[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Figure 1. 
Mean total cranial brain volume, including confidence intervals illustrated as error bars, adjusted for age, sex, body mass index (calculated as weight in kilograms divided by height in meters squared), and Framingham Stroke Risk Profile (slope = −0.25; P < .01; R2 = 0.96). TCV indicates total cranial volume; low, 1 to 7 drinks per week; moderate, 8 to 14 drinks per week; and high, more than 14 drinks per week.

Mean total cranial brain volume, including confidence intervals illustrated as error bars, adjusted for age, sex, body mass index (calculated as weight in kilograms divided by height in meters squared), and Framingham Stroke Risk Profile (slope = −0.25; P < .01; R2 = 0.96). TCV indicates total cranial volume; low, 1 to 7 drinks per week; moderate, 8 to 14 drinks per week; and high, more than 14 drinks per week.

Figure 2. 
Mean values for total cranial brain volume, including confidence intervals illustrated by error bars, for men (slope = −0.20; P = .02) and women (slope = −0.025; P = .02), adjusted for age, sex, body mass index (calculated as weight in kilograms divided by height in meters squared), and Framingham Stroke Risk Profile. TCV indicates total cranial volume; low, 1 to 7 drinks per week; moderate, 8 to 14 drinks per week; and high, more than 15 drinks per week.

Mean values for total cranial brain volume, including confidence intervals illustrated by error bars, for men (slope = −0.20; P = .02) and women (slope = −0.025; P = .02), adjusted for age, sex, body mass index (calculated as weight in kilograms divided by height in meters squared), and Framingham Stroke Risk Profile. TCV indicates total cranial volume; low, 1 to 7 drinks per week; moderate, 8 to 14 drinks per week; and high, more than 15 drinks per week.

Table 1. 
Distribution of Alcohol Consumption in the Total Sample, Men, and Womena
Distribution of Alcohol Consumption in the Total Sample, Men, and Womena
Table 2. 
Comparison of Means (SD) of Covariates Within Categories of Alcohol Consumptiona
Comparison of Means (SD) of Covariates Within Categories of Alcohol Consumptiona
1.
Di Castelnuovo  ACostanzo  SBagnardii  V  et al.  Alcohol dosing and total mortality in men and women: an updated meta-analysis of 34 prospective studies.  Arch Intern Med 2006;166 (22) 2437- 2445PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
Ellison  RCMartinic  M The harms and benefits of moderate drinking: summary of findings of an international symposium.  Annals of Epidemiology 2007;17 (5 suppl) S1- S15Google ScholarCrossref
3.
Mukamal  KJJensen  MKGrønbaek  M  et al.  Drinking frequency, mediating biomarkers, and risk of myocardial infarction in women and men.  Circulation 2005;112 (10) 1406- 1413PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
DeCarli  CMassaro  JHarvey  D  et al.  Measures of brain morphology and infarction in the Framingham heart study: establishing what is normal.  Neurobiol Aging 2005;26 (4) 491- 510Google ScholarCrossref
5.
Seshadri  S Wolf  PBeiser  A  et al.  Stroke risk profile, brain volume and cognitive function: the Framingham Offspring Study.  Neurology 2004;63 (9) 1591- 1599PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Bigler  EDKerr  BVictoroff  JTate  DFBreitner  JC White matter lesions, quantitative magnetic resonance imaging, and dementia.  Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 2002;16 (3) 161- 170PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Kuller  LHLopez  OLJagust  WJ  et al.  Determinants of vascular dementia in the Cardiovascular Health Cognition Study.  Neurology 2005;64 (9) 1548- 1552PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Prins  NDvan Dijk  EJden Heijer  T  et al.  Cerebral small-vessel disease and decline in information processing speed, executive function and memory.  Brain 2005;128 (pt 9) 2034- 2041PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Au  RMassaro  JWolf  P  et al.  Association of white matter hyperintensity volume with decreased cognitive functioning: the Framingham Heart Study.  Arch Neurol 2006;63 (2) 246- 250PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Jeerakathil  TWolf  PBeiser  A  et al.  Stroke risk profile predicts white matter hyperintensity volume.  Stroke 2004;35 (8) 1857- 1861PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
Shimamura  APSquire  LR Korsakoff's syndrome: a study of the relation between anterograde amnesia and remote memory impairment.  Behav Neurosci 1986;100 (2) 165- 170PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Espeland  MAGu  LMasaki  KH  et al.  Association between reported alcohol intake and cognition: results from the Women's Health Initiative Memory Study.  Am J Epidemiol 2005;161 (3) 228- 238PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Galanis  DJJoseph  CMasaki  KHPetrovitch  HRoss  GWWhite  L A longitudinal study of drinking and cognitive performance in elderly Japanese American men: the Honolulu-Asia Aging Study.  Am J Public Health 2000;90 (8) 1254- 1259PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
Mukamal  KJKuller  LHFitzpatrick  ALLongstreth  WT  JrMittleman  MASiscovick  DS Prospective study of alcohol consumption and risk of dementia in older adults.  JAMA 2003;289 (11) 1405- 1413PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Luchsinger  JATang  MXSiddiqui  MShea  SMayeux  R Alcohol intake and risk of dementia.  J Am Geriatr Soc 2004;52 (4) 540- 546PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
den Heijer  TVermeer  SEvan Dijk  EJ  et al.  Alcohol intake in relation to brain magnetic resonance imaging findings in older persons without dementia.  Am J Clin Nutr 2004;80 (4) 992- 997PubMedGoogle Scholar
17.
Mukamal  KJLongstreth  WT  JrMittleman  MACrum  RMSiscovick  DS Alcohol consumption and subclinical findings on magnetic resonance imaging of the brain in older adults: the Cardiovascular Health Study.  Stroke 2001;32 (9) 1939- 1946PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
Ding  JEigenbrodt  MMosley  T  et al.  Alcohol intake and cerebral abnormalities on magnetic resonance imaging in a community-based population of middle-aged adults.  Stroke 2004;35 (1) 16- 21PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
19.
D'Agostino  RBWolf  PABelanger  AJKannel  WB Stroke risk profile: adjustment for antihypertensive medication: the Framingham Study.  Stroke 1994;25 (1) 40- 43PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Taki  YKinomura  SSato  K  et al.  Both global gray matter volume and regional gray matter volume negatively correlate with lifetime alcohol intake in non–alcohol-dependent Japanese men: a volumetric analysis and a voxel-based morphometry.  Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2006;30 (6) 1045- 1050PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
Anstey  KJJorm  AReglade-Mesin  R  et al.  Weekly alcohol consumption, brain atrophy and white matter hyperintensities in a community-based sample aged 60 to 64 years.  Psychosom Med 2006;68 (5) 778- 785PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
Swan  GEDeCarli  CMiller  BLReed  TWolf  PACarmelli  D Biobehavioral characteristics of nondemented older adults with subclinical brain atrophy.  Neurology 2000;54 (11) 2108- 2114PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
23.
Wong  TYKlein  RSharrett  AR  et al.  Cerebral white matter lesions, retinopathy, and incident clinical stroke.  JAMA 2002;288 (1) 67- 74PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Stockwell  TDonath  SCooper-Stanbury  MChikritzhs  TCatalano  PMateo  C Under-reporting of alcohol consumption in household surveys: a comparison of quantity-frequency, graduated-frequency and recent recall.  Addiction 2004;99 (8) 1024- 1033PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
Rothman  KJGreenland  S Modern Epidemiology. 2nd ed. Philadelphia, PA Lippincott Williams & Wilkins1998;
Original Contribution
October 2008

Association of Alcohol Consumption With Brain Volume in the Framingham Study

Author Affiliations

Author Affiliations: Neuroscience Program, Wellesley College, Wellesley, Massachusetts (Ms Paul); Departments of Neurology (Drs Au, Seshadri, and Wolf), Epidemiology (Ms Paul and Dr Fredman), and Biostatistics, Boston University School of Public Health, Boston, Massachusetts (Dr Massaro); Department of Mathematics & Statistics, Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts (Dr Massaro); Framingham Heart Study, Framingham, Massachusetts (Drs Au, Massaro, Seshadri, and Wolf); and Department of Neurology and Center for Neuroscience, University of California, Davis, California (Dr DeCarli).

Arch Neurol. 2008;65(10):1363-1367. doi:10.1001/archneur.65.10.1363
Abstract

Background  While adults who drink low to moderate amounts of alcohol have lower rates of cardiovascular disease than other adults, the effect of alcohol on the brain is less clear. There is evidence that drinking large amounts of alcohol is related to brain atrophy. It is uncertain what the effects of low to moderate consumption might be.

Objective  To determine whether consumption of smaller amounts of alcohol negatively affects brain volume or is protective in reducing the well-documented age-related decline in brain volume.

Design  Total cerebral brain volume (TCBV) was computed, correcting for head size. Multivariate linear regression models were used to evaluate the association between 5 categories of alcohol consumption (abstainers, former drinkers, low, moderate, high) and TCBV, adjusting for age, sex, education, height, body mass index (calculated as weight in kilograms divided by height in meters squared), and the Framingham Stroke Risk Profile. Pairwise comparisons were also conducted between the alcohol consumption groups.

Participants  A total of 1839 subjects from the Framingham Offspring Study who had magnetic resonance imaging of the brain between 1999 and 2001.

Results  Most participants reported low alcohol consumption, and men were more likely than women to be moderate or heavy drinkers. There was a significant negative linear relationship between alcohol consumption and TCBV (r = −0.25; P < .001). This relationship was modified by sex, with alcohol consumption having a stronger association with TCBV in women than in men (r = −0.29 vs −0.20).

Conclusions  In contrast to studies on cardiovascular disease, this study found that moderate alcohol consumption was not protective against normal age-related differences in total brain volume. Rather, the more alcohol consumed, the smaller the total brain volume.

Many studies have considered the costs and benefits of alcohol consumption in diverse populations. Moderate alcohol consumption has frequently been reported to have a beneficial effect on cardiovascular disease (CVD).1-3 Because the brain is perfused by the cardiovascular system, moderate alcohol consumption may attenuate age-related decline in brain volume.

It is recognized that brain volume declines with age4 at an estimated rate of 1.9% per decade,5 while white matter lesions (WMH) increase with age.6 In addition, decline in brain volume and increase in WMH accompany the progression of dementia7,8 and cognitive deficits.9 Lower brain volumes and larger WMH are also observed in persons with higher cardiovascular risk.5,10

Excess alcohol consumption has often been correlated with decline in cognition and can lead to Korsakoff syndrome.11 Yet moderate alcohol consumption has been associated with improved cognitive functioning,12-14 a lower risk of Alzheimer disease (AD),15 and less severe WMH,16,17 although one study found no protective effect of moderate alcohol intake on cerebral infarction.18

The effect of alcohol on brain volume and WMH, however, has not been examined in a community-based sample that is free of clinically evident neurological disease. This cross-sectional study tested the hypothesis that low or moderate alcohol consumption was associated with larger brain volume and less WMH when compared with no drinking or high levels of alcohol consumption in a sample of community-dwelling adults.

Methods
Subjects

The Framingham Offspring cohort, begun in 1971, included children from the original Framingham Heart Study cohort and their spouses (n = 5124) aged 33 to 88 years during the study. Study participants had had 7 health examinations every 4 to 8 years for the past 30 years. Participants at examination 7 (n = 3539; 1999-2001) were invited to have a magnetic resonance image (MRI) of the brain; 1886 participants were imaged through August 2001. Major reasons for not being imaged were residence out of state, medical contraindications for MRI such as pacemakers, and claustrophobia. An additional 47 participants were excluded for prevalent stroke, dementia, or other neurological disorder, resulting in a sample of 1839 respondents without stroke or dementia. Unpublished data find that those who did not have a brain MRI were older and had a higher risk of cardiovascular disease, suggesting that our findings are a conservative estimate of the relationship between alcohol consumption and brain volume.

Measurement of total brain volume

The total (parenchymal) brain volume (TBV) and WMH volumes were corrected for total cranial volume (TCV); the resulting measures are TCBV = (TBV/TCV) × 100 and WMHV = (WMH/TCV) × 100, as described by DeCarli et al,4 where TCBV indicates total cerebral brain volume; WMHV, white matter lesion volume.

Measurement of alcohol consumption

Participants reported at examination 7 the number of alcoholic drinks per week (beer, white wine, red wine, or liquor) they consumed during the past month. Alcohol consumption was recorded as a continuous variable and participants were classified into 1 of 5 categories that have been used in other studies14,17,18: abstainers, former drinkers (drinkers based on their drinking status at earlier examinations), low (1-7 drinks per week), moderate (8-14 drinks per week), and high (> 14 drinks per week).

Covariates

Covariates obtained at examination 7 included age (in years), sex, education (categorized as [1] fourth grade or less, [2] fifth through seventh grade, [3] high school or some college, and [4] college graduate), height, body mass index (BMI; calculated as weight in kilograms divided by height in meters squared), and Framingham Stroke Risk Profile (FSRP, an estimate of the 10-year risk of stroke on a scale of 0 to 1 based on age, sex, systolic blood pressure, antihypertensive therapy, diabetes, smoking status, history of CVD, and the presence of atrial fibrillation and left ventricular hypertrophy19).

Statistical analysis

The mean TCBV and WMHV were calculated for each of the 5 alcohol consumption groups and the means were compared using an analysis of covariance with pairwise comparisons (α = .0125 for each pairwise comparison). Means were tested for a linear trend across alcohol consumption groups at a level of significance of α = 0.5. The initial model included all covariates; those covariates that did not contribute significantly to the model were eliminated 1 at a time, provided that their exclusion did not alter the β coefficient for the alcohol consumption term and that they were not clinically important. Sex was assessed as an effect modifier to determine whether the association between amount of alcohol consumption and TCBV and WMH differed between men and women. Analyses were performed using SAS version 8.2 (SAS Inc, Cary, North Carolina).

Results

The sample included 1839 participants aged 33 to 88 years (mean [SD] age, 60.64 [9.42] years). There were 861 men and 978 women. As shown in Table 1, most participants reported consuming 1 to 7 drinks per week, followed by former drinkers. While women were more likely than men to be abstainers, former drinkers, or those who drink 1 to 7 drinks per week, men were more likely to report moderate or high alcohol consumption.

For the total sample, the mean (SD) TCBV was 77.40% (3.41%) for men (range, 64.89%-85.52%) and 78.35% (2.94%) (range, 67.84%-85.85%) for women.

The covariates for each group were evaluated separately for women and men (Table 2). Among women, amount of alcohol consumption was significantly associated with each covariate (age, sex, BMI, and FSRP score), but among men, alcohol consumption was associated only with FSRP score. Notably, women who consumed moderate amounts of alcohol had the lowest BMI, most education, and lowest FSRP scores, while men who drank low amounts had the lowest FSRP scores.

Association between alcohol consumption and tcbv and wmhv in the total sample

In unadjusted analyses, participants who had low alcohol consumption had slightly larger TCBV compared with the other groups. However, when adjusted for covariates (age, sex, education, BMI, and FSRP score), there was a significant negative linear association between the amount of alcohol consumed and TCBV (β coefficient, −0.25; P < .01) (Figure 1). This slope of −0.25 is slightly larger than the average decline in brain volume per year with aging. From the pairwise comparisons between groups, moderate drinkers had significantly smaller TCBV than former drinkers (P = .03), and participants who consumed more than 14 drinks per week had significantly smaller TCBV than the other groups (high drinkers vs abstainers, P < .01; vs former, P < .01; vs low, P < .01; vs moderate, P = .04). By contrast, there was no significant association between alcohol consumption and WMH.

Association between alcohol consumption and tcbv by sex

In sex-specific analyses, adjusted mean TCBV was higher for women than men in every category of alcohol consumption (Figure 2). Moreover, the linear regression curve for the association between alcohol consumption and TCBV was steeper for women than for men. The interaction between drinking and sex on TCBV was significant (P < .01).

Among women, greater amount of alcohol consumed was significantly associated with smaller TCBV in multivariable analyses (P = .02). From the pairwise comparisons, adjusted mean TCBV only differed significantly between women who were abstainers and moderate drinkers (P = .01), former and low drinkers (P = .01), and former and moderate drinkers (P < .01). Among men, alcohol consumption was also significantly associated with smaller TCBV (P = .02), but while the trend was in the same direction as for women, the overall association was somewhat weaker (β coefficient, −0.20 for men vs −0.29 for women). Adjusted mean TCBV only differed significantly between men who were high drinkers and each of the other alcohol consumption categories (abstainers, P = .03; former, P = .02; low, P < .01; moderate, P < .01) (Figure 2).

To verify that there was no protective effect of alcohol on TCBV, the low consumption group was contrasted with all of the other alcohol consumption groups for both women and men and no significant difference was found for either sex (P = .07 and .39 for women and men, respectively). In addition, there was no quadratic relationship between alcohol consumption and TCBV for men (P = .31).

Comment

This study found no protective effects of alcohol in reducing the normal age-related differences in brain volume in the Framingham Offspring Cohort. Instead, higher levels of alcohol consumption were consistently associated with smaller brain volume after adjusting for covariates. This association was modified by the participants' sex, with women showing larger TCBV than men at every level of alcohol consumption and a steeper negative slope in the line relating alcohol consumption and TCBV. However, the direction of this relationship was the same for both sexes and the magnitude of the differences between sexes was small. We also found no significant correlation between alcohol consumption and WMHV.

Our results are consistent with 2 recent studies in smaller samples. In a study of 405 Japanese men, both global and regional gray matter volumes were negatively correlated with lifetime alcohol intake.20 In another sample of 385 adults aged 60 to 64 years, greater alcohol consumption was associated with more brain atrophy.21 Neither of these studies found beneficial effects of low to moderate alcohol consumption.

Two other studies related alcohol consumption to ventricular and sulcal size.17,18 It is noteworthy that both studies found increasing ventricular sizes with increasing amounts of alcohol consumed. These findings are consistent with our results because ventricular size is an inverse measure of brain atrophy.

Our finding that sex modified the association between alcohol consumption and brain volume could be related to biological factors. Alcohol is absorbed more rapidly in women than in men, and in general women are more vulnerable to the effects of alcohol than men. Women, on average, are smaller than men and have less blood to dilute the alcohol. Results from this study suggest that alcohol also has a greater negative effect on brain volume in women than in men. In our sample, twice as many men as women reported high alcohol consumption and more men also reported moderate alcohol consumption, which may suggest a behavioral rather than a biological explanation for these findings.

Our hypothesis was based on many observations of a J-shaped association between amount of alcohol consumption and CVD. However, our results indicate that there is no neuroprotective effect of alcohol on neurons.

This study had several limitations. It was restricted to TBV, while regional brain areas have not yet been considered independently. Because some of the alcohol consumption groups contained few participants, statistical power was low for some analyses. Additionally, the Framingham Offspring cohort is predominantly of European origin and the mean educational level is high, thus these results may not be generalizable to other racial and economic groups. Finally, only about 50% of the sample had MRI scans; however this participation rate is found for all of the Framingham Heart Study articles and is consistent with most other MRI studies.17,22,23

Another potential limitation is the difficulty of distinguishing the effects of alcohol consumption from those of age on TCBV. Age is a normally accompanied by smaller brain volume4 and is inextricably linked to many of the other covariates, especially BMI and FSRP score.

Use of self-reported data to determine the quantity of alcohol consumed is also a concern because participants are likely to underreport their true drinking pattern.24 However, underreporting of alcohol consumption would most likely have resulted in an underestimate of the association,25 suggesting that the true association between alcohol consumption and brain volume would be even larger than the one observed.

Finally, these results were observed in cross-sectional data. Thus, we cannot relate alcohol consumption to decline in brain volume. Prospective analyses of this association would be important to establish the temporal relationship between alcohol consumption and brain volume.

Nonetheless, this study has many strengths. It included MRIs performed on more than 1800 Framingham Study participants without clinical dementia or stroke. These volumetric MRI data were examined after adjusting for a wealth of baseline data on cardiovascular risk. Finally, the MRI readers were blinded to participants' demographic and clinical data.

The public health effect of this study gives a clear message about the possible dangers of drinking alcohol. While there are many reports of the beneficial effects of moderate alcohol consumption, these reports are often accompanied by concerns that the data not be interpreted as encouraging drinking. Prospective longitudinal studies are needed to confirm these results as well as to determine whether there are any functional consequences associated with increasing alcohol consumption. This study suggests that, unlike the associations with CVD, alcohol consumption does not have any protective effect on brain volume.

Correspondence: Carol Ann Paul, MS, Neuroscience Program, Wellesley College, 106 Central St, Wellesley, MA 02418 (cpaul@wellesley.edu).

Accepted for Publication: April 4, 2008.

Author Contributions: Ms Paul had full access to all of the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis. Study concept and design: Paul, Fredman, and Seshadri. Acquisition of data: Au, Seshadri, DeCarli, and Wolf. Analysis and interpretation of data: Paul, Au, Fredman, Massaro, Seshadri, and Wolf. Drafting of the manuscript: Paul, Seshadri, and DeCarli. Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Au, Fredman, Massaro, Seshadri, and Wolf. Statistical analysis: Paul, Fredman, and Massaro. Obtained funding: Wolf. Administrative, technical, and material support: Wolf. Study supervision: Au and DeCarli.

Financial Disclosure: None reported.

Funding/Support: This study was supported by contract N01-HC-25195 from the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute's Framingham Heart Study, National Institutes of Health; grants 5R01-AG08122 and 2R01-AG16495 from the National Institute on Aging; and grant 2R01-NS017950 from the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke.

References
1.
Di Castelnuovo  ACostanzo  SBagnardii  V  et al.  Alcohol dosing and total mortality in men and women: an updated meta-analysis of 34 prospective studies.  Arch Intern Med 2006;166 (22) 2437- 2445PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
Ellison  RCMartinic  M The harms and benefits of moderate drinking: summary of findings of an international symposium.  Annals of Epidemiology 2007;17 (5 suppl) S1- S15Google ScholarCrossref
3.
Mukamal  KJJensen  MKGrønbaek  M  et al.  Drinking frequency, mediating biomarkers, and risk of myocardial infarction in women and men.  Circulation 2005;112 (10) 1406- 1413PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
DeCarli  CMassaro  JHarvey  D  et al.  Measures of brain morphology and infarction in the Framingham heart study: establishing what is normal.  Neurobiol Aging 2005;26 (4) 491- 510Google ScholarCrossref
5.
Seshadri  S Wolf  PBeiser  A  et al.  Stroke risk profile, brain volume and cognitive function: the Framingham Offspring Study.  Neurology 2004;63 (9) 1591- 1599PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Bigler  EDKerr  BVictoroff  JTate  DFBreitner  JC White matter lesions, quantitative magnetic resonance imaging, and dementia.  Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 2002;16 (3) 161- 170PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Kuller  LHLopez  OLJagust  WJ  et al.  Determinants of vascular dementia in the Cardiovascular Health Cognition Study.  Neurology 2005;64 (9) 1548- 1552PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Prins  NDvan Dijk  EJden Heijer  T  et al.  Cerebral small-vessel disease and decline in information processing speed, executive function and memory.  Brain 2005;128 (pt 9) 2034- 2041PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Au  RMassaro  JWolf  P  et al.  Association of white matter hyperintensity volume with decreased cognitive functioning: the Framingham Heart Study.  Arch Neurol 2006;63 (2) 246- 250PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Jeerakathil  TWolf  PBeiser  A  et al.  Stroke risk profile predicts white matter hyperintensity volume.  Stroke 2004;35 (8) 1857- 1861PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
Shimamura  APSquire  LR Korsakoff's syndrome: a study of the relation between anterograde amnesia and remote memory impairment.  Behav Neurosci 1986;100 (2) 165- 170PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Espeland  MAGu  LMasaki  KH  et al.  Association between reported alcohol intake and cognition: results from the Women's Health Initiative Memory Study.  Am J Epidemiol 2005;161 (3) 228- 238PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Galanis  DJJoseph  CMasaki  KHPetrovitch  HRoss  GWWhite  L A longitudinal study of drinking and cognitive performance in elderly Japanese American men: the Honolulu-Asia Aging Study.  Am J Public Health 2000;90 (8) 1254- 1259PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
Mukamal  KJKuller  LHFitzpatrick  ALLongstreth  WT  JrMittleman  MASiscovick  DS Prospective study of alcohol consumption and risk of dementia in older adults.  JAMA 2003;289 (11) 1405- 1413PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Luchsinger  JATang  MXSiddiqui  MShea  SMayeux  R Alcohol intake and risk of dementia.  J Am Geriatr Soc 2004;52 (4) 540- 546PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
den Heijer  TVermeer  SEvan Dijk  EJ  et al.  Alcohol intake in relation to brain magnetic resonance imaging findings in older persons without dementia.  Am J Clin Nutr 2004;80 (4) 992- 997PubMedGoogle Scholar
17.
Mukamal  KJLongstreth  WT  JrMittleman  MACrum  RMSiscovick  DS Alcohol consumption and subclinical findings on magnetic resonance imaging of the brain in older adults: the Cardiovascular Health Study.  Stroke 2001;32 (9) 1939- 1946PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
Ding  JEigenbrodt  MMosley  T  et al.  Alcohol intake and cerebral abnormalities on magnetic resonance imaging in a community-based population of middle-aged adults.  Stroke 2004;35 (1) 16- 21PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
19.
D'Agostino  RBWolf  PABelanger  AJKannel  WB Stroke risk profile: adjustment for antihypertensive medication: the Framingham Study.  Stroke 1994;25 (1) 40- 43PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Taki  YKinomura  SSato  K  et al.  Both global gray matter volume and regional gray matter volume negatively correlate with lifetime alcohol intake in non–alcohol-dependent Japanese men: a volumetric analysis and a voxel-based morphometry.  Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2006;30 (6) 1045- 1050PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
Anstey  KJJorm  AReglade-Mesin  R  et al.  Weekly alcohol consumption, brain atrophy and white matter hyperintensities in a community-based sample aged 60 to 64 years.  Psychosom Med 2006;68 (5) 778- 785PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
Swan  GEDeCarli  CMiller  BLReed  TWolf  PACarmelli  D Biobehavioral characteristics of nondemented older adults with subclinical brain atrophy.  Neurology 2000;54 (11) 2108- 2114PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
23.
Wong  TYKlein  RSharrett  AR  et al.  Cerebral white matter lesions, retinopathy, and incident clinical stroke.  JAMA 2002;288 (1) 67- 74PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Stockwell  TDonath  SCooper-Stanbury  MChikritzhs  TCatalano  PMateo  C Under-reporting of alcohol consumption in household surveys: a comparison of quantity-frequency, graduated-frequency and recent recall.  Addiction 2004;99 (8) 1024- 1033PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
Rothman  KJGreenland  S Modern Epidemiology. 2nd ed. Philadelphia, PA Lippincott Williams & Wilkins1998;
×