Background
Neurochemical and pathologic studies show that mild cognitive impairment (MCI) is frequently a transitional state between normal aging and Alzheimer disease (AD). Neuropathologic sample sizes have been limited because relatively few individuals with MCI die before dementia develops. Decreased neocortical nicotinic receptor binding is characteristic of AD but has not been investigated in subjects with MCI.
Objective
To assess nicotinic receptor binding and pathologic differences in control subjects with no dementia (ND) and in subjects with clinically and pathologically described MCI or Alzheimer disease.
Design
This was a clinicopathologic analysis. Subjects with ND had no demonstrable cognitive or functional impairment. Subjects with MCI met Petersen clinical criteria for single- or multiple-domain amnestic MCI and died before the disorder progressed to AD. Subjects with AD met National Institute for Neurological Diseases and Stroke/Alzheimer’s Disease and Related Disorders Association clinical criteria for AD. All subjects underwent a complete diagnostic and semiquantitative neuropathologic examination. Data were examined after both clinical and histopathologic classification of subjects.
Setting
Sun Health Research Institute Brain Donation Program, and Arizona Alzheimer Disease Center.
Participants
Twenty-one control subjects with ND, 8 subjects with MCI, and 70 subjects with AD, prospectively followed up to autopsy.
Main Outcome Measures
Nicotinic acetylcholine receptor binding value, total tangle density, total plaque density, and Braak stage.
Results
At the last examination before death, subjects with AD were significantly younger, less educated, and more cognitively and globally impaired compared with subjects with ND. When categorized by clinical diagnosis, MCI was always intermediate between ND and AD. On the whole, MCI was pathologically intermediate between ND and AD for senile plaque density, neurofibrillary tangle density, and Braak stage, but some subjects with MCI lacked neuritic plaques entirely. Binding for nicotinic acetylcholine receptors did not differ between the ND and MCI groups, but it was significantly less in the AD group.
Conclusions
Most MCI may be considered a transitional state between ND and AD clinically and neuropathologically, but in some MCI cases there is lack of neuritic plaques, and, therefore, it cannot be considered early AD. Nicotinic receptor binding seems to be lost during the transition from MCI to AD.
Mild cognitive impairment (MCI) has been represented as a condition occurring between normal aging and Alzheimer disease (AD). While there continues to be debate about classification of the various subtypes of MCI, it is generally agreed that MCI involves some degree of cognitive impairment but of insufficient severity to constitute dementia.1 It has been widely considered a prodrome of AD,2 but a recent clinicopathologic study found that 19 (29%) of 67 subjects with MCI had different primary pathology.3
If we assume the clinical construct of MCI as a transitional state between normal aging and AD, the conclusion might be that MCI represents a transitional phase pathologically, radiologically, and neurochemically.4 For this reason, postmortem studies of MCI should subclassify subjects by histopathologic stage of AD rather than by clinical measures alone.
We investigated whether MCI is not only a transitional state between no dementia (ND) and AD clinically and pathologically but also whether it is a transitional state for nicotinic acetylcholine receptor (nAChR) loss. Further, we investigated whether there are clinical and pathologic differences among groups when they are subdivided by presence of histopathologic findings of AD.
The subjects with ND, MCI, and AD in the present study are participants in the Sun Health Research Institute Brain Donation Program. After institutional review board–approved consent is attained, subjects of the Sun Health Research Institute Brain Donation Program undergo complete medical, neurologic, and neuropsychologic assessments. All subjects included in the present study were prospectively characterized, diagnosed, and assessed during life and were followed up to autopsy.
Included were 70 subjects who met NINCDS-ADRDA (National Institute for Neurological Diseases and Stroke/Alzheimer’s Disease and Related Disorders Association) criteria for a clinical diagnosis of probable or possible AD,5 and National Institute on Aging–Reagan Institute Working Group on Diagnostic Criteria for the Neurological Assessment of Alzheimer's Disease (NIA-Reagan)6 postmortem criteria for intermediate or high probability of AD and classified by Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's Disease (CERAD)7 postmortem criteria. Subjects with a postmortem diagnosis of dementia with Lewy bodies according to the Consortium on Dementia with Lewy Bodies criteria were excluded.8 Age at onset of AD was ascertained by interview of the caregiver, dated to the earliest evidence of cognitive symptoms noted. Duration of disease was derived from subtracting age at onset of AD symptoms from age at death.
Subjects with MCI were selected from among 13 individuals with a clinical diagnosis of MCI. Of these 13 subjects with MCI, the 8 included in the study were diagnosed using Petersen clinical criteria9; all had subjective reports of memory loss, objective impairment of memory (≥1.5 SD on neuropsychologic tests of memory [Auditory Verbal Learning Test], and were without significant functional decline. All were categorized as having single- or multiple-domain amnestic MCI. The remaining 5 subjects with MCI were excluded because they had either a comorbid condition, such as parkinsonism, or nonamnestic MCI.
The 21 subjects in the ND group were defined as having no demonstrable cognitively based limitations on activities of daily living, including, when applicable, employment. Rigorous criteria were used to exclude any subject with any type of symptomatic or severe brain-related neurologic or psychiatric illness, for example, mental retardation, epilepsy, cerebral infarction or hemorrhage, multiple sclerosis, brain tumor, major depressive disorder (unipolar or bipolar), schizophrenia, traumatic brain injury, and substance abuse. This was done by prospective interview of the participants and careful scrutiny of their medical records.
Cognitive, functional, and global assessments
The mean ± SD interval between last cognitive assessment and death was 6.9 ± 4.3 months. Gross cognitive status was ascertained using the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE),10 functional assessment using the Functional Assessment Staging Scale (FAST),11 and global assessment using the Global Deterioration Scale (GDS).12,13
Neuropathologic examination
The mean postmortem interval was 2.8 hours. Paraffin blocks containing brain tissue were cut at 5-μm intervals and stained with hematoxylin-eosin for analysis. Additional paraffin sections containing tissue from the anterior cingulate gyrus, amygdala, entorhinal cortex, middle frontal gyrus, middle temporal gyrus, inferior parietal lobule, and anterior medulla14 were stained for immunohistochemical analysis of α-synuclein (LB509 monoclonal antibody) to identify Lewy bodies and Lewy body–related neurites. Sections from frozen blocks were stained with Campbell-Switzer, Gallyas,14 and thioflavine S methods to identify plaques, tangles, and other inclusions. Large 4 × 3-cm frozen sections containing tissue from the coronal planes through most of the frontal, temporal, parietal, and occipital lobes were stained with hematoxylin-eosin and Luxol fast blue to detect cerebral white matter rarefaction. Additional immunohistochemical procedures were used as needed, including those for ubiquitin to detect intraneuronal inclusions of motoneuron disease with dementia, and αB-crystallin and phosphorylated neurofilament to detect swollen neurons in corticobasal degeneration. For all stains except hematoxylin-eosin and Luxol fast blue, both positive and negative control sections were processed with every batch of slides.
Density of neuritic plaques was determined in the hippocampus, entorhinal cortex, temporal lobe, parietal lobe, and frontal lobe using CERAD criteria.7 Scoring was on a scale of 0 (none) to 3 (frequent), based on the highest score attained in any lobe. In addition, all plaques, both neuritic and diffuse, in the same 3 neocortical regions and in the hippocampus and entorhinal cortex, were rated in the same manner (termed “total plaque density”) except that scores were assigned as the perceived mean score for each area. Total scores for the 5 areas ranged from 0 to a possible maximum of 15. Neurofibrillary tangle density was calculated in a similar manner (termed “total tangle density”) and also using the Braak staging system.15
[3H](±)EPIBATIDINE BINDING
Midfrontal cortex (Brodmann areas 38, 39, and 46) was homogenized and assayed for [3H](±)epibatidine binding as previously described.16 In brief, nAChR levels were determined by incubating 0.75 to 2.5 mg of protein in 1 mL of assay buffer containing 5 nmol/L of [3H](±)epibatidine (45-65 Ci/mmole [1.7 = 2.4 GBq/mol]; New England Nuclear Corp, Boston, Mass). Nonspecific binding was determined in the presence of 1 μmol/L of unlabeled (±)epibatidine (RBI, Natick, Mass). Samples were incubated on ice for 2 hours, and the assay was terminated by rapid filtration through GF/C filters (Whatman, Florham Park, NJ), presoaked in 0.5% polyethylene amine for at least 1 hour, using a cell harvester (Brandell Instruments, Gaithersburg, Md), and radioactivity was quantified by liquid scintillation spectrometry (Tri-Carb 1600TR; Packard Instruments Co, Meriden, Conn). There were 3 replicates per experiment, with the data representing the average of 1 to 3 separate experiments per sample. Protein content in the samples was determined using a protein assay (BCA; Pierce Chemical Co, Rockford, Ill) with bovine serum albumin as the standard.
Comparison of means was done using 1-way analysis of variance (ANOVA). Post hoc tests were done using the least significant difference (LSD) test. All analyses were conducted with Excel 2003 (Microsoft Corp, Redmond, Wash) and Instat (GraphPad Software Inc, San Diego, Calif) software. Subjects were examined by diagnostic category and also by the clinical and histopathologic staging of AD according to NIA-Reagan criteria. Spearman rank correlations of nicotinic receptor binding density with CERAD neuritic plaque density and Braak stage were done both within the 3 categories and across the 3 diagnostic groups.
Ninety-nine subjects were recruited for the study for data collection and interpretation. Mean MMSE, FAST, and GDS scores, and mean ages and disease duration are given in Table 1. Subjects with AD had a significantly lower mean age at death compared with subjects with MCI. They also had significantly fewer years of education compared with subjects with ND. Disease duration differed significantly between subjects with MCI and AD. The MMSE, FAST, and GDS scores differed significantly between the MCI and AD groups and between the ND and AD groups.
The total plaque and neurofibrillary tangle (NFT) densities and the Braak stages are given in Table 2.17 Total plaque, NFT, and Braak stages differed significantly between the ND and AD groups and between the MCI and AD groups (Table 2). Subjects with AD had more total plaque and NFT and higher Braak stages compared with subjects with ND and MCI. The nAChR binding did not differ between the ND and MCI groups. However, nAChR binding was significantly lower in the AD group compared with the ND group.
The sample was subdivided by clinical diagnosis and histopathologic findings. Clinically, subjects with AD were younger at death, were less educated, had longer disease duration, and were more cognitively, functionally, and globally impaired at death compared with subjects in both the ND and MCI groups. Nevertheless, there were no significant clinical differences in age at death, years of education, disease duration, and MMSE, FAST, and GDS scores between subjects in the ND group with or without AD pathology, and no differences between subjects in the MCI group with or without AD pathology.
Neuropathologic classification was performed in subjects in the ND, MCI, and AD groups (Table 3 and Table 4). In Table 3, subjects in the control group (ND) not having AD were classified as ND-ND, and those possibly having AD were classified as ND-AD. Using the same criteria, subjects with MCI were classified in the same manner: MCI-ND and MCI-AD, respectively. All 21 subjects in the ND group were classified as not meeting NIA-Reagan criteria because they did not have clinical dementia. By neuropathologically based CERAD criteria, 12 subjects were classified as not having AD, and 9 as possibly having AD, because of the presence of moderate density of neuritic plaques. All of the subjects in the MCI group were classified as not meeting NIA-Reagan criteria, but 3 of the 8 had sufficient neuritic plaque density to meet CERAD criteria for possible AD. All subjects with AD met NIA-Reagan criteria for AD, with most classified as having a high probability of dementia. All subjects with AD were classified as definitely or probably having AD by CERAD criteria.
There was a significant difference in age at death (P = .02, ANOVA), with the AD group being significantly younger than the MCI-ND group (post hoc LSD) (Table 3). There were no significant differences in mean educational achievement. There was a significant difference in disease duration (P<.001, ANOVA). There was a significant difference in mean FAST scores (P<.001, ANOVA), mean GDS scores (P<.001, ANOVA), and mean MMSE scores (P<.001, ANOVA). The differences were mainly between subjects in the ND and AD groups (post hoc LSD).
The total plaque counts differed significantly (P<.001, ANOVA) between groups, with significant differences between ND-ND and AD, ND-AD and AD, and MCI-AD and AD groups (post hoc LSD) (Table 4). Total NFT counts differed significantly (P<.001, ANOVA), with significant differences between ND-ND and AD, ND-AD and AD, and MCI-AD and AD groups (post hoc LSD). Braak stages differed significantly (P<.001 ANOVA), with significant differences between ND-ND and AD, ND-AD and AD, MCI-ND and AD, and MCI-AD and AD groups (post hoc LSD). The nAChR binding differed significantly (P<.05, ANOVA), with significant differences between ND-AD and AD groups (post hoc LSD).
Spearman rank correlations of nAChR binding density with CERAD neuritic plaque density and Braak stage were done both within the 3 categories and across the 3 diagnostic categories. Overall, no correlations were found between nAChR binding and neuropathologic measures. Only correlations done across all groups were near the significance level, specifically, the correlation of nAChR binding density with neuritic plaque density (P = .16; R2 = −0.21) and with Braak stage (P = .11; R2 = −0.24).
In this clinicopathologic analysis of a prospectively evaluated cohort followed up to autopsy, we found several important observations. First, although amnestic MCI is widely thought of as a transitional state between ND and AD clinically, fewer than half of subjects have sufficient AD pathology to meet a diagnosis of AD by CERAD criteria. Cognitive impairment in these subjects is, therefore, questionably attributable solely to AD pathology. Second, nAChRs are lost mainly during the transition from MCI to AD. Finally, we found that, after separation by histopathology, subjects with ND and MCI with and without AD pathology cannot be differentiated clinically.
Other clinicopathologic studies have investigated subjects with MCI compared with subjects with ND and AD. One recently published study investigated 37 subjects with MCI proximate to death and compared them with subjects with ND and AD.18 This investigation found that most (25/37) subjects with MCI met CERAD criteria for possible, probable, or definite AD.18 Another clinicopathologic study revealed that subjects with clinically classified MCI had higher NFT density compared with subjects with ND and that NFT density but not amyloid plaque density correlated with memory scores.19 That study was limited by a small number of subjects with MCI (n = 3). Like Guillozet et al,19 we find that subjects with MCI, in general, had more tangle pathology and higher Braak stages compared with subjects with ND. We did not perform correlations with specific instruments of memory function. Two recent clinicopathologic studies provide additional information. A study by Markesbery et al20 compares the pathologic findings of ND, MCI, and early AD by region. These investigators found that, in MCI, neuritic plaques were significantly elevated in the frontal, temporal, parietal, amygdala, and posterior cingulate regions compared with ND, and were not different from early AD. In MCI, NFTs were increased in the parietal lobe, amygdala, entorhinal cortex, hippocampal CA1 neurons, and subiculum compared with ND. Similar to our study finding, mean Braak stage was 3.3. When their data are analyzed in the context of pathologic diagnoses, many subjects did not meet pathologic criteria for AD. Similarly, the other recent study, by Petersen et al,3 with 67 subjects with MCI, found that most subjects with amnestic MCI do not meet the pathologic criteria for AD and that 19 (29%) of subjects had primary neuropathologic findings other than AD. Our findings, therefore, agree with these studies in that, while MCI is a transitional clinical state between normal aging and dementia, it is neuropathologically heterogeneous. Future clinicopathologic studies of MCI should classify subjects by neuropathologic findings to prevent mixing different pathologic entities.
Biochemical studies of MCI are still in a preliminary stage. Studies of cerebrospinal fluid show increased tau21,22 and decreased β-amyloid(42) protein levels in subjects with clinically classified MCI that progresses to AD.21 In some studies23-25 of subjects with clinically defined ND, MCI, and AD, it has been concluded that the cholinergic deficit is not yet present at the MCI stage. These studies may be difficult to interpret, however, because clinical, rather than histopathologic, staging of AD was used, resulting in the mixing of subjects with and without significant AD histopathology. When subjects with ND are classified on the basis of AD histopathology, it has been shown that choline acetyltransferase activity declines commensurate with plaque development and accumulation,26,27 placing the cholinergic deficit at the preclinical stage.
Another marker of cholinergic activity is nAChR binding. Nicotine binding levels are modified as a result of altered synthesis, transcription, translation, and transport of nicotine receptors.28 Thus, extensive investigations have found nAChRs to be substantially lost in the neocortex in AD, but it has not yet been investigated in MCI.16,28-32 In this study, we found that nAChR loss does not occur in the transition from ND to MCI but during the transition from MCI to AD. Since the subjects with MCI were older, on average, than subjects with ND, it is unlikely that the absence of nAChR binding changes between ND and MCI is the result of age, since nAChRs decrease with age. This suggests that nAChR loss may be secondary to the accumulation of AD pathology. Alternatively, subjects with AD may be compensating for receptor density loss from cholinergic presynaptic terminals by increasing nicotinic receptor density on postsynaptic neurons. One caveat to consider is that our nAChR binding is of the midfrontal cortex only and the temporal neocortex and hippocampus were not examined. Nicotinic receptor density may not be so closely related to AD pathology as choline acetyltransferase activity, since choline acetyltransferase activity is purely presynaptic, whereas nicotinic receptors are both presynaptic and postsynaptic.
Limitations of this study include the small sample size of the subjects with MCI. This is apparent especially when groups are divided by histopathologic findings. Only subjects with single- or multiple-domain amnesia were examined to ensure less heterogeneity. Subjects were selected by diagnostic categories. Because of the small sample in the MCI group, it is difficult to match groups by age and educational achievement. Similar to Petersen et al,3 we found that subjects with MCI were very old. We excluded subjects who had Lewy body pathology.
Understanding the biochemical changes that occur during the transition from ND to MCI and from MCI to AD may provide insight into the changes in the brain that are responsible for the clinical phenotype of the progressive cognitive decline.
Correspondence: Marwan N. Sabbagh, MD, Cleo Roberts Center for Clinical Research, Sun Health Research Institute, 10515 W Santa Fe Dr, Sun City, AZ 85351 (Marwan.sabbagh@sunhealth.org).
Accepted for Publication: June 13, 2006.
Author Contributions: Dr Sabbagh had full access to all of the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis. Study concept and design: Sabbagh, Reid, Peterson, and Beach. Acquisition of data: Sabbagh, Sue, Connor, Peterson, and Beach. Analysis and interpretation of data: Sabbagh, Shah, and Beach. Drafting of the manuscript: Sabbagh, Shah, Sue, and Beach. Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Sabbagh, Reid, Connor, Peterson, and Beach. Statistical analysis: Sabbagh and Beach. Obtained funding: Beach. Administrative, technical, and material support: Reid, Sue, Connor, and Peterson. Study supervision: Sue and Beach. Dr Reid performed the nicotinic binding; Ms Sue and Dr Beach performed the autopsy evaluations; Dr Connor reviewed the neuropsychologic testing data to ensure accuracy of the diagnostic assignments; Drs Sabbagh and Connor evaluated all subjects in this study and provided the clinical diagnoses; and Dr Sabbagh, Ms Shah, and Mr Peterson gathered and analyzed the data.
Financial Disclosure: None reported.
Funding/Support: This study was supported by the Sun Health Research Institute, the Arizona Alzheimer Disease Consortium, and grant P30 AG 019610 from the National Institute on Aging to the Arizona Alzheimer Disease Core Center.
2.Winblad
BPalmer
KKivipelto
M
et al. Mild cognitive impairment: beyond controversies, towards a consensus: report of the International Working Group on Mild Cognitive Impairment.
J Intern Med 2004;256240- 246
Google ScholarCrossref 3.Petersen
RCParisi
JEDickson
DW
et al. Neuropathologic features of amnestic mild cognitive impairment.
Arch Neurol 2006;63665- 672
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 4.Lopez
OLDeKosky
ST Neuropathology of Alzheimer's disease and mild cognitive impairment.
Rev Neurol 2003;37155- 163
PubMedGoogle Scholar 5.McKhann
GDrachman
DFolstein
MKatzman
RPrice
DStadlan
EM Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer's Disease.
Neurology 1984;34939- 944
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 6.Hyman
BTTrojanowski
JQ Consensus recommendations for the postmortem diagnosis of Alzheimer disease from the National Institute on Aging and the Reagan Institute Working Group on Diagnostic criteria for the Neuropathological Assessment of Alzheimer Disease.
J Neuropathol Exp Neurol 1997;561095- 1097
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 7.Mirra
SSHeyman
AMcKeel
D
et al. The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's Disease (CERAD), II: standardization of the neuropathologic assessment of Alzheimer's disease.
Neurology 1991;41479- 486
Google ScholarCrossref 8.McKeith
IGGalasko
DKosaka
K
et al. Consensus guidelines for the clinical and pathologic diagnosis of dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB): report of the Consortium on DLB international workshop.
Neurology 1996;471113- 1124
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 9.Petersen
RCSmith
GEWaring
SCIvnik
RJTangalos
EGKokmen
E Mild cognitive impairment: clinical characterization and outcome.
Arch Neurol 1999;56303- 308
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 10.Folstein
MFFolstein
SEMcHugh
PR “Mini-Mental State”: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician.
J Psychiatr Res 1975;12189- 198
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 12.Reisberg
BFerris
SDe Leon
MCrook
T The Global Deterioration Scale for assessment of primary degenerative dementia.
Am J Psychiatry 1982;1391136- 1139
PubMedGoogle Scholar 13.Reisberg
BFerris
SHDe Leon
MJCrook
T Global Deterioration Scale (GDS).
Psychopharmacol Bull 1988;24661- 663
PubMedGoogle Scholar 14.Braak
HBraak
E Demonstration of amyloid deposits and neurofibrillary changes in whole brain sections.
Brain Pathol 1991;1213- 216
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 15.Terry
RDMasliah
EHansen
LA The neuropathology of Alzheimer disease and the structural basis of its cognitive alterations.
In:
RD
TerryR
KatzmanKL
BickS
Sisodia
eds.
Alzheimer Disease. Philadelphia, Pa: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins; 1999
Google Scholar 16.Sabbagh
MNReid
RCorey-Bloom
J
et al. Correlation of nicotinic binding to neurochemical markers in Alzheimer's disease.
J Neural Transm 1998;105709- 717
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 18.Bennett
DASchneider
JABienia
JLEvans
DAWilson
RS Mild cognitive impairment is related to Alzheimer disease pathology and cerebral infarctions.
Neurology 2005;64834- 841
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 19.Guillozet
ALWeintraub
SMash
DCMesulam
MM Neurofibrillary tangles, amyloid, and memory in aging and mild cognitive impairment.
Arch Neurol 2003;60729- 736
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 20.Markesbery
WRSchmitt
FAKryscio
RJDavis
DGSmith
CDWekstein
DR Neuropathologic substrate of mild cognitive impairment.
Arch Neurol 2006;6338- 46
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 21.Riemenschneider
MLautenschlager
NWagenpfeil
SDiehl
JDrzezga
AKurz
A Cerebrospinal fluid tau and beta-amyloid 42 proteins identify Alzheimer disease in subjects with mild cognitive impairment.
Arch Neurol 2002;591729- 1734
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 22.Maruyama
MMatsui
TTanji
H
et al. Cerebrospinal fluid tau protein and periventicular white matter lesions in patients with mild cognitive impairment: implications for 2 major pathways.
Arch Neurol 2004;61716- 720
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 23.Davis
KLMohs
RCMarin
D
et al. Cholinergic markers in elderly patients with early signs of Alzheimer disease.
JAMA 1999;2811401- 1406
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 24.DeKosky
STIkonomovic
MDStyren
SD
et al. Upregulation of choline acetyltransferase activity in hippocampus and frontal cortex of elderly subjects with mild cognitive impairment.
Ann Neurol 2002;51145- 155
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 25.Tiraboschi
PHansen
LAAlford
MMasliah
EThal
LJCorey-Bloom
J The decline in synapses and cholinergic activity is asynchronous in Alzheimer's disease.
Neurology 2000;551278- 1283
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 26.Beach
TGHoner
WGHughes
LH Cholinergic fibre loss associated with diffuse plaques in the non-demented elderly: the preclinical stage of Alzheimer's disease?
Acta Neuropathol (Berl) 1997;93146- 153
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 27.Beach
TGKuo
YMSpiegel
K
et al. The cholinergic deficit coincides with Abeta deposition at the earliest histopathologic stages of Alzheimer disease.
J Neuropathol Exp Neurol 2000;59308- 313
PubMedGoogle Scholar 28.Wevers
ASchröder
H Nicotinic acetylcholine receptors in Alzheimer's disease.
J Alzheimers Dis 1999;1207- 219
Google Scholar 29.Sabbagh
MNLukas
RSparks
DLReid
RT The nicotinic acetylcholine receptor, smoking and Alzheimer's disease.
J Alzheimers Dis 2002;4317- 325
Google Scholar 30.Pauly
R Nicotinic cholinergic receptor deficits in Alzheimer's disease: where's the smoke?
J Alzheimers Dis 1999;1221- 230
Google Scholar 31.Perry
EKMorris
CMCourt
JA
et al. Alteration in nicotine binding sites in Parkinson's disease, Lewy body dementia and Alzheimer's disease: possible index of early neuropathology.
Neuroscience 1995;64385- 395
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 32.Warpman
UNordberg
A Epibatidine and ABT 418 reveal selective losses of alpha 4 beta 2 nicotinic receptors in Alzheimer brains.
Neuroreport 1995;62419- 2423
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref