.Storandt
MHill
R Very mild senile dementia of the Alzheimer type, II: psychometric test performance.
Arch Neurol 1989;46383- 386
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 3.Tierney
MCSnow
WGReid
DWZorzitto
MLFisher
RH Psychometric differentiation of dementia: replication and extension of the findings of Storandt and coworkers.
Arch Neurol 1987;44720- 722
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 4.Ivnik
RJSmith
GECerhan
JHBoeve
BFTangalos
EGPetersen
RC Understanding the diagnostic capabilities of cognitive tests.
Clin Neuropsychol 2001;14114- 124
Google ScholarCrossref 5.Cerhan
JHIvnik
RJSmith
GETangalos
ECPetersen
RCBoeve
BF Diagnostic utility of letter fluency, category fluency, and fluency difference scores in Alzheimer's disease.
Clin Neuropsychol 2002;1635- 42
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 6.Monsch
AUBondi
MWButters
NSalmon
DPKatzman
RThal
LJ Comparisons of verbal fluency tasks in the detection of dementia of the Alzheimer type.
Arch Neurol 1992;491253- 1258
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 7.Testa
JAIvnik
RJBoeve
BF
et al. Confrontation naming does not add incremental diagnostic utility in MCI and Alzheimer's disease.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2004;10504- 512
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 8.Bondi
MWSalmon
DPGalasko
DThomas
RGThal
LJ Neuropsychological function and apolipoprotein E genotype in the preclinical detection of Alzheimer's disease.
Psychol Aging 1999;14295- 303
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 9.Jacobson
MWDelis
DCBondi
MWSalmon
DP Do neuropsychological tests detect preclinical Alzheimer's disease? individual-test versus cognitive-discrepancy score analyses.
Neuropsychology 2002;16132- 139
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 10.Tierney
MCSzalai
JPSnow
WG
et al. Prediction of probable Alzheimer's disease in memory-impaired patients: a prospective longitudinal study.
Neurology 1996;46661- 665
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 11.Connor
DJSalmon
DPSandy
TJGalasko
DHansen
LAThal
LJ Cognitive profiles of autopsy-confirmed Lewy body variant vs pure Alzheimer's disease.
Arch Neurol1998559941000
[published correction appears in
Arch Neurol. 1998;55:1352]
PubMedGoogle Scholar 12.Rascovsky
KSalmon
DPHo
GJ
et al. Cognitive profiles differ in autopsy-confirmed frontotemporal dementia and AD.
Neurology 2002;581801- 1808
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 13.Salmon
DPThomas
RGPay
MM
et al. Alzheimer's disease can be accurately diagnosed in very mildly impaired individuals.
Neurology 2002;591022- 1028
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 14.Gosche
KMMortimer
JASmith
CDMarkesbery
WRSnowdon
DA Hippocampal volume as an index of Alzheimer neuropathology: findings from the Nun Study.
Neurology 2002;581476- 1482
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 15.Smith
GEIvnik
RJMalec
JFPetersen
RCKokmen
ETangalos
EC The Mayo Cognitive Factor Scales (MCFS): derivation of a short battery and norms for factor scores.
Neuropsychologia 1994;8194- 202
Google ScholarCrossref 16.American Psychiatric Association Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Revised Third Edition. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 1987
17.American Psychiatric Association Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 1994
18.McKhann
GDrachman
DFolstein
MKatzman
RPrice
DStadlan
EM Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA work group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer's Disease.
Neurology 1984;34939- 944
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 19.Hyman
BTTrojanowski
JQ Consensus recommendations for the postmortem diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease from the National Institute on Aging and the Reagan Institute Working Group on diagnostic criteria for the neuropathological assessment of Alzheimer's disease.
J Neuropathol Exp Neurol 1997;561095- 1097
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 20.Wechsler
D Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised. New York, NY: Psychological Corp; 1981
21.Wechsler
D Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised. New York: Psychological Corp; 1987
22.Rey
A L'examen clinique en psychologie. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France; 1964
23.Mattis
S Mattis Dementia Rating Scale (MDRS). Odessa, Fla: Psychological Assessment Resources;1973
24.Grober
EDickson
DSliwinski
M
et al. Memory and mental status correlates of modified Braak staging.
Neurobiol Aging 1999;20573- 579
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 25.Riley
KPSnowdon
DAMarkesbery
WR Alzheimer's neurofibrillary pathology and the spectrum of cognitive function: findings from the Nun Study.
Ann Neurol 2002;51567- 577
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 26.Backman
LSmall
BFratiglioni
L Stability of the preclinical episodic memory deficit in Alzheimer's disease.
Brain 2001;12496- 102
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 27.Elias
MFBeiser
AWolf
PAAu
RWhite
RFD'Agostino
RB The preclinical phase of Alzheimer disease: a 22-year prospective study of the Framingham Cohort.
Arch Neurol 2000;57808- 813
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 29.Kanne
SMBalota
DAStorandt
MMcKeel
DW
JrMorris
JC Relating anatomy to function in Alzheimer's disease: neuropsychological profiles predict regional neuropathology 5 years later.
Neurology 1998;50979- 985
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 30.Smith
GCerhan
JIvnik
R Clinical Interpretation of the WAIS-III and the WMS-III. New York, NY: Academic Press;2003