[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Figure. 
Flow diagram of the progress of participants through the parent clinical trial and the magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) substudy.

Flow diagram of the progress of participants through the parent clinical trial and the magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) substudy.

Figure. 
Representative magnetic resonance images used to determine medial temporal atrophy ratings. See Table 1 for complete descriptions of the ratings.

Representative magnetic resonance images used to determine medial temporal atrophy ratings. See Table 1 for complete descriptions of the ratings.

Figure. 
Distribution of mean medial temporal atrophy (MTA) ratings for the 190 study participants. Seventy-nine patients (41.6%) had MTA scores of 1.0 or greater, and 15 patients (7.9%) had scores of 2.0 or greater.

Distribution of mean medial temporal atrophy (MTA) ratings for the 190 study participants. Seventy-nine patients (41.6%) had MTA scores of 1.0 or greater, and 15 patients (7.9%) had scores of 2.0 or greater.

Figure. 
Kaplan-Meier curves for participants with medial temporal atrophy ratings of 2.0 or less compared with those with ratings greater than 2.0.

Kaplan-Meier curves for participants with medial temporal atrophy ratings of 2.0 or less compared with those with ratings greater than 2.0.

Table 1. 
Medial Temporal Atrophy Rating Algorithm
Medial Temporal Atrophy Rating Algorithm
Table 2. 
Demographics of the Study Participants*
Demographics of the Study Participants*
Table 3. 
Impact of Individual Ratings of MTA
Impact of Individual Ratings of MTA
1.
Petersen  RCSmith  GEWaring  SCIvnik  RJTangalos  EGKokmen  E Mild cognitive impairment: clinical characterization and outcome.  Arch Neurol 1999;56303- 308PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
Petersen  RC Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity.  J Intern Med 2004;256183- 194PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
Morris  JCPrice  AL Pathologic correlates of nondemented aging, mild cognitive impairment, and early-stage Alzheimer's disease.  J Mol Neurosci 2001;17101- 118PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Price  JLMorris  JC Tangles and plaques in nondemented aging and “preclinical” Alzheimer's disease.  Ann Neurol 1999;45358- 368PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Morris  JCStorandt  MMiller  JP  et al.  Mild cognitive impairment represents early-stage Alzheimer disease.  Arch Neurol 2001;58397- 405PubMedGoogle Scholar
6.
Bennett  DAWilson  RSSchneider  JA  et al.  Natural history of mild cognitive impairment in older persons.  Neurology 2002;59198- 205PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Ganguli  MDodge  HHShen  CDeKosky  ST Mild cognitive impairment, amnestic type: an epidemiologic study.  Neurology 2004;63115- 121PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Petersen  RCStevens  JCGanguli  MTangalos  EGCummings  JLDeKosky  ST Practice parameter: early detection of dementia: mild cognitive impairment (an evidence-based review): report of the Quality Standards Subcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology.  Neurology 2001;561133- 1142PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Ritchie  KArtero  STouchon  J Classification criteria for mild cognitive impairment: a population-based validation study.  Neurology 2001;5637- 42PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Folstein  MFFolstein  SEMcHugh  PR “Mini-Mental State”: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician.  J Psychiatr Res 1975;12189- 198PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
Jack  CR  JrPetersen  RCXu  YC  et al.  Prediction of AD with MRI-based hippocampal volume in mild cognitive impairment.  Neurology 1999;521397- 1403PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Jack  CR  JrPetersen  RCXu  YC  et al.  Medial temporal atrophy on MRI in normal aging and very mild Alzheimer's disease.  Neurology 1997;49786- 794PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Kaye  JASwihart  THowieson  D  et al.  Volume loss of the hippocampus and temporal lobe in healthy elderly persons destined to develop dementia.  Neurology 1997;481297- 1304PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
de Leon  MJGolomb  JGeorge  AE  et al.  The radiologic prediction of Alzheimer disease: the atrophic hippocampal formation.  AJNR Am J Neuroradiol 1993;14897- 906PubMedGoogle Scholar
15.
Korf  ESCWahlund  LOVisser  PJScheltens  P Medial temporal lobe atrophy on MRI predicts dementia in patients with mild cognitive impairment.  Neurology 2004;6394- 100PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Scheltens  PLeys  DBarkhof  F  et al.  Atrophy of medial temporal lobes on MRI in “probable” Alzheimer's disease and normal aging: diagnostic value and neuropsychological correlates.  J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1992;55967- 972PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
Scheltens  PLauner  LJBarkhof  FWeinstein  HCvan Gool  WA Visual assessment of medial temporal lobe atrophy on magnetic resonance imaging: interobserver reliability.  J Neurol 1995;242557- 560PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
Scheltens  PPasquier  FWeerts  JGBarkhof  FLeys  D Qualitative assessment of cerebral atrophy on MRI: inter- and intra-observer reproducibility in dementia and normal aging.  Eur Neurol 1997;3795- 99PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
19.
Launer  LJScheltens  PLindeboom  JBarkhof  FWeinstein  HCJonker  C Medial temporal lobe atrophy in an open population of very old persons: cognitive, brain atrophy, and sociomedical correlates.  Neurology 1995;45747- 752PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Petersen  RCThomas  RGGrundman  M  et al. Alzheimer's Disease Cooperative Study Group, Vitamin E and donepezil for the treatment of mild cognitive impairment.  N Engl J Med 2005;3522379- 2388PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
Grundman  MPetersen  RCFerris  SH  et al. Alzheimer's Disease Cooperative Study Group, Mild cognitive impairment can be distinguished from Alzheimer disease and normal aging for clinical trials.  Arch Neurol 2004;6159- 66PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
Wechsler  D Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised.  San Antonio, Tex: Psychological Corp; 1981
23.
Berg  LHughes  CPCoben  LADanziger  WLMartin  RLKnesevich  J Mild senile dementia of Alzheimer type: research diagnostic criteria, recruitment, and description of a study population.  J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1982;45962- 968PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Morris  JCMcKeel  DWFulling  KTorack  RMBerg  L Validation of clinical diagnostic criteria for Alzheimer's disease.  Ann Neurol 1988;2417- 22PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
McKhann  GDrachman  DFolstein  MKatzman  RPrice  DStadlan  EM Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer's Disease.  Neurology 1984;34939- 944PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
Grundman  MSencakova  DJack  CR  Jr  et al. Alzheimer's Disease Cooperative Study, Brain MRI hippocampal volume and prediction of clinical status in a mild cognitive impairment trial.  J Mol Neurosci 2002;1923- 27PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
27.
Grundman  MJack  CR  JrPetersen  RC  et al.  Hippocampal volume is associated with memory but not nonmemory cognitive performance in patients with mild cognitive impairment.  J Mol Neurosci 2003;20241- 248PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
28.
Bartko  JJCarpenter  WT On the methods and theory of reliability.  J Nerv Ment Dis 1976;163307- 317PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
29.
Wahlund  LOJulin  PJohansson  SEScheltens  P Visual rating and volumetry of the medial temporal lobe on magnetic resonance imaging in dementia: a comparative study.  J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2000;69630- 635PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
30.
Fleiss  JL Statistical Methods for Rates and Proportions. 2nd ed. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons Inc; 1981
31.
Cox  DR Regression models and life-tables.  J R Stat Soc B 1972;34187- 220Google Scholar
32.
Shrout  PE Measurement reliability and agreement in psychiatry.  Stat Methods Med Res 1998;7301- 317PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
33.
Cutler  SJGrams  AE Correlates of self-reported everyday memory problems.  J Gerontol 1988;43S82- S90PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
34.
Fillit  HHill  JWFutterman  R Health care utilization and costs of Alzheimer's disease: the role of co-morbid conditions, disease stage, and pharmacotherapy.  Fam Med 2002;34528- 535PubMedGoogle Scholar
35.
Hill  JWFutterman  RDuttagupta  SMastey  VLloyd  JRFillit  H Alzheimer's disease and related dementias increase costs of comorbidities in managed Medicare.  Neurology 2002;5862- 70PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
36.
Seltzer  BZolnouni  PNunez  M  et al. Donepezil “402” Study Group, Efficacy of donepezil in early-stage Alzheimer disease: a randomized placebo-controlled trial.  Arch Neurol 2004;611852- 1856PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
37.
Hebert  LEScherr  PABienias  JLBennett  DAEvans  DA Alzheimer disease in the US population: prevalence estimates using the 2000 census.  Arch Neurol 2003;601119- 1122PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
38.
Fillit  HHill  J The economic benefits of acetylcholinesterase inhibitors for patients with Alzheimer disease and associated dementias.  Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 2004;18(suppl 1)S24- S29PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
Original Contribution
January 2007

Qualitative Estimates of Medial Temporal Atrophy as a Predictor of Progression From Mild Cognitive Impairment to Dementia

Author Affiliations

Author Affiliations: Department of Neurology and Imaging of Dementia and Aging Laboratory, Center for Neuroscience, University of California at Davis (Dr DeCarli); Laboratory of Epidemiology Neuroimaging and Telemedicine, Istituto di Ricovero e Cura a Carettere Scientifico San Giovanni di Dio Fatebenefratelli, Brescia, Italy (Dr Frisoni); Department of Neurology, Alzheimer's Disease Center, and Institute on Aging, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia (Dr Clark); Division of Biostatistics, Department of Public Health Sciences, University of California at Davis (Dr Harvey); Departments of Neurosciences (Drs Grundman and Thal) and Family Preventive Medicine (Dr Jin), University of California at San Diego; Departments of Neurology (Dr Petersen) and Radiology (Dr Jack), Mayo Clinic, Rochester, Minn; and Department of Neurology and Alzheimer Center, VU University Medical Center, Amsterdam, the Netherlands (Dr Scheltens). Dr Grundman is now with the Alzheimer's Disease Program, Elan Pharmaceuticals, San Diego.

Arch Neurol. 2007;64(1):108-115. doi:10.1001/archneur.64.1.108
Abstract

Background  Individuals diagnosed as having mild cognitive impairment (MCI) have a high likelihood of progressing to dementia within 3 to 5 years, but not all individuals with MCI progress to dementia. Prognostic uncertainty suggests the need for additional measures to assist the clinician.

Objective  To assess the added value of qualitative measures of medial temporal atrophy (MTA) to estimate the relative risk of progressing from MCI to dementia.

Design  A 3-year, double-blind, placebo-controlled Alzheimer's Disease Cooperative Study initially designed to evaluate the efficacy of donepezil hydrochloride or vitamin E vs placebo to delay progression of MCI to dementia.

Setting  Memory assessment centers.

Patients  A total of 190 individuals with MCI.

Main Outcome Measures  Ratings of MTA performed using magnetic resonance images obtained at baseline. Log-rank tests and Cox proportional hazards ratios examining the significance of MTA estimates in predicting progression of MCI to dementia.

Results  A mean MTA score greater than 2.0 was associated with a greater than 2-fold increased likelihood of progression to dementia during the observation period (hazards ratio, 2.30; 95% confidence interval, 1.09-4.92; P = .03) after controlling for age, education, sex, and baseline Mini-Mental State Examination score.

Conclusions  Adjusted estimates of MTA were associated with significantly increased risk of developing dementia within 3 years, suggesting that obtaining a magnetic resonance image during the evaluation of MCI may offer additional independent information about the risk of progression to dementia. Given the relatively high prevalence of MCI in the general population, use of this method as part of routine clinical evaluation may help identify individuals who might benefit from increased surveillance and future treatment.

Trial Registration  clinicaltrials.gov Identifier: NCT00000173.

Clinical and pathological evidence indicates that older individuals who have significant memory impairment but are not demented may be in a transition phase between normal aging and Alzheimer disease (AD), often denoted as “amnestic mild cognitive impairment” (aMCI).1-5 Patients with aMCI have a high likelihood of progressing to clinically probable AD within 3 to 5 years,1,6,7 and current American Academy of Neurology guidelines suggest careful monitoring of these individuals.8 The high population prevalence1,9 and clinical uncertainty of progression to dementia1 suggest the need for additional measures to assist the clinician.8 Bedside cognitive tests such as the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE)10 are relatively insensitive to early detection of high-risk individuals, and current guidelines suggest the use of detailed neuropsychological testing to determine actual risk.8 Neuropsychological testing procedures, however, are time-consuming, sensitive to extracerebral patient features (such as fatigue and pain), expensive, and not generally available to physicians practicing in a community setting.

Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) is a commonly available tool with possible predictive value. Quantitative MRI studies suggest that hippocampal atrophy is present before dementia is clinically evident.11-13 Prospective studies of patients with aMCI11 find a 4-fold increase in the percentage of individuals converting to dementia within 5 years when hippocampal size was 2.5 SDs below age- and sex-defined reference ranges.

These findings support the utility of brain imaging in aMCI to predict progression to AD. For this method to be applicable in clinical practice, however, a simple and convenient method for reliable and reasonably accurate estimates of hippocampal size is required. Two studies14,15 using qualitative estimates of hippocampal size showed considerable promise but were limited to small groups of patients with aMCI in a retrospective and unblinded manner. For this study we tested the predictive abilities of a simple and reliable scale16-19 on a subgroup of individuals enrolled in an ongoing therapeutic trial for aMCI.20 This study examined the efficacy of using donepezil hydrochloride, 10 mg/d, or vitamin E, 2000 IU/d, to delay progression of aMCI to dementia compared with placebo across 3 years.20 Compared with the placebo group, there were no significant differences in the probability of progression to dementia in the vitamin E group or the donepezil group at the end of 3 years of treatment. We hypothesize that qualitative estimates of medial temporal atrophy (MTA) would significantly increase the likelihood of identifying patients with aMCI at risk for converting to AD above conventional clinical practice, which includes the history, neurological examination, and MMSE. The absence of a significant treatment effect for the study allowed us to combine participants across treatment groups to assess the ability of MTA measures to predict progression of aMCI to dementia.

Methods
Participants

The details of study rationale, design, and participant characteristics for the parent study and the MRI substudy have previously been described in detail.20,21 In brief, 769 participants were recruited from 69 Alzheimer's Disease Cooperative Studycenters in the United States and Canada. Enrollment was based on the criteria for aMCI as previously described,1 modified to use the logical memory II subtest of the Wechsler Memory Scale–Revised,22 adjusted for educational achievement.21 Additional requirements included a Clinical Dementia Rating scale score of 0.523,24 and insufficient cognitive or functional impairment to meet NINCDS-ADRDA (National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Diseases and Stroke and the Alzheimer's Disease and Related Disorders Association) criteria for AD.25 The study was conducted according to Good Clinical Practice guidelines, the Declaration of Helsinki, and the US Code of Federal Regulations Title 21 Part 50 (Protection of Human Subjects) and Title 21 Part 56 (Institutional Review Boards). Written informed consent was obtained from all the participants and from study partners who had knowledge of the participants' functional activities. A data and safety monitoring board reviewed the blinded safety data every 3 months during the trial.

A subset of 195 individuals underwent a research brain MRI examination at entry to the study as part of an ancillary study.26,27 These individuals were selected solely on the basis of their willingness to participate and the availability of suitable MRI equipment at the clinical sites participating in the parent trial.

Mri acquisition

The imaging protocol for this study included a T1-weighted, 3-dimensional, volumetric spoiled-gradient recalled echo sequence with 124 contiguous partitions, 1.6-mm section thickness, a 22.0 × 16.5-cm field of view, 192 views, and a 25° flip angle. The MRI data were transmitted from participating sites to a central data repository at the Mayo Clinic (C.R.J.), where imaging data were checked for compliance with the prescribed imaging sequences and then cataloged.

For this study, deidentified image data were transferred from the Mayo Clinic data repository to the Imaging of Dementia and Aging Laboratory at the University of California at Davis. Of the original 195 images, 5 were not read owing to technical difficulties, leaving a total of 190. Three additional individuals were lost to follow-up. Details of study participant recruitment, enrollment, and outcome are described in Figure 1.

Qualitative analysis

The MRI analysis was restricted to MTA estimates of the MRI at the time of study enrollment. Each MRI was rated using a previously published qualitative rating scale16-18 that assesses the extent of MTA by estimating the combined widths of the choroidal fissure and the temporal horn of the lateral ventricle and the height of the hippocampus in the coronal oblique orientation to derive 1 score based on 5 different categories ranging from 0 (no atrophy) to 4 (severe atrophy) (Figure 2 and Table 1). The right and left medial temporal structures were rated separately, and an overall estimate was created using the average of the 2 ratings. All the images were rated under conditions common to conventional clinical radiologic interpretation.

Four neurologists (C.D., G.B.F., C.M.C., and P.S.) with considerable experience in the clinical diagnosis of AD but with varying degrees of expertise in qualitative analysis of MRIs separately analyzed each MRI. All the ratings were performed without clinical information (eg, age, sex, and progression status) other than the knowledge that they were enrolled in a clinical trial to study treatment of aMCI.

All the raters viewed coronal MRIs at 3× magnified view on video monitors attached to computer workstations (Ultra 5; Sun Microsystems Inc, Santa Clara, Calif) using locally developed viewing software that allowed the rater to view the image in a manner similar to that of a standard radiologic viewing console. Data were recorded directly into spreadsheet program (Excel 2000; Microsoft Corp, Redmond, Wash) files for analyses using statistical software (SAS Version 9; SAS Institute Inc, Cary, NC).

The MTA ratings proceeded in 3 consecutive steps. The first step consisted of an approximately 1-hour training session led by one of us (P.S.) where the 3 other raters were instructed on use of the rating scale through shared review and rating of 35 deidentified images selected from the Imaging of Dementia and Aging Laboratory archives previously confirmed to vary widely in MTA. After completion of the training session, each rater independently rated a second and unique group of 50 deidentified images selected from the Imaging of Dementia and Aging Laboratory archives and confirmed to vary widely in MTA to test interrater agreement. After statistical analysis found satisfactory interrater agreement (see the “Results” section), the raters rated the 190 images that compose the data set for this study.

Statistical analyses

The interclass correlation coefficient, which estimates the variability of different ratings of the same individual to the total variation across all ratings and all individuals,28 was used to assess the interrater reliability of the qualitative scale. The primary outcome for this analysis was time of progression to dementia associated with MTA scores obtained at the time of study enrollment. Kaplan-Meier curves were used to illustrate differences in the pattern of progression between those scoring 2.0 or less and those scoring greater than 2.0 on the scale based on previous evidence that a score of 3.0 or higher is consistent with AD.16 A second analysis based on modified criteria previously reported29 also evaluated the patterns of progression based on MTA scores between 0 and 1.0 and those greater than 1.0. The results for each rater were examined separately as was a summary analysis using the mean qualitative rating score from the 4 raters. Because dichotomized measures based on MTA scores were used for the evaluation of clinical efficacy, we also computed κ statistics for the group using MTA cutoff scores of 1.0 and 2.0.30 To evaluate the added independent effects of MTA measures, Cox proportional hazards models with MMSE were fit separately, followed by models with both MMSE and MTA measures. The independent effects of treatment and apolipoprotein E genotype on conversion to dementia were also examined in separate analyses. These models were used to assess the association of the dichotomized qualitative score with time to progression to dementia after adjusting for age, sex, education, and MMSE score.31 Sensitivity, specificity, and positive and negative predictive values were also determined for MTA.

Results
Interrater reliability

Interclass correlation coefficients for interrater reliability were calculated in 2 ways. First, each of the 3 less experienced raters was individually compared with the developer of the scale (P.S.) on right, left, and mean MTA scores. Interclass correlation coefficients for each rater pair were consistently greater than 0.82 and varied between 0.82 and 0.86. Second, all the raters were compared as a group. There was moderate to substantial agreement,32 with interclass correlation coefficients of 0.81, 0.78, and 0.82 for right, left, and mean MTA scores, respectively. In addition, there was little variation across the raters despite differences in experience with the qualitative rating procedure. Group κ statistics30 were also calculated using MTA scores of 1.0 and 2.0. Moderate agreements32 of 0.53 and 0.47 were found for the group using MTA cutoff scores of 1.0 and 2.0, respectively. Raters rarely differed by more than 1 category of MTA score, and there were no systematic differences among the raters.

Participants

Of the 190 MRIs with data available, progression data were available for 187. Demographics of the individuals studied are summarized in Table 2. Mean age, education, and sex distributions for this subsample were similar to those of the parent study,20,21 where the mean ± SD age was 72.9 ± 7.3 years, the mean ± SD educational achievement was 14.7 ± 3.1 years, and women made up 45.8% of the sample. Baseline mean ± SD MMSE scores were nearly identical between the parent study (27.3 ± 1.8) and the MRI substudy (27.5 ± 1.8). Randomization by treatment arm for this subset of individuals was also nearly identical, with 32.1% randomized to donepezil, 31.1% to vitamin E, and 36.8% to placebo. A total of 66 participants (33.7%) in the MRI group progressed to dementia during the 3-year observation period. After adjusting for person-years of observation, this resulted in a 16.1% yearly rate of progression to dementia for the group, nearly identical to that of the full cohort from the parent study.20

Participants who progressed to dementia were older than those who remained at aMCI throughout the study but had similar educational achievement and sex distributions (Table 2). Participants who progressed to dementia had significantly higher mean ± SD MTA ratings than did those maintaining an aMCI diagnosis (1.15 ± 0.75 vs 0.84 ± 0.59; P = .002), indicating greater MTA atrophy at the time of enrollment in the study. Mean ± SD MMSE scores were significantly lower at baseline for patients who progressed to dementia (26.8 ± 1.86 vs 27.9 ± 1.70; P<.001). The MTA ratings also significantly correlated with MMSE scores (r = −0.20; P = .005). Alzheimer disease was the clinically determined cause of dementia at the time of dementia diagnosis for 99.0% of the study participants.20

DISTRIBUTION OF MTA RATINGS WITH aMCI

The distribution of ratings is graphically illustrated in Figure 3 and varied from 0 to 4 for the individual raters. Consistent with previous qualitative ratings of patients with aMCI,15 no subject consistently received a rating of 4 across all raters, limiting the maximal average rating to 3.1. Only 15 subjects (7.9%) had a mean MTA score greater than or equal to 2.0, and only 12 (6.3%) had a mean MTA score greater than 2.0, indicating that severe MTA15-18 was uncommon, whereas 79 subjects (41.6%) had a mean MTA score greater than or equal to 1.0 and 70 (36.9%) had a mean MTA score greater than 1.0.

Mta and progression to dementia

As noted in the “Participants” subsection, the overall progression to dementia was 33.7% during the 3 years of the study. Increasing MTA ratings were associated with an increasing percentage of individuals progressing to dementia. For individuals with a mean rating of 1.0 or less, only 29.1% progressed to dementia, whereas for those with a mean rating greater than 1.0, 45.7% progressed to dementia; for 2.0 or greater, 60.0% progressed to dementia; and for greater than 2.0, 75% progressed to dementia.

Kaplan-meier curves

Kaplan-Meier curves were used to illustrate progression patterns according to MTA ratings where substantial atrophy was defined as an MTA score greater than 2.0 for the mean of the group ratings (Figure 4). Individuals with MTA scores greater than 2.0 had a hazards ratio (HR) of converting to dementia that was 2.30 (95% confidence interval, 1.09-4.92) times that of an individual with more modest atrophy (β = 0.84; SE = 0.38; P = .03) after adjusting for age, education, sex, and baseline MMSE performance. Similar analyses using MTA scores greater than 1.0 revealed an HR of converting that was 2.30 (95% confidence interval, 1.33-4.08) times that of an individual with more modest atrophy (β = 0.84; SE = 0.29; P = .003).

The MMSE scores were also significantly associated with increased likelihood for progression from aMCI to dementia. After adjusting for age, education, and sex, a difference of a 1-U increase in the MMSE score was associated with a 25% reduction in the likelihood of progression to dementia (β = −0.31; SE = 0.08; P<.001; HR, 0.73; 95% confidence interval, 0.63-0.85). The inclusion of MMSE scores in the analysis only marginally reduced the HR of MTA ratings on predicting progression.

The effects of treatment group and apolipoprotein E genotype were also separately evaluated. Including treatment group in the analysis with age, education, and sex did not substantially change the HR of converting to dementia using MTA cutoff scores of 1.0 (HR, 2.64; P<.001) and 2.0 (HR, 3.14; P = .003). Including apolipoprotein E genotype in the analysis marginally reduced the HR of converting to dementia using MTA cutoff scores of 1.0 (HR, 2.34; P = .003) and 2.0 (HR, 2.44; P = .02).

Table 3 summarizes individual performance using unadjusted MTA cutoff scores of 1.0 and 2.0. The ability of the individual raters to predict progression to dementia was generally significant but somewhat variable. Using a cutoff score greater than 2.0, HRs varied from 2.0 to more than 5.0 but were less consistent among raters, whereas using a cutoff value greater than 1.0 we found more similarity in the group and closer correlation with the group mean measure. These differences likely reflect variability among raters for the relatively small number of MTA scores greater than 2.0.

Sensitivity, specificity, and positive and negative predictive values

Sensitivity, specificity, and positive and negative predictive values can be used to estimate the added value of MTA ratings in addition to the clinical designation of aMCI. Using a cutoff MTA score greater than 2.0, sensitivity was 14%, specificity was 98%, the positive predictive value was 75%, and the negative predictive value was 69%. Using a cutoff MTA score greater than 1.0, sensitivity was 51%, specificity was 69%, the positive predictive value was 46%, and the negative predictive value was 73%. These results support the observation that many fewer individuals with low MTA scores (≤1.0) progressed to dementia (29.1%) compared with the 60.0% with MTA scores of 2.0 or greater or the 45.7% with scores greater than 1.0.

Comment

These results suggest that estimates of MTA convey added value to routine clinical evaluation of individuals with aMCI most likely to progress to dementia within 3 years. Although previous studies14,15 have reported similar results, to our knowledge this is the first such study to evaluate the added impact of MTA ratings in a prospective and blinded manner.

The predictive power of these results is also comparable to previously reported quantitative analyses. For example, in 1 study11 an HR of 1.45 was reported for each 1-U decrease in the hippocampal W score (increase in atrophy) for progression to AD. This suggests that quantitative and qualitative ratings of MTA may have similar abilities to predict outcome in patients with aMCI. Qualitative ratings, however, have the additional advantage of being easy to perform in the clinical setting while retaining high interrater agreement.

The present results might also benefit the clinical evaluation of patients with memory complaints that may or may not meet the strict criteria for aMCI. Complaints of memory impairment are common in the elderly,33 making it difficult to distinguish the very earliest features of AD from other disorders. Epidemiologic studies1,9 also suggest that aMCI is relatively common, with a prevalence of 3% among individuals 65 years and older. Because AD is associated with increased health care costs34,35 and current treatments are known to be clinically effective even when the disease is mild,36 the ability to distinguish individuals at greatest risk for future dementia from the host of individuals visiting a clinical practice with memory complaints is likely to result in public health benefits through increased monitoring, education, and timely treatment.

Although this study focused on a highly select group of individuals who met strict research criteria for aMCI, these results may still translate well into clinical practice where subjective memory complaints are common, but the time and resources to adequately evaluate these common complaints are limited. Sensitivity and specificity are also important. In this regard, use of a high MTA cutoff score, such as greater than 2.0, although infrequent in this study, was associated with a high rate of progression to dementia (75.0%) and a specificity of 98%, which compares favorably with quantitative measures of the hippocampus.11,26 Although relatively insensitive, the high specificity of this measure is ideal for application to general clinical practices that favor a more conservative approach.

The strengths of this study include the blinded prospective nature of the study design, inclusion of a well-defined patient cohort, and detailed surveillance by physicians with dementia experience. In addition, the method used is simple, has good reliability, and could be easily translated into standard clinical practice. Weaknesses of this study include the select nature of the study cohort, which included well-educated individuals who had moderately severe episodic memory impairments and a clinical course suggestive of early AD.5 In addition, the present results indicate that experience in using the scale may have a modest effect on the predictive value of the scale, particularly when using a cutoff score greater than 2.0. At least some of the differences among the raters reflect the study design, which included only a few participants (n = 15) with mean MTA scores greater than 2.0, thereby exacerbating differences in outcome with only small differences in ratings.

In conclusion, these data show that use of a relatively simple, clinically applicable MTA rating scale significantly increases the likelihood of identifying individuals with aMCI who are destined to progress to dementia within 3 years above standard clinical evaluation, which includes MMSE testing. The method is easy to use and reliable, characteristics that offer the potential for use in routine clinical practice. Given the increasing prevalence of AD in our aging society37 and the potential benefits of early diagnosis and treatment of AD,38 we believe that this approach may be useful for identifying patients with aMCI at high risk for progression to AD.

Back to top
Article Information

Correspondence: Charles DeCarli, MD, Department of Neurology, University of California at Davis, 4860 Y St, Suite 3700, Sacramento, CA 95817 (cdecarli@ucdavis.edu).

Accepted for Publication: July 28, 2006.

Author Contributions:Study concept and design: DeCarli, Grundman, and Scheltens. Acquisition of data: DeCarli, Frisoni, Clark, Grundman, Thal, Jack, and Scheltens. Analysis and interpretation of data: DeCarli, Harvey, Petersen, Jin, and Scheltens. Drafting of the manuscript: DeCarli, Frisoni, Clark, Harvey, Grundman, and Scheltens. Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: DeCarli, Clark, Harvey, Petersen, Thal, Jin, Jack, and Scheltens. Statistical analysis: Harvey and Jin. Obtained funding: DeCarli, Grundman, Petersen, and Thal. Administrative, technical, and material support: DeCarli, Grundman, Jack, and Scheltens. Study supervision: DeCarli and Scheltens.

Group Members: The Alzheimer's Disease Cooperative Study Group Investigators are as follows: John Adair, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque; Geoffrey Ahern, University of Arizona, Tucson; Sandra Black, Sunnybrook Health Sciences, Toronto, Ontario; Bradley Boeve and David Knopman, Mayo Clinic, Rochester; Jeffrey Cummings, University of California, Los Angeles; Sultan Darvesh, Queen Elizabeth II Health Sciences Centre, Halifax, Nova Scotia; Charles DeCarli and Grisel J. Lopez, Kansas University, Kansas City; Steven DeKosky, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, Pa; Ranjan Duara, Wien Center, Miami Beach, Fla; Charles Echols, Barrow Neurology Group, Phoenix, Ariz; Howard Feldman, U.B.C. Clinic for Alzheimer's Disease, Vancouver, British Columbia; Steven Ferris and Mony deLeon, New York University Medical Center, New York; Serge Gauthier, McGill Centre for Studies in Aging, Verdun, Quebec; Neill Graff-Radford, Mayo Clinic, Jacksonville, Fla; Danilo Guzman, E. Bruyere Memory Disorder Research, Ottawa, Ontario; Jeffrey Kaye, Oregon Health and Science University, Portland; Alan Lerner, University Hospitals Health System, Cleveland, Ohio; Richard Margolin, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, Tenn; Marsel Mesulam, Northwestern University, Chicago, Ill; Richard Mohs, Mount Sinai School of Medicine, Bronx, NY; John Olichney, University of California, San Diego; Brian Ott, Memorial Hospital of Rhode Island, Pawtucket; Elaine Peskind, University of Washington, Seattle; Nunzio Pomara, Nathan S. Kline Institute for Psychiatric Research, Orangeburg, NY; Christopher van Dyck, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, Conn; Myron Weiner, The University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center, Dallas; and Kristine Yaffe, University of California, San Francisco.

Financial Disclosure: This study was conducted independent of any pharmaceutical company sponsorship. Dr DeCarli is a consultant to Eisai Pharmaceuticals and has received research support from Eisai and Genzyme Pharmaceuticals. Dr Thal is a consultant to Amgen, Avera, Elan, Lilly, Merck, Rinat, Sanofi, Toyama, Acumen, and Ceregene Pharmaceuticals. Dr Petersen is a consultant to Elan and Sevier Pharmaceuticals and has received educational grants from Pfizer, Eisai, Novartis, and Janssen Pharmaceuticals. Dr Frisoni has received honorarium from Pfizer, Novartis, Bracco, and Janssen Pharmaceuticals.

Funding/Support: This study was supported by grants 990802 and 2002073 from the Institute for the Study of Aging and by grants NIA P30 AG10129 and NIA U01-10483 from the National Institutes of Health.

References
1.
Petersen  RCSmith  GEWaring  SCIvnik  RJTangalos  EGKokmen  E Mild cognitive impairment: clinical characterization and outcome.  Arch Neurol 1999;56303- 308PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
Petersen  RC Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity.  J Intern Med 2004;256183- 194PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
Morris  JCPrice  AL Pathologic correlates of nondemented aging, mild cognitive impairment, and early-stage Alzheimer's disease.  J Mol Neurosci 2001;17101- 118PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Price  JLMorris  JC Tangles and plaques in nondemented aging and “preclinical” Alzheimer's disease.  Ann Neurol 1999;45358- 368PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Morris  JCStorandt  MMiller  JP  et al.  Mild cognitive impairment represents early-stage Alzheimer disease.  Arch Neurol 2001;58397- 405PubMedGoogle Scholar
6.
Bennett  DAWilson  RSSchneider  JA  et al.  Natural history of mild cognitive impairment in older persons.  Neurology 2002;59198- 205PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Ganguli  MDodge  HHShen  CDeKosky  ST Mild cognitive impairment, amnestic type: an epidemiologic study.  Neurology 2004;63115- 121PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Petersen  RCStevens  JCGanguli  MTangalos  EGCummings  JLDeKosky  ST Practice parameter: early detection of dementia: mild cognitive impairment (an evidence-based review): report of the Quality Standards Subcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology.  Neurology 2001;561133- 1142PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Ritchie  KArtero  STouchon  J Classification criteria for mild cognitive impairment: a population-based validation study.  Neurology 2001;5637- 42PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Folstein  MFFolstein  SEMcHugh  PR “Mini-Mental State”: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician.  J Psychiatr Res 1975;12189- 198PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
Jack  CR  JrPetersen  RCXu  YC  et al.  Prediction of AD with MRI-based hippocampal volume in mild cognitive impairment.  Neurology 1999;521397- 1403PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Jack  CR  JrPetersen  RCXu  YC  et al.  Medial temporal atrophy on MRI in normal aging and very mild Alzheimer's disease.  Neurology 1997;49786- 794PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Kaye  JASwihart  THowieson  D  et al.  Volume loss of the hippocampus and temporal lobe in healthy elderly persons destined to develop dementia.  Neurology 1997;481297- 1304PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
de Leon  MJGolomb  JGeorge  AE  et al.  The radiologic prediction of Alzheimer disease: the atrophic hippocampal formation.  AJNR Am J Neuroradiol 1993;14897- 906PubMedGoogle Scholar
15.
Korf  ESCWahlund  LOVisser  PJScheltens  P Medial temporal lobe atrophy on MRI predicts dementia in patients with mild cognitive impairment.  Neurology 2004;6394- 100PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Scheltens  PLeys  DBarkhof  F  et al.  Atrophy of medial temporal lobes on MRI in “probable” Alzheimer's disease and normal aging: diagnostic value and neuropsychological correlates.  J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1992;55967- 972PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
Scheltens  PLauner  LJBarkhof  FWeinstein  HCvan Gool  WA Visual assessment of medial temporal lobe atrophy on magnetic resonance imaging: interobserver reliability.  J Neurol 1995;242557- 560PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
Scheltens  PPasquier  FWeerts  JGBarkhof  FLeys  D Qualitative assessment of cerebral atrophy on MRI: inter- and intra-observer reproducibility in dementia and normal aging.  Eur Neurol 1997;3795- 99PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
19.
Launer  LJScheltens  PLindeboom  JBarkhof  FWeinstein  HCJonker  C Medial temporal lobe atrophy in an open population of very old persons: cognitive, brain atrophy, and sociomedical correlates.  Neurology 1995;45747- 752PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Petersen  RCThomas  RGGrundman  M  et al. Alzheimer's Disease Cooperative Study Group, Vitamin E and donepezil for the treatment of mild cognitive impairment.  N Engl J Med 2005;3522379- 2388PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
Grundman  MPetersen  RCFerris  SH  et al. Alzheimer's Disease Cooperative Study Group, Mild cognitive impairment can be distinguished from Alzheimer disease and normal aging for clinical trials.  Arch Neurol 2004;6159- 66PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
Wechsler  D Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised.  San Antonio, Tex: Psychological Corp; 1981
23.
Berg  LHughes  CPCoben  LADanziger  WLMartin  RLKnesevich  J Mild senile dementia of Alzheimer type: research diagnostic criteria, recruitment, and description of a study population.  J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1982;45962- 968PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Morris  JCMcKeel  DWFulling  KTorack  RMBerg  L Validation of clinical diagnostic criteria for Alzheimer's disease.  Ann Neurol 1988;2417- 22PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
McKhann  GDrachman  DFolstein  MKatzman  RPrice  DStadlan  EM Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer's Disease.  Neurology 1984;34939- 944PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
Grundman  MSencakova  DJack  CR  Jr  et al. Alzheimer's Disease Cooperative Study, Brain MRI hippocampal volume and prediction of clinical status in a mild cognitive impairment trial.  J Mol Neurosci 2002;1923- 27PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
27.
Grundman  MJack  CR  JrPetersen  RC  et al.  Hippocampal volume is associated with memory but not nonmemory cognitive performance in patients with mild cognitive impairment.  J Mol Neurosci 2003;20241- 248PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
28.
Bartko  JJCarpenter  WT On the methods and theory of reliability.  J Nerv Ment Dis 1976;163307- 317PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
29.
Wahlund  LOJulin  PJohansson  SEScheltens  P Visual rating and volumetry of the medial temporal lobe on magnetic resonance imaging in dementia: a comparative study.  J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2000;69630- 635PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
30.
Fleiss  JL Statistical Methods for Rates and Proportions. 2nd ed. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons Inc; 1981
31.
Cox  DR Regression models and life-tables.  J R Stat Soc B 1972;34187- 220Google Scholar
32.
Shrout  PE Measurement reliability and agreement in psychiatry.  Stat Methods Med Res 1998;7301- 317PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
33.
Cutler  SJGrams  AE Correlates of self-reported everyday memory problems.  J Gerontol 1988;43S82- S90PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
34.
Fillit  HHill  JWFutterman  R Health care utilization and costs of Alzheimer's disease: the role of co-morbid conditions, disease stage, and pharmacotherapy.  Fam Med 2002;34528- 535PubMedGoogle Scholar
35.
Hill  JWFutterman  RDuttagupta  SMastey  VLloyd  JRFillit  H Alzheimer's disease and related dementias increase costs of comorbidities in managed Medicare.  Neurology 2002;5862- 70PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
36.
Seltzer  BZolnouni  PNunez  M  et al. Donepezil “402” Study Group, Efficacy of donepezil in early-stage Alzheimer disease: a randomized placebo-controlled trial.  Arch Neurol 2004;611852- 1856PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
37.
Hebert  LEScherr  PABienias  JLBennett  DAEvans  DA Alzheimer disease in the US population: prevalence estimates using the 2000 census.  Arch Neurol 2003;601119- 1122PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
38.
Fillit  HHill  J The economic benefits of acetylcholinesterase inhibitors for patients with Alzheimer disease and associated dementias.  Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 2004;18(suppl 1)S24- S29PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
×