Objective
To evaluate the influences of a family history of Alzheimer dementia (FHxAD) and the apolipoprotein E ε4 genotype (APOE ε4) on cognitive decline.
Design, Setting, and Participants
Residents of Cache County, Utah, aged 65 years or older, were invited to participate. At baseline, 2957 participants provided DNA for genotyping of APOE and a detailed FHxAD. They also completed the Modified Mini-Mental State Examination. Cognitive status was reexamined after 3 and 7 years. We used mixed-effects models to examine the association among FHxAD, APOE ε4, and cognitive trajectories.
Main Outcome Measure
Modified Mini-Mental State Examination score trajectories over time.
Results
Compared with participants who did not have APOE ε4 or an FHxAD, those with APOE ε4 scored lower on the Modified Mini-Mental State Examination at baseline (−0.70 points; 95% confidence interval [CI], −1.15 to −0.24). Participants with an FHxAD and APOE ε4 differed less, if at all, in baseline score (−0.46 points; 95% CI, −1.09 to 0.16) but declined faster during the 7-year study (−9.75 points [95% CI, −10.82 to −8.67] vs −2.91 points [95% CI, −3.37 to −2.44]). After exclusion of participants who developed prodromal AD or incident dementia, the group with an FHxAD and APOE ε4 declined much less during the 7-year study (−1.54; 95% CI, −2.59 to −0.50).
Conclusions
Much of the association among FHxAD, APOE ε4, and cognitive decline may be attributed to undetected incipient (latent) disease. In the absence of latent disease, the 2 factors do not appear individually to be associated with cognitive decline, although they may be additive.
Alzheimer dementia (AD) is a neurodegenerative condition that begins with mild memory loss and progresses to total memory loss and loss of independence1 and is a leading cause of death.1,2 A report from the nationally representative Aging, Demographics, and Memory Study indicated that AD accounts for almost 70% of all dementias in the population.3 Individuals with an expressed family history of AD (FHxAD) have a 39% higher lifetime risk of disease by 96 years of age.4 When both parents develop the disease, the risk for offspring is higher still (41.8% by 70 years of age; 54% by 80 years of age).4,5 Few studies have evaluated the effect of FHxAD on cognitive trajectories over time. An FHxAD may be viewed as an indicator for genetic contributions in the absence of confirmed associations with risk genes, although shared environment may account for a portion of the variance associated with FHxAD.
Three genes predispose individuals to an early onset of AD; however, these are rare in the general population.6-9 Other as yet unconfirmed genetic associations will likely account for a much greater proportion of disease.10 The only nonmendelian risk gene that has been firmly established is the ε4 variant of the apolipoprotein (APOE) gene (+107741).11-15 The ε4 allele of this gene is a well-known risk factor16 that is thought to affect the timing of disease.17,18 Interestingly, the association between APOE ε4 and cognitive decline is somewhat equivocal in the literature. Previous findings have suggested that APOE ε4 is associated with cognitive decline in many19-26 but not all27-30 studies. Some researchers theorize that the difference may be the inclusion of incipient AD cases in the samples.23,24,31 One very large study found no differences associated with APOE ε4 on cognitive tests. However, the participants were young, ranging in age from 20 to 64 years, and the study was cross-sectional.32 Few studies have evaluated the influences of both FHxAD and APOE ε4 status on cognitive trajectories in elderly populations. Herein we evaluate the combined effects of the presence or absence of an FHxAD and the APOE ε4 genetic variant on cognition over time.
The Cache County Study of Memory, Health, and Aging is an epidemiological investigation of memory in aging in Cache County, Utah.17 Protocols and procedures were approved by the institutional review boards of Utah State University, Duke University, and The Johns Hopkins University. Informed consent was obtained from all study participants at each stage of the study. Spouses or next of kin gave consent when participants were unable to provide it.
Research protocols for the study have been previously described in detail.17,33 Briefly, at the beginning of the study in early 1995, all residents of the county who were 65 years or older were invited to participate. A total of 5092 persons completed baseline screening and a risk factor questionnaire. Surviving members of the cohort who were willing to participate underwent screening again 3 and 7 years later (1999 and 2003). For the current evaluation, we set aside 357 cases of prevalent all-cause dementia. Another 491 individuals without complete family history reports were excluded, as were 95 individuals who did not provide DNA for APOE genotyping. Of 4149 individuals with the necessary information, a total of 2957 completed at least 1 subsequent evaluation and were eligible for inclusion in the analytic sample. Individuals who were excluded (n = 2135) tended to be older but did not differ regarding FHxAD. Of those who provided DNA for APOE genotyping, proportionally more individuals in the excluded group had APOE ε4. This difference was attributed to an overrepresentation of ε3/ε4 and ε4/ε4 genotypes among prevalent dementia cases (χ2 = 93.70; P < .001).
At each evaluation point, a cognitive screening test, the Modified Mini-Mental State Examination (3MS)34 adapted for epidemiological studies35 was used to evaluate participants' cognitive status. The 3MS is frequently used as a screening instrument to detect changes in global cognition. Based on the Mini-Mental State Examination, it was adapted for epidemiological studies by changing some questions and adjusting the scoring from a range of 1 to 30 to a range of 1 to 100. Demographic questions that are difficult to verify in fieldwork (ie, date and place of birth) were changed to questions about current and past presidents. With fewer ceiling effects than the Mini-Mental State Examination, the 3MS is more sensitive in the detection of dementia and milder cognitive syndromes.36 Previously, we reported the sensitivity, specificity,37,38 and normative data on the 3MS in this cohort.39 In the first 2 evaluations, we used the 3MS in combination with the Dementia Questionnaire40 (3MS cut point, 86/87); at the second follow-up, we combined the 3MS with a brief battery of neuropsychological tests (3MS cut point, 90/91). The sensitivities for the detection of dementia at each evaluation were 84.6%, 93.6%, and 96.7%, respectively. Individuals with screening results that were positive for a cognitive disorder received a full clinical assessment. At the second follow-up evaluation, all individuals 85 years or older underwent clinical assessments. Dementia cases were diagnosed using standard criteria41-43 as described previously.17,33 Cases of prodromal AD44 were diagnosed clinically on the basis of a history of mild symptomatic disease (eg, memory loss or changes in instrumental activities of daily living). Diagnoses were assigned if the participants' history, medical evaluation, and clinical profile indicated subsyndromal AD after excluding competing causes.
An FHxAD was determined with a structured questionnaire that was administered at baseline and at the first follow-up evaluation. Participants were asked to list the names of biological parents and siblings and to provide information about whether they ever had memory problems. If memory problems were reported, further questions determined the age at which problems began, whether they began suddenly or slowly, if they progressed over time, if there were limitations with activities of daily living, and whether a physician's diagnosis was received. In addition, a list of problems (AD, Parkinson disease, Down syndrome, hardening of the arteries, ministrokes or transient ischemic attacks, arteriosclerosis, and other neurological conditions) was presented as a cue for diagnoses that may have been given. Each relative was classified as having suspected AD if a physician had given a diagnosis of AD or, lacking a physician's diagnosis, if the relative's memory problems had worsened over time, causing limitations with daily activities. If a first-degree relative had died before 50 years of age but did not have dementia, that person's information was coded as missing. Study participants were classified as having an FHxAD if at least 1 first-degree relative was categorized as having suspected AD.
Initial comparisons were made between individuals who reported an FHxAD and those who reported no FHxAD. We compared categorical variables using χ2 tests and evaluated continuous variables using 2-sample t tests. To assess the effects of an FHxAD and APOE ε4 genotype on cognition over time, we used the 3MS as a measure of global cognitive performance and applied mixed-effects modeling techniques using the SAS PROC MIXED procedure.45 This procedure accommodates fixed and random effects that account for individual differences in cognitive performance at baseline and at subsequent measurements. All mixed-effects models were adjusted for factors significantly associated with baseline 3MS scores, including age, sex, and education. Time was evaluated as a nominal variable (0, 3, and 7 years) corresponding with baseline and average time to each of 2 follow-up evaluations. A quadratic term for time (time2) was included to allow for nonlinear changes in cognition over time. For the main exposures of interest, we constructed a categorical variable classifying each participant into 1 of the following 4 mutually exclusive groups: those with no FHxAD and no APOE ε4, those with an FHxAD only, those with APOE ε4 only, and those with both risk factors. The variable was dummy coded with the no FHxAD and no APOE ε4 category as the reference group. To assess differences in cognitive trajectories over time, we included interaction terms for linear and quadratic time with each of the FHxAD × APOE ε4 groups. Parameterized in this way, main-effect terms for each of the 3 nonreference FHxAD × APOE ε4 groups provide estimates of mean differences in 3MS scores compared with the reference group at baseline, whereas the interaction terms represent differences in the rate of change over time. We evaluated the significance of including the interaction terms with time using an omnibus likelihood ratio test to determine whether there were any differences in the rate of change among the FHxAD × APOE ε4 groups. Post hoc comparisons of the differences in the rate of change between each individual group and the reference group were performed separately using multivariate Wald tests. An additional model was constructed with an alternate parameterization including separate dichotomous indicator terms for the presence or absence of an FHxAD and APOE ε4 and another term for the product between the two to test for multiplicative interaction.
Demographic characteristics of the sample are reported in Table 1. From a total sample of 2957 individuals participating in at least 2 evaluations, 28.4% had an FHxAD and 30.2% had APOE ε4; 10.4% had both. Most participants were female (57.8%) (mean age, 74 years), and most had a high school education or more. The average baseline 3MS score was 92.1. Groups with APOE ε4 alone or an FHxAD plus APOE ε4 were significantly younger. The average follow-up time was 6 (SD, 2.1; range, 2-9) years.
Modeling cognitive trajectories based on 3MS scores showed that the inclusion of interaction terms between time (linear and quadratic components) and each of the FHxAD × APOE ε4 groups was significant (likelihood ratio test, χ26 = 17.8; P = .007). In the model with these interactions (model 1 in Table 2), there was no evidence of differences in the rate of change in the 3MS score over time for those with an FHxAD only (multivariate Wald, P = .91). The presence of APOE ε4 was significantly associated with a lower baseline score (−0.70; 95% confidence interval [CI], −1.15 to −0.24) and a faster decline over time (−6.64; 95% CI, −7.38 to −5.90; multivariate Wald, P < .001) than the reference group. Participants with both an FHxAD and APOE ε4 declined significantly faster than did those with neither risk factor (multivariate Wald, P < .001). We estimated that those with both an FHxAD and APOE ε4 declined on average 9.75 points on the 3MS score during the 7-year study (95% CI, −10.82 to −8.67) compared with a 2.91-point decline (95% CI, −3.37 to −2.44) for the reference group. Results shown in Figure 1 represent the average participant in each group after adjustment for baseline age (centered at 74 years of age), sex, and education (centered at 13.4 years). A separate model designed to test multiplicative interaction between an FHxAD and APOE ε4 revealed no significant interaction between these 2 risk factors (P = .59; data not shown).
We then undertook a second evaluation of cognitive trajectories after removing from the sample 348 individuals who eventually received a diagnosis of dementia (72.7% were AD cases) and 464 individuals who at their last observation point were diagnosed as having prodromal AD. A total of 2145 individuals at the first follow-up and 1369 at the second follow-up remained in the sample. These results are presented in model 2 in Table 2. As a result of removing incident cases of dementia and prodromal AD, the slope of the curves decreased (Figure 2). A post hoc multivariate Wald test revealed that a statistically significant difference between the reference group and the group with an FHxAD and APOE ε4 remained (P = .007), although the change over time was not clinically meaningful (−1.54 points during the 7 years).
We evaluated the associations between an FHxAD and APOE ε4 and their combination on cognitive change over time compared with participants with neither risk factor. The cumulative decline during the 7-year study in the group with both risk factors roughly approximates the sum of the declines for the groups with an FHxAD alone and APOE ε4 alone, suggesting that the 2 factors are additive. A formal test for a multiplicative interaction between an FHxAD and APOE ε4 revealed none. After removing incident cases of dementia and prodromal AD, trajectories of the groups became more similar, but a significant effect remained for the group with an FHxAD and APOE ε4. These findings imply that the greater cognitive decline found in those with APOE ε4 reflects the known relation of APOE ε4 to AD and not to another independent phenotype of poor cognition.
These analyses may help explain the lack of consensus in the literature on the effects of APOE ε4 on cognitive change over time. Sources of variation in results have been attributed to different study designs, study populations, follow-up periods, and age distributions of the samples under study. In some instances, previous research did not take future dementia diagnoses into account. For example, in the MacArthur Studies of Successful Aging, a population-based multisite 7-year study of 965 individuals, incident dementia cases were not removed from the sample, and the authors concluded that the ε4 allele was related to cognitive decline.19In studies in which incident cases were removed from the sample27,30 or groups were analyzed separately according to cognitive status,28,29 no decline has been found in cognitively normal individuals with APOE ε4. However, a careful study by Christensen et al20 found that individuals with APOE ε4 had poorer scores on the Mini-Mental State Examination and the Symbol Digit Modalities Test after controlling for other risk factors. When mild cases of cognitive disorder were set aside, the results were unchanged. Similarly, Hofer et al21 found a significant effect of APOE ε4 on cognitive decline during a 7-year study using latent growth curve modeling. In this study, dementia cases were identified only at the first 2 evaluations and not the third. Cases of mild impairment or FHxAD were not considered. Indeed, most studies of APOE ε4 and cognitive decline do not consider family history in their analyses.
In our study, the difference between groups was attenuated with the removal of prodromal AD and incident dementia cases, suggesting that much of the decline was due to incipient disease. Nonetheless, a clinically small but statistically significant difference remained. There are several possible explanations for this phenomenon. It may be that we did not identify all incident cases in the sample and that the resulting decline is the result of unidentified cases. We doubt this is the case, because the protocol of the Cache County Study of Memory, Health, and Aging called for close scrutiny of those with APOE ε4. A more likely explanation is that much of the residual decline represents early change that has not yet revealed itself as a recognizable cognitive syndrome. In our sample, once incident dementia and prodromal AD cases were removed, the group with APOE ε4 and no FHxAD did not show significantly greater decline over time than did the reference group. Finally, we note the effects of the combination of FHxAD and APOE ε4, suggesting that the former may act independently, possibly reflecting other genetic influences or shared environment, and these influences may result in cognitive decline that does not reflect incipient AD.
Very few studies have evaluated the combined effects of APOE ε4 and family history. In a 6-year follow-up of the Kungsholmen Project cohort, Huang et al46 concluded that a family history of dementia was associated only with an increased risk of dementia among APOE ε4 carriers. Duara et al47 evaluated the combined effects of family history and APOE ε4 on time to AD onset in a sample of 197 individuals and concluded that APOE ε4 and family history of disease are independent risk factors that operate in an additive manner. Our results extend these findings and suggest a similar additive effect of APOE ε4 and FHxAD on cognitive decline.
Our study has several strengths and limitations. Our determination of FHxAD was based on self-reported data. We acknowledge that, with the advent of widespread genetic testing, self-reported family history information may soon become obsolete. However, the method of data collection applied herein was a structured, detailed interview similar to those commonly used in genetic studies. This method has been successfully used in numerous studies and found to be reasonably reliable40 albeit with some measurement error. In the present study, measurement error may bias our results in favor of those who have APOE ε4 because it is associated with earlier disease onset. Individuals with APOE ε4 and an FHxAD were younger than those with an FHxAD only (P < .01). Information about the FHxAD in the present analysis was updated through the second data point but was not available for the full cohort at the third evaluation. Therefore, it is possible that some individuals with an FHxAD were misclassified; however, the effect of such a misclassification would tend to make our results more conservative. Our measure of cognitive performance, the 3MS, mainly reflects global cognition rather than specific cognitive domains. For this reason it is possible that we were unable to detect subtle changes that may be specific to particular cognitive domains. The strengths of the study include the large sample size and long-term follow-up. We implemented a comprehensive evaluation of cognitive status at all 3 time points, although it is possible that some incident cases of cognitive decline may have been misclassified. This may explain the remaining observed decline among those with an FHxAD and APOE ε4 when incident cases were removed. Although this finding was statistically significant, the decline itself was not clinically significant.48
There may appear to be some tautology inherent in this study in that the removal of individuals with cognitive impairment from the analysis yields a sample of individuals with little or no cognitive decline. Although this is true, the objective of the study was to evaluate the performance of individuals with an FHxAD and APOE ε4 compared with individuals with neither risk factor to see whether decline could be observed before any clinical manifestation of disease. We have shown that an FHxAD and APOE ε4 have deleterious effects on cognition over time. This effect is significant for those who have both risk factors and suggests, as we know, that genes beyond APOE have influences on AD risk and expression. When prodromal AD and incident dementia cases are removed from the sample, the effects of genes on cognitive decline are mitigated, implying that much of the decline can be attributed to early expression of disease. Observable decline in cognitive screening scores over time (among individuals classified as normal) may be a useful indicator for subgroups at risk for AD or already expressing mild symptomatic disease. Further work is needed to elucidate more fully the cognitive patterns that precede and predict cognitive decline for each of these groups.
Correspondence: Kathleen M. Hayden, PhD, Joseph and Kathleen Bryan Alzheimer's Disease Research Center, Duke University Medical Center, 2200 W Main St, Ste A-200, Durham, NC 27705 (khayden@duke.edu).
Accepted for Publication: April 12, 2009.
Author Contributions: Dr Hayden had full access to all the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis. Study concept and design: Hayden, Zandi, West, Tschanz, Breitner, and Welsh-Bohmer. Acquisition of data: West, Tschanz, Norton, and Welsh-Bohmer. Analysis and interpretation of data: Hayden, Zandi, West, Corcoran, Breitner, and Welsh-Bohmer. Drafting of the manuscript: Hayden, Zandi, and Welsh-Bohmer. Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Hayden, Zandi, West, Tschanz, Norton, Corcoran, and Breitner. Statistical analysis: Hayden, Zandi, and Corcoran. Obtained funding: Welsh-Bohmer. Administrative, technical, and material support: West, Tschanz, Norton, Breitner, and Welsh-Bohmer.
Cache County Study Investigators: Other investigators involved in this project include James Anthony, PhD, Erin Bigler, PhD, Ron Brookmeyer, PhD, James Burke, MD, PhD, Eric Christopher, MD, Jane Gagliardi, MD, Robert Green, MD, Michael Helms, BS, Christine Hulette, MD, Ara Khachaturian, PhD, Liz Klein, MPH, Carol Leslie, MS, Constantine Lyketsos, MD, MHS, Lawrence Mayer, MD, John Morris, MD, Ron Munger, PhD, MPH, Chiadi Onyike, MD, MHS, Truls Ostbye, MD, PhD, MPH, Ron Petersen, MD, Carl Pieper, DrPH, Kathy Piercy, PhD, Brenda Plassman, PhD, Peter Rabins, MD, Pritham Raj, MD, Russell Ray, MS, Linda Sanders, MPH, Ingmar Skoog, MD, PhD, David Steffens, MD, MHS, Martin Steinberg, MD, Marty Toohill, PhD, Leslie Toone, MS, Jeannette Townsend, MD, Lauren Warren, PhD, Heidi Wengreen, PhD, Michael Williams, MD, and Bonita Wyse, PhD. Neuropsychological testing and clinical assessment procedures were developed by Drs Welsh-Bohmer and Breitner. Dr Tschanz provided training and oversight of all field staff and reviewed all individual neuropsychological test results to render professional diagnoses. The board-certified or board-eligible geriatric psychiatrists or neurologists who examined the study members included Drs Steinberg, Breitner, Steffens, Lyketsos, Gagliardi, Raj, Christopher, and Green. Dr Williams also examined several subjects and provided expert neurologic consultation. Autopsy examinations were conducted by Dr Townsend. Ms Leslie coordinated the autopsy enrollment program. Diagnosticians at the expert consensus conferences included Drs Breitner, Burke, Lyketsos, Plassman, Steffens, Steinberg, Toohill, Tschanz, and Welsh-Bohmer.
Financial Disclosure: None reported.
Funding/Support: This work was supported by grants R01-AG-11380 (Dr Welsh-Bohmer) and K01-AG-029336 (Dr Hayden) from the National Institutes of Health.
Additional Contributions: The Steering Committee provided thoughtful review of this manuscript. The neurogenetics laboratory of the Joseph and Kathleen Bryan Alzheimer's Disease Research Center at Duke University performed the APOE ε4 genotyping. Cara Brewer, BA, Michelle McCart, BA, Roxane Pfister, MS, Nancy Sassano, PhD, and Sarah Schwartz, MS, provided expert technical assistance.
1.Alzheimer's Association, 2008 Alzheimer's disease facts and figures.
Alzheimers Dement 2008;4
(2)
110- 133
PubMedGoogle Scholar 2.Kung
HCHoyert
DLXu
JQMurphy
SL Deaths: final data for 2005.
Natl Vital Stat Rep 2008;56
(10)
1- 120
PubMedGoogle Scholar 3.Plassman
BLLanga
KMFisher
GG
et al. Prevalence of dementia in the United States: the Aging, Demographics, and Memory Study.
Neuroepidemiology 2007;29
(1-2)
125- 132
PubMedGoogle Scholar 4.Lautenschlager
NTCupples
LARao
VS
et al. Risk of dementia among relatives of Alzheimer's disease patients in the MIRAGE study: what is in store for the oldest old?
Neurology 1996;46
(3)
641- 650
PubMedGoogle Scholar 5.Jayadev
SSteinbart
EJChi
YYKukull
WASchellenberg
GDBird
TD Conjugal Alzheimer disease: risk in children when both parents have Alzheimer disease.
Arch Neurol 2008;65
(3)
373- 378
PubMedGoogle Scholar 6.Goate
AChartier-Harlin
MCMullan
M
et al. Segregation of a missense mutation in the amyloid precursor protein gene with familial Alzheimer's disease.
Nature 1991;349
(6311)
704- 706
PubMedGoogle Scholar 7.Levy-Lahad
EWasco
WPoorkaj
P
et al. Candidate gene for the chromosome 1 familial Alzheimer's disease locus.
Science 1995;269
(5226)
973- 977
PubMedGoogle Scholar 8.Levy-Lahad
EWijsman
EMNemens
E
et al. A familial Alzheimer's disease locus on chromosome 1.
Science 1995;269
(5226)
970- 973
PubMedGoogle Scholar 9.Schellenberg
GDBird
TDWijsman
EM
et al. Genetic linkage evidence for a familial Alzheimer's disease locus on chromosome 14.
Science 1992;258
(5082)
668- 671
PubMedGoogle Scholar 10.Daw
EWPayami
HNemens
EJ
et al. The number of trait loci in late-onset Alzheimer disease.
Am J Hum Genet 2000;66
(1)
196- 204
PubMedGoogle Scholar 11.Corder
EHSaunders
AMStrittmatter
WJ
et al. Gene dose of apolipoprotein E type 4 allele and the risk of Alzheimer's disease in late onset families.
Science 1993;261
(5123)
921- 923
PubMedGoogle Scholar 12.Pericak-Vance
MABebout
JLGaskell
PC
Jr
et al. Linkage studies in familial Alzheimer disease: evidence for chromosome 19 linkage.
Am J Hum Genet 1991;48
(6)
1034- 1050
PubMedGoogle Scholar 13.Saunders
AMStrittmatter
WJSchmechel
D
et al. Association of apolipoprotein E allele epsilon 4 with late-onset familial and sporadic Alzheimer's disease.
Neurology 1993;43
(8)
1467- 1472
PubMedGoogle Scholar 14.Schmechel
DESaunders
AMStrittmatter
WJ
et al. Increased amyloid beta-peptide deposition in cerebral cortex as a consequence of apolipoprotein E genotype in late-onset Alzheimer disease.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1993;90
(20)
9649- 9653
PubMedGoogle Scholar 15.Strittmatter
WJSaunders
AMSchmechel
D
et al. Apolipoprotein E: high-avidity binding to beta-amyloid and increased frequency of type 4 allele in late-onset familial Alzheimer disease.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1993;90
(5)
1977- 1981
PubMedGoogle Scholar 16.Farrer
LACupples
LAHaines
JL
et al. APOE and Alzheimer Disease Meta Analysis Consortium, Effects of age, sex, and ethnicity on the association between apolipoprotein E genotype and Alzheimer disease: a meta-analysis.
JAMA 1997;278
(16)
1349- 1356
PubMedGoogle Scholar 17.Breitner
JCWyse
BWAnthony
JC
et al. APOE-ε4 count predicts age when prevalence of AD increases, then declines: the Cache County Study [published correction appears in
Neurology. 2000;55(1):161-162].
Neurology 1999;53
(2)
321- 331
PubMedGoogle Scholar 18.Meyer
MRTschanz
JTNorton
MC
et al. APOE genotype predicts when—not whether—one is predisposed to develop Alzheimer disease.
Nat Genet 1998;19
(4)
321- 322
PubMedGoogle Scholar 19.Bretsky
PGuralnik
JMLauner
LAlbert
MSeeman
TEMacArthur Studies of Successful Aging, The role of APOE-ε4 in longitudinal cognitive decline: MacArthur Studies of Successful Aging.
Neurology 2003;60
(7)
1077- 1081
PubMedGoogle Scholar 20.Christensen
HBatterham
PJMackinnon
AJ
et al. The association of APOE genotype and cognitive decline in interaction with risk factors in a 65-69 year old community sample [published online July 14, 2008].
BMC Geriatr 2008;814
PubMed10.1186/1471-2318-8-14
Google Scholar 21.Hofer
SMChristensen
HMackinnon
AJ
et al. Change in cognitive functioning associated with apoE genotype in a community sample of older adults.
Psychol Aging 2002;17
(2)
194- 208
PubMedGoogle Scholar 22.Jonker
CSchmand
BLindeboom
JHavekes
LMLauner
LJ Association between apolipoprotein E ε4 and the rate of cognitive decline in community-dwelling elderly individuals with and without dementia.
Arch Neurol 1998;55
(8)
1065- 1069
PubMedGoogle Scholar 23.Mayeux
RSmall
SATang
M-XTycko
BStern
Y Memory performance in healthy elderly without Alzheimer's disease: effects of time and apolipoprotein-E1.
Neurobiol Aging 2001;22
(4)
683- 689
PubMedGoogle Scholar 24.Small
BJBasun
HBackman
L Three-year changes in cognitive performance as a function of apolipoprotein E genotype: evidence from very old adults without dementia.
Psychol Aging 1998;13
(1)
80- 87
PubMedGoogle Scholar 25.Wilson
RSSchneider
JABarnes
LL
et al. The apolipoprotein E ε4 allele and decline in different cognitive systems during a 6-year period.
Arch Neurol 2002;59
(7)
1154- 1160
PubMedGoogle Scholar 26.Yaffe
KCauley
JSands
LBrowner
W Apolipoprotein E phenotype and cognitive decline in a prospective study of elderly community women.
Arch Neurol 1997;54
(9)
1110- 1114
PubMedGoogle Scholar 27.Bunce
DFratiglioni
LSmall
BJWinblad
BBackman
L APOE and cognitive decline in preclinical Alzheimer disease and non-demented aging.
Neurology 2004;63
(5)
816- 821
PubMedGoogle Scholar 28.Dik
MGJonker
CBouter
LMGeerlings
MIvan Kamp
GJDeeg
DJ APOE-ε4 is associated with memory decline in cognitively impaired elderly.
Neurology 2000;54
(7)
1492- 1497
PubMedGoogle Scholar 29.Lange
KLBondi
MWSalmon
DP
et al. Decline in verbal memory during preclinical Alzheimer's disease: examination of the effect of APOE genotype.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2002;8
(7)
943- 955
PubMedGoogle Scholar 30.Winnock
MLetenneur
LJacqmin-Gadda
HDallongeville
JAmouyel
PDartigues
JF Longitudinal analysis of the effect of apolipoprotein E ε4 and education on cognitive performance in elderly subjects: the PAQUID Study.
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2002;72
(6)
794- 797
PubMedGoogle Scholar 31.Bondi
MWSalmon
DPGalasko
DThomas
RGThal
LJ Neuropsychological function and apolipoprotein E genotype in the preclinical detection of Alzheimer's disease.
Psychol Aging 1999;14
(2)
295- 303
PubMedGoogle Scholar 32.Jorm
AFMather
KAButterworth
PAnstey
KJChristensen
HEasteal
S APOE genotype and cognitive functioning in a large age-stratified population sample.
Neuropsychology 2007;21
(1)
1- 8
PubMedGoogle Scholar 33.Miech
RABreitner
JCZandi
PPKhachaturian
ASAnthony
JCMayer
L Incidence of AD may decline in the early 90s for men, later for women: the Cache County Study.
Neurology 2002;58
(2)
209- 218
PubMedGoogle Scholar 34.Teng
ELChui
HC The Modified Mini-Mental State (3MS) examination.
J Clin Psychiatry 1987;48
(8)
314- 318
PubMedGoogle Scholar 35.Tschanz
JTWelsh-Bohmer
KAPlassman
BLNorton
MCWyse
BWBreitner
JCCache County Study Group, An adaptation of the Modified Mini-Mental State Examination: analysis of demographic influences and normative data: the Cache County Study.
Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol 2002;15
(1)
28- 38
PubMedGoogle Scholar 36.Bland
RCNewman
SC Mild dementia or cognitive impairment: the Modified Mini-Mental State Examination (3MS) as a screen for dementia.
Can J Psychiatry 2001;46
(6)
506- 510
PubMedGoogle Scholar 37.Khachaturian
ASGallo
JJBreitner
JC Performance characteristics of a two-stage dementia screen in a population sample.
J Clin Epidemiol 2000;53
(5)
531- 540
PubMedGoogle Scholar 38.Hayden
KMKhachaturian
ASTschanz
JTCorcoran
CNortond
MBreitner
JCCache County Study Group, Characteristics of a two-stage screen for incident dementia.
J Clin Epidemiol 2003;56
(11)
1038- 1045
PubMedGoogle Scholar 39.Welsh-Bohmer
KAOstbye
TSanders
L
et al. Cache County Study Group, Neuropsychological performance in advanced age: influences of demographic factors and apolipoprotein E: findings from the Cache County Memory Study.
Clin Neuropsychol 2009;23
(1)
77- 99
PubMedGoogle Scholar 40.Silverman
JMBreitner
JCMohs
RCDavis
KL Reliability of the family history method in genetic studies of Alzheimer's disease and related dementias.
Am J Psychiatry 1986;143
(10)
1279- 1282
PubMedGoogle Scholar 41.Román
GCTatemichi
TKErkinjuntti
T
et al. Vascular dementia: diagnostic criteria for research studies: report of the NINDS-AIREN International Workshop.
Neurology 1993;43
(2)
250- 260
PubMedGoogle Scholar 42. McKhann
GDrachman
DFolstein
MKatzman
RPrice
DStadlan
EM Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer's Disease.
Neurology 1984;34
(7)
939- 944
PubMedGoogle Scholar 43.American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 3rd rev ed. Washington, DC American Psychiatric Association1987;
44.Breitner
JCWelsh
KAGau
BA
et al. Alzheimer's disease in the National Academy of Sciences–National Research Council Registry of Aging Twin Veterans, III: detection of cases, longitudinal results, and observations on twin concordance.
Arch Neurol 1995;52
(8)
763- 771
PubMedGoogle Scholar 45. SAS/STAT [computer program]. Version 9.1. Cary, NC SAS Institute Inc2003;
46.Huang
WQiu
Cvon Strauss
EWinblad
BFratiglioni
L
APOE genotype, family history of dementia, and Alzheimer disease risk: a 6-year follow-up study [published correction appears in
Arch Neurol. 2005;62(3):453].
Arch Neurol 2004;61
(12)
1930- 1934
PubMedGoogle Scholar 47.Duara
RBarker
WWLopez-Alberola
R
et al. Alzheimer's disease: interaction of apolipoprotein E genotype, family history of dementia, gender, education, ethnicity, and age of onset.
Neurology 1996;46
(6)
1575- 1579
PubMedGoogle Scholar 48.Andrew
MKRockwood
K A five-point change in Modified Mini-Mental State Examination was clinically meaningful in community-dwelling elderly people.
J Clin Epidemiol 2008;61
(8)
827- 831
PubMedGoogle Scholar