Objective
To understand the scope of semantic impairment in semantic dementia.
Design
Case study.
Setting
Academic medical center.
Patient
A man with semantic dementia, as demonstrated by clinical, neuropsychological, and imaging studies.
Main Outcome Measures
Music performance and magnetic resonance imaging results.
Results
Despite profoundly impaired semantic memory for words and objects due to left temporal lobe atrophy, this semiprofessional musician was creative and expressive in demonstrating preserved musical knowledge.
Conclusion
Long-term representations of words and objects in semantic memory may be dissociated from meaningful knowledge in other domains, such as music.
Semantic dementia (SemD) is a primary progressive form of aphasia characterized by impaired semantic memory affecting confrontation naming and the interpretation of word and object meaning.1 Two recent cases of relatively preserved musical knowledge in SemD have been reported,2,3 although the absence of detailed analyses limits our ability to learn from these patients. We describe preserved ability to perform music in a 64-year-old semiprofessional harpsichordist with SemD. Analysis of his performance of pieces from the Baroque period that were familiar and unfamiliar to him revealed remarkably preserved musical expression abilities and creativity despite profound difficulty with word and object meaning and significant left anterior temporal atrophy. These observations are consistent with a modular approach to semantic memory that dissociates long-term representations of word and object meaning from meaningful knowledge in other domains, such as music.
Our proband is a right-handed computer programmer with 20 years of educational attainment. He was diagnosed with SemD at age 59 years, ie, 5 years after symptom onset and 5 years prior to this study. Initial evaluation revealed word-finding difficulty, impaired confrontation naming, poor comprehension of familiar words such as “thumb” and “stool,” and surface dyslexia. Visual-perceptual skills, praxis, episodic memory, and mathematical abilities had been spared. Comprehension of words and objects gradually declined, and word-finding became increasingly limited. At the time of musical assessment on October 3, 2008 (which had been approved by University of Pennsylvania Institutional Review Board), our proband had virtually no comprehension of oral or written language and was mute. He mishandled objects such as a corkscrew and a tuning fork, appearing not to understand their intended functions. He continued to perform multidigit written calculations rapidly and accurately and easily copied a complex geometric design. His social comportment was generally appropriate, although he had begun to demonstrate mild euphoria, as manifested in actions such as overly exuberant greetings. He followed established daily routines that could be interrupted by his wife without causing him distress. Longitudinal neuropsychological testing is summarized in eTable 1. Quantitative analysis of structural magnetic resonance imaging in our proband relative to 15 age-matched control individuals showed significant left anterior temporal cortical atrophy (P < .05 error-corrected familywise; Figure, A and eTable 2).
Musical study of our proband
Our proband had begun playing piano at age 8 and had studied organ throughout college before turning to the harpsichord. He became skilled in Baroque music performance involving embellishments with ornaments (eg, trills and rhythmic variation) that are minimally notated in the score. He participated in master classes, lectured on public radio, and performed for 26 years. Since his retirement at the time of diagnosis, he has played harpsichord for 2 hours daily.
Herein, we report digital recordings of our proband's performances and analyses of several pieces he played from scores during two 40-minute sessions. These pieces included 9 French Baroque works he had selected, ranging from frequently played favorites to pieces he had not played in more than a decade. These complex and technically demanding pieces were played with rich, varied, skillfully executed ornamentation. Appropriate and expressive phrasing was clearly present. His rhythm was occasionally uneven, but wrong notes were rare.
He also played 7 brief works we provided. Although our proband may have heard these works previously, they were not part of his repertoire and they were unfamiliar to his wife, thus presumably requiring him to sight-read them. In the representative piece we analyze in detail herein, (Minuet in F, no. 3, from Johann Sebastian Bach's Notebooks for Anna Magdalena Bach), our proband's error rate was minimal: of a total of 260 notes, he played 1 (0.4%) wrong and omitted 3 (1.2%); his playing was rhythmically unstable in 4 of 32 (12.5%) measures. He produced novel, stylistically appropriate embellishments in 11 (34.4%) of the measures. These included creative and expressive additions of ornamentation (7 instances), rhythmic variation (5 instances), and emphases on melodic or harmonic structure (2 instances). Figure, B shows 4 examples. In measure 16 (part 1), our proband extended the melody line to emphasize a harmonic modulation. In measure 21 (part 2), he enriched texture by filling in melodic gaps and adding rhythmic variation. In measure 23 (part 3), he altered the rhythmic pattern of 3 notes written as equal beats, a stylistic practice known as notes inégales. In measures 31 and 32, which end the piece (part 4), he embellished the melody line and emphasized the closing of the work by repeating each of the 2 final bass-line notes an octave lower. When playing the same piece 2 months later, his embellishments differed. In measures 15 and 16, for example, he originally added notes in measure 16 but not in measure 15 (part 1); in the subsequent performance (part 5), he added notes in measure 15 but not in measure 16 (contrast the “as played” portions of parts 1 and 5).
Our proband also demonstrated his understanding of formal music structure in his embellished performance of a familiar piece (Suite in C Major: Passacaille, by François Couperin) containing an alternating theme-and-variation pattern. He differentiated the notationally identical initial and final presentations of the theme with a retard, ie, a gradual slowing of the tempo to mark the close of the work. His final presentation of the theme was 27% longer than in the initial presentation (16.81 vs 13.12 seconds, respectively). The longer duration of the final presentation represents gradual changes culminating in a lengthened final measure, not simply a consistently slower tempo. The first, penultimate, and final measures of the 8-measure theme lasted 1.46, 1.57, and 1.40 seconds, respectively, when introduced at the beginning of the piece, whereas the corresponding measures lasted 1.25, 1.89, and 4.36 seconds, respectively, when played at the end of the piece. Moreover, our proband emphasized the retard by slowing the ornamentation: the theme's closing measure contained a rolled chord, which he played at half speed in the final compared with the initial presentation (0.44 vs 0.22 seconds, respectively).
Our proband performed technically demanding, structurally complex compositions in an expressive manner. Beyond the motor skills required in playing a keyboard instrument and the visuoperceptual skills required to read scores, his performance erred minimally with regard to the cognitive skills involved in interpreting symbolic notation. Most significantly, he created novel, varied, stylistically appropriate embellishments that were not specified in the score. This was observed during unfamiliar pieces, emphasizing that his musical knowledge does not depend on episodic memory. Our proband's varied embellishments throughout the period of the study also demonstrate that his retained musical ability is not procedural (ie, habitually striking keys in response to visual stimuli) but reflect his meaningful and productive control over musical knowledge.
These observations help clarify the nature of semantic memory loss in SemD. Our findings are consistent with the hypothesis that the semantic impairment in SemD targets a particular type of concept without robbing these patients of all meaningful knowledge. Specifically, patients with SemD, unlike the general population, appear to maintain semantic memory for abstract concepts (such as “justice”) relatively better than concrete object concepts (such as “chair”).4-7 Semantic impairment in SemD thus appears to manifest most markedly regarding visually based concepts such as concrete objects and their names. This may occur partly because of the degradation of visuoperceptual features of object concepts represented in the visual association cortex of the compromised temporal lobe.7,8 Whereas there are many theories of musical meaning, musical semantics depends partly on understanding the structural attributes of a musical piece9-12 without ties to concrete visual referents and thus partly may resemble the semantics of abstract concepts. Consistent with this argument, we found that key elements of musical semantics are preserved in our proband, who has profound difficulty understanding object concepts owing to anterior temporal disease. This unique feature was difficult to discern in previous reports of preserved music in SemD. One case individual continued musical composition and performance for 1 year following the onset of diminished language comprehension but details of his musical and semantic abilities were not provided.2 A second patient with SemD was an untrained musician able to hum familiar, popular songs.3
Neuroimaging studies of the neurobiology of music implicate frontal, parietal, and posterior temporal regions.13 These areas are largely preserved in our proband, particularly in the right hemisphere, and thus are consistent with the possibility that musical knowledge may be relatively preserved in SemD. Preserved knowledge also has been observed in other domains that rely on less-affected cortical areas in SemD. For instance, the meaning of number concepts appears to depend largely on the parietal cortex,14 an area relatively spared in SemD, and competence with number knowledge has been demonstrated in these patients.15,16 These observations, along with those of our case study, are consistent with the hypothesis that knowledge represented in semantic memory is partly modular, dissociating long-term representations of object and word concepts from meaningful knowledge in other domains, such as music.
Correspondence: Murray Grossman, MD, Department of Neurology, University of Pennsylvania School of Medicine, Gibson Bldg, 2nd Fl, 3400 Spruce St, Philadelphia, PA 19104-4283 (mgrossma@mail.med.upenn.edu).
Accepted for Publication: April 22, 2010.
Author Contributions: Drs Weinstein, Koenig, McMillan, Bonner, and Grossman had full access to all the data in the study and take responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis. Study concept and design: Weinstein, Koenig, Bonner, and Grossman. Acquisition of data: Koenig and Grossman. Analysis and interpretation of data: Weinstein, Koeng, Gunawardena, McMillan, and Grossman. Drafting of the manuscript: Weinstein, Koenig, Gunawardena, McMillan, and Grossman. Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Weinstein, Koenig, and Grossman. Statistical analysis: Weinstein, Koenig, McMillan, and Grossman. Obtained funding: Grossman. Administrative, technical, and material support: Weinstein and Grossman. Study supervision: Koenig and Grossman.
Funding/Support: This case report was supported in part by the National Institutes of Health (grants AG17586, AG15116, NS44266, AG32953, and NS53488).
Previous Presentations: Portions of this study were presented at the annual meeting of the American Academy of Neurology; April 28, 2009, Seattle, Washington.
Financial Disclosure: Dr Grossman is a consultant for Allon Therapeutics Inc, Forest Laboratories Inc, and Pfizer Inc.
2.Miller
BLBoone
KCummings
JLRead
SLMishkin
F Functional correlates of musical and visual ability in frontotemporal dementia.
Br J Psychiatry 2000;176458- 463
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 3.Hailstone
JCOmar
RWarren
JD Relatively preserved knowledge of music in semantic dementia.
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2009;80
(7)
808- 809
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 4.Yi
H-AMoore
PGrossman
M Reversal of the concreteness effect for verbs in patients with semantic dementia.
Neuropsychology 2007;21
(1)
9- 19
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 5.Breedin
SDSaffran
EMCoslett
HB Reversal of a concreteness effect in a patient with semantic dementia.
Cogn Neuropsychol 1995;11617- 660
Google ScholarCrossref 7.Bonner
MFVesely
LPrice
C
et al. Reversal of the concreteness effect in semantic dementia.
Cogn Neuropsychol 2009;26
(6)
568- 579
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 8.Grossman
MMcMillan
CMoore
P
et al. What's in a name: voxel-based morphometric analyses of MRI and naming difficulty in Alzheimer's disease, frontotemporal dementia and corticobasal degeneration.
Brain 2004;127
(pt 3)
628- 649
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 9.Koelsch
SKasper
ESammler
DSchulze
KGunter
TFriederici
AD Music, language and meaning: brain signatures of semantic processing.
Nat Neurosci 2004;7
(3)
302- 307
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 10.Meyer
LB Emotion and Meaning in Music. Chicago, Illinois University of Chicago Press1961;
13.Zatorre
RJChen
JLPenhune
VB When the brain plays music: auditory-motor interactions in music perception and production.
Nat Rev Neurosci 2007;8
(7)
547- 558
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 15.Halpern
CHGlosser
GClark
R
et al. Dissociation of numbers and objects in corticobasal degeneration and semantic dementia.
Neurology 2004;62
(7)
1163- 1169
PubMedGoogle Scholar 16.Cappelletti
MButterworth
BKopelman
MD Spared numerical abilities in a case of semantic dementia.
Neuropsychologia 2001;39
(11)
1224- 1239
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref