Key PointsQuestion
What is the association between hearing impairment and neurocognitive function in survivors of childhood cancer?
Findings
In this cross-sectional study of 1520 childhood cancer survivors, more than one-third who were treated with ototoxic therapy had severe hearing impairment. Survivors with severe hearing impairment were at an increased risk for neurocognitive deficits, independent of neurotoxic therapy, compared with survivors with normal hearing or with mild hearing impairment.
Meaning
These findings indicate that severe hearing impairment after ototoxic therapy appears to be associated with neurocognitive deficits.
Importance
Despite advancements in cancer therapy and supportive care, childhood cancer survivors remain at risk for chronic morbidities associated with disease and treatment, such as hearing impairment (HI) and neurocognitive deficits. This study, to our knowledge, is the first to objectively measure hearing and neurocognitive function in a large cohort of long-term survivors of childhood cancer stratified by treatment exposures.
Objective
To assess the association of HI with neurocognitive function and the factors in HI that mediate neurocognitive outcomes in survivors of childhood cancer.
Design, Setting, and Participants
Data analyzed in this cross-sectional study were collected for the period April 25, 2007, to June 30, 2017, from participants in the St. Jude Lifetime Cohort Study (SJLIFE), an ongoing study that quantifies the long-term health outcomes of survivors of childhood cancer. Participants included those treated at St. Jude Children’s Research Hospital (Memphis, Tennessee) for childhood cancer who survived 5 or more years after their original diagnosis and who were eligible for audiologic and neurocognitive testing. Hearing outcomes were coded using the Chang Ototoxicity Grading Scale. Data analysis was performed from March 22, 2019, to March 5, 2020.
Main Outcomes and Measures
Hearing and neurocognitive function. Survivors were grouped by hearing sensitivity (normal hearing [Chang grade 0], mild HI [Chang grades 1a, 1b, and 2a], or severe HI [Chang grade ≥2b]) and stratified by treatment exposure (platinum-only exposure group [treated with cisplatin and/or carboplatin chemotherapy], cochlear radiotherapy [RT] exposure group [treated with cochlear RT with or without platinum-based chemotherapy], or no exposure group [no platinum-based chemotherapy or cochlear RT]). Multivariable log-binomial models were adjusted for age at diagnosis, time since diagnosis, sex, and relevant treatment exposures.
Results
A total of 1520 survivors of childhood cancer were analyzed, among whom 814 were male survivors (53.6%), the median (interquartile range [IQR]) age was 29.4 (7.4-64.7) years, and the median (IQR) time since diagnosis was 20.4 (6.1-53.8) years. Prevalence and risk of severe HI among survivors were higher in survivors in the platinum-only (n = 107 [34.9%]; relative risk [RR], 1.68 [95% CI, 1.20-2.37]) or cochlear RT (n = 181 [38.3%]; RR, 2.69 [95% CI, 2.02-3.57) exposure group compared with those in the no exposure group (n = 65 [8.8%]). Severe HI was associated with deficits in verbal reasoning skills (no exposure group RR, 1.11 [95% CI, 0.50-2.43]; platinum-only exposure group RR, 1.93 [95% CI, 1.21-3.08]; cochlear RT exposure group RR, 2.00 [95% CI, 1.46-2.75]), verbal fluency (no exposure group RR, 1.86 [95% CI, 1.19-2.91]; platinum-only exposure group RR, 1.83 [95% CI, 1.24-2.71]; cochlear RT exposure group RR, 1.45 [95% CI, 1.09-1.94]), visuomotor speed (no exposure group RR, 1.87 [95% CI, 1.07-3.25]; platinum-only exposure group RR, 3.10 [95% CI, 1.92-4.99]; cochlear RT exposure group RR, 1.40 [95% CI, 1.11-1.78]), and mathematics skills (no exposure group RR, 1.90 [95% CI, 1.18-3.04]; platinum-only exposure group RR, 1.63 [95% CI, 1.05-2.53]; cochlear RT exposure group RR, 1.58 [95% CI, 1.15-2.18]), compared with survivors with normal hearing or with mild HI.
Conclusions and Relevance
Results of this study suggest that severe HI in childhood cancer survivors is associated with neurocognitive deficits independent of the neurotoxic treatment received. Early screening and intervention for HI may facilitate the development and maintenance of neurocognitive function and identify individuals at risk for impairment.
Long-term survival from childhood cancers now exceeds 85% in the United States.1 Despite major advancements in cancer therapy and supportive care, childhood cancer survivors remain at risk for chronic morbidities associated with disease and treatment, such as hearing impairment (HI)2,3 and neurocognitive deficits.4-6 Among survivors, those who received cranial radiotherapy (RT) for central nervous system (CNS) malignant neoplasms experience the highest risk for neurocognitive dysfunction.6-10 Treatment-induced HI is associated with platinum-based chemotherapy11-15 or radiotherapy directed to the cochlea16-18 and is typically bilateral, permanent, and progressive.12-17
The association between HI and neurocognitive deficits in healthy children has been reported in the literature19-22 but has only recently been examined in childhood cancer survivors. Gurney et al23 observed that child survivors of neuroblastoma with parent-reported HI were twice as likely to have difficulties with reading, mathematics, and/or attention; required more special educational services; and experienced worse quality of life than those with normal hearing. Specific areas of inferior cognition associated with these poor outcomes were not identified. In a retrospective study of 5937 adult survivors of non-CNS cancers, HI was associated with diminished task efficiency, organization, memory, and emotional regulation24; however, neurocognitive functioning and HI were self-reported rather than clinically assessed. Previous studies of the implication of HI for pediatric CNS tumor survivors treated with platinum-based chemotherapy and cranial RT demonstrated an independent association between HI and low-level neurocognitive performance.25-27
The association of HI with neurocognitive function in childhood cancer survivors who were not treated with cranial RT has not been reported. The goals of the current study were to describe the prevalence, severity, and risk of objectively assessed HI in a large cohort of childhood cancer survivors; to assess the association of HI with neurocognitive function; and to examine the HI factors that mediate neurocognitive outcomes among survivors treated with cranial RT.
This cross-sectional study was approved by St. Jude Children's Research Hospital Institutional Review Board. All participants provided written informed consent. Data were collected for the period April 25, 2007, to June 30, 2017. Data analysis was performed from March 22, 2019, to March 5, 2020.
The present study analyzed data of eligible survivors who participate in the St. Jude Lifetime Cohort Study (SJLIFE), which has been previously described.28 Briefly, SJLIFE is an ongoing institutional follow-up study designed to quantify the long-term health outcomes of survivors of childhood cancer. These participants included individuals treated at St. Jude Children’s Research Hospital (Memphis, Tennessee) for childhood cancer who survived 5 or more years after their original diagnosis and who were eligible for audiologic and neurocognitive testing. Of the 1678 eligible survivors we identified, 137 did not participate in the present study, 17 had preexisting or non–treatment-induced HI, and 4 did not have evaluable audiologic data. A total of 1520 participants were evaluable for this cross-sectional study (Figure 1).
Neurocognitive and Audiologic Assessments
Survivors completed a standard battery of developmentally and age-appropriate neurocognitive assessments, which were administered by certified examiners under the general supervision of a board-certified neuropsychologist (K.R.K.). Assessment measures were categorized under these main domains: intelligence,29,30 attention,31-34 memory,35,36 executive function,32-34,37 processing speed,32,33,38,39 and academic function.40 Survivors were permitted to wear hearing aids during testing; measures for which the examiner observed that HI interfered with the validity of the test were not included in analyses. Age-adjusted z scores were calculated, and deficits were defined as z scores of –1.28 or lower, which were equivalent to the 10th percentile of the normative distribution. All subtests for each neurocognitive domain are listed in eTable 1 in the Supplement.
Survivors completed otoscopy, tympanometry, pure-tone audiometry, and speech audiometry. Pure-tone air conduction thresholds were measured at 0.25, 0.5, 1, 2, 3, 4, 6, and 8 kHz, and bone conduction thresholds were assessed at 0.25, 0.5, 1, 2, 3, and 4 kHz to establish the type of HI (ie, conductive, sensorineural, or mixed). Each audiogram was assigned a grade using the Chang Ototoxicity Grading Scale41 (grades range from 0-4, with the highest grade indicating the most severe HI) (eTable 2 in the Supplement).
For the present study, we assigned a Chang grade to permanent conductive or mixed HI on the basis of air conduction thresholds rather than bone conduction measurements to accurately reflect the severity of HI. We graded sensorineural and temporary conductive or mixed hearing losses on the basis of bone conduction thresholds or air conduction thresholds with a normal tympanogram result. Chang grades were categorized as follows: 0 for normal hearing; 1a, 1b, and 2a for mild HI; and 2b or higher for severe HI. The grade for the better-hearing ear was used for survivors with asymmetrical HI, and the result of the audiologic evaluation conducted closest to the neurocognitive assessment date was used for survivors with multiple examinations. The audiologist (J.K.B.) was unaware of the survivor’s neurocognitive function at the time of testing and at the time of assigning a Chang grade. Adherence to the use of a hearing intervention (ie, hearing aid or cochlear implant) was self-reported by survivors.
We used summary statistics to describe demographic (including investigator-identified categories of race/ethnicity) and treatment characteristics of survivors. Survivors were grouped by HI severity (Chang grade <2b vs ≥2b) and treatment exposure (platinum-only exposure group [cisplatin and/or carboplatin chemotherapy], cochlear radiotherapy [RT] exposure group [cochlear RT with or without platinum-based chemotherapy], and no exposure group [no platinum-based chemotherapy or cochlear RT]). Multivariable log-binomial models with a modified Poisson approach were used to examine the relative risk (RR) by treatment exposure for severe HI, adjusting for age at diagnosis, time since diagnosis, sex, and cisplatin or carboplatin chemotherapy treatment (yes or no). A standardized incidence ratio was calculated by comparing the proportion of severe HI in survivors (grouped by age) who were not exposed to ototoxic treatment with the high-frequency HI data in the general adult population of the 2011 to 2012 National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey,42,43 to assess whether the rate of HI differed from that of the survivor reference group. Within each treatment exposure group, multivariable log-binomial models were used to examine the associations between severe HI (vs normal hearing and mild HI) and neurocognitive deficits, adjusting for age at diagnosis, time since diagnosis, sex, and neurotoxic treatment variables if existing within the group (ie, cranial RT dose, intrathecal methotrexate sodium, high-dose intravenous methotrexate, and high-dose intravenous cytarabine chemotherapy). A summary of outcomes, factors, and covariates are presented in the Box.
Box Section Ref IDBox.
Factors Associated With Neurocognitive Deficits in Survivors of Childhood Cancer and Covariates Used in Multivariable Log-Binomial Models
No exposure groupa
Factors:
Severe HI
High-dose IV methotrexate (yes or no)
Intrathecal methotrexate (yes or no)
High-dose IV cytarabine chemotherapy (yes or no)
Covariates:
Age at diagnosis, in years
Time since diagnosis, in years
Sex
Platinum-only exposure groupb
Factor:
Covariates:
Age at diagnosis, in years
Time since diagnosis, in years
Sex
Cochlear RT exposure groupc
Factors:
Severe HI
Cranial RT dose (continuous)
High-dose IV methotrexate (yes or no)
Intrathecal methotrexate (yes or no)
Covariates:
Age at diagnosis, in years
Time since diagnosis, in years
Sex
Abbreviations: HI, hearing impairment; IV, intravenous; RT, radiotherapy.
a No exposure group (n = 740) comprised survivors who were not treated with platinum-based chemotherapy or cochlear RT.
b Platinum-only exposure group (n = 307) comprised survivors who were treated with cisplatin and/or carboplatin chemotherapy.
c Cochlear RT exposure group (n = 473) comprised survivors who were treated with cochlear RT with or without platinum-based chemotherapy.
We performed a mediation (path) analysis to examine the association between cranial RT and neurocognitive deficits mediated by severe HI. To prepare for path analysis, we conducted multivariable log-binomial models to examine the associations between neurocognitive deficits and potential risk factors, including severe HI (vs normal hearing and mild HI), age at diagnosis, time since diagnosis, sex, and neurotoxic treatment variables if existing within the group (ie, cranial RT dose, intrathecal methotrexate [yes or no], high-dose intravenous methotrexate [yes or no], and high-dose intravenous cytarabine chemotherapy [yes or no]). Any risk factor that was statistically significantly associated with neurocognitive deficits was considered a factor for direct association in the path analysis. Any neurocognitive deficit that was statistically significantly associated with both severe HI and cranial RT dose observed from the multivariable analysis was considered in these models. Other factors that were statistically significantly associated with neurocognitive dysfunction and severe HI were considered covariates, such as platinum-based chemotherapy (yes or no), age at diagnosis per year, and time since diagnosis per year.
All statistical tests were unpaired, 2-tailed tests. All P values were 2-sided, and P = .05 was used to indicate statistical significance. The path analysis was conducted using Mplus, version 8.2 (Muthén & Muthén). All other analyses were performed with SAS, version 9.4 (SAS Institute Inc).
A total of 1520 participants in SJLIFE were included in this cross-sectional study. Of these survivors, the median (interquartile range [IQR]) age was 29.4 (7.4-64.7) years and the median (IQR) time since diagnosis was 20.4 (6.1-53.8) years. This cohort comprised 706 female survivors (46.5%) and 814 male survivors (53.6%).
Demographic and treatment characteristics of survivors in each treatment exposure group are provided in eTable 3 in the Supplement. Survivors who were not exposed to platinum-based chemotherapy or cochlear RT (n = 740) did not differ from survivors treated with platinum-based chemotherapy only (n = 307) in sex (female: 359 [48.5%] vs 154 [50.2%]; male: 381 [51.5%] vs 153 [49.8%]), race/ethnicity (eg, white: 597 [80.7%] vs 233 [75.9%]), median (IQR) age at diagnosis (6.2 [2.9-12.6] years vs 5.4 [1.3-12.3] years), median (IQR) age at follow-up (27.6 [20.1-36.6] years vs 27.2 [20.0-34.7] years), and median (IQR) time since diagnosis (18.6 [12.0-28.7] years vs 19.7 [13.3-25.5] years). Survivors treated with cochlear RT (>1 Gy) (n = 473) were slightly older at diagnosis (median [IQR] age, 7.9 [4.1-12.5] years) and had longer follow-up time (median [IQR] age at follow-up, 32.6 [27.0-39.6] years) compared with survivors in the no exposure and platinum-only exposure groups.
The distribution of HI for survivors by treatment exposure group is presented in the Table. The frequency of mild HI was 7.3% (n = 54) among the survivors in the no exposure group, 20.2% (n = 62) in the platinum-only exposure group, and 22.2% (n = 105) in the cochlear RT exposure group. Risk for developing severe HI was higher among survivors in the platinum-only exposure group (107 [34.9%]; RR, 1.68 [95% CI, 1.20-2.37]) and cochlear RT exposure group (181 [38.3%]; RR, 2.69 [95% CI, 2.02-3.57]) compared with those in the no exposure group (65 [8.8%]) (Table and eTable 4 in the Supplement). Of note, among the 138 survivors with normal hearing in the platinum-only exposure group, 96 (69.6%) were treated with carboplatin chemotherapy only. Most survivors with mild HI (41 of 62 [66.1%]) or severe HI (78 of 107 [72.9%]) in the platinum-only exposure group received cisplatin chemotherapy only. Severe HI in the no exposure group was more prevalent in survivors aged 7 to 39 years compared with the general US population (age 7-29 years standardized incidence ratio, 2.34 [95% CI, 1.45-3.57]; age 30-39 years standardized incidence ratio, 2.59 [95% CI, 1.45-4.27]) (eTable 5 in the Supplement).
The rate of severe HI among survivors in the no exposure group was likely elevated because survivors with HI that was considered permanently conductive, noise-induced, or aminoglycoside-related were included in the analyses. Moreover, the Chang Ototoxicity Grading Scale is more sensitive in detecting HI than the criterion used in the 2011 to 2012 National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey.42 Thus, more cases of HI were identified in the present cohort. Among the 330 survivors with severe HI for whom a hearing aid was recommended, 75 (22.7%) reported using a hearing aid or cochlear implant.
Mean z scores and number of survivors with impaired neurocognitive function for each of the 15 tests are listed by treatment exposure group in eTable 6 in the Supplement. After adjusting for chemotherapy, cranial RT dose, and other relevant covariates, statistically significant differences in neurocognitive performance were observed between survivors with and without severe HI across treatment exposure groups (Figure 2). Compared with those with normal hearing or with mild HI, survivors with severe HI were at higher risk for deficits on assessment measures that were heavily language dependent, such as verbal fluency (no exposure group: RR, 1.86 [95% CI, 1.19-2.91]; platinum-only exposure group: RR, 1.83 [95% CI, 1.24-2.71]; and cochlear RT exposure group: RR, 1.45 [95% CI, 1.09-1.94]), verbal reasoning skills (no exposure group: RR, 1.11 [95% CI, 0.50-2.43]; platinum-only exposure group: RR, 1.93 [95% CI, 1.21-3.08]; cochlear RT exposure group: RR, 2.00 [95% CI, 1.46-2.75]), word reading skills (no exposure group: RR, 1.76 [95% CI, 0.69-4.48]; platinum-only exposure group: RR, 3.47 [95% CI, 1.56-7.73]; and cochlear RT exposure group: RR, 2.31 [95% CI, 1.36-3.92]), and mathematical computation skills (no exposure group: RR, 1.90 [95% CI, 1.18-3.04]; platinum-only exposure group: RR, 1.63 [95% CI, 1.05-2.53]; and cochlear RT exposure group: RR, 1.58 [95% CI, 1.15-2.18]) (Figure 2).
Performance on measures that were less language dependent, such as attention, executive function, and processing speed, was also associated with severe HI. Compared with those with normal hearing or with mild HI, survivors with severe HI in the platinum-only and cochlear RT exposure groups were more likely to have impaired focused attention (RRs, 2.56 [95% CI, 1.45-4.52] and 1.57 [95% CI, 1.16-2.14]) and cognitive flexibility (RRs, 1.64 [95% CI, 1.15-2.34] and 1.34 [95% CI, 1.06-1.68]). The presence of severe HI in survivors in all 3 treatment exposure groups was significantly associated with slower visuomotor speed (no exposure group: RR, 1.87 [95% CI, 1.07-3.25]; platinum-only exposure group: RR, 3.10 [95% CI, 1.92-4.99]; and cochlear RT exposure group: RR, 1.40 [95% CI, 1.11-1.78]). Survivors with severe HI showed 55% higher risk (RR, 1.55; 95% CI, 1.14-2.10) in the platinum-only exposure group and 25% higher risk (RR, 1.25; 95% CI, 1.04-1.49) in the cochlear RT exposure group for inadequate fine motor speed (Figure 2) compared with survivors without severe HI. Even survivors with milder forms of HI showed a 110% to 247% increased risk for neurocognitive dysfunction in the domains of attention, executive function, processing speed, and intelligence compared with normal-hearing survivors (eFigure 1 in the Supplement).
Mediation Analysis of the Association Between Cranial RT and Neurocognitive Outcomes
Results of the path analysis model for verbal fluency in survivors exposed to cranial RT are presented in eFigure 2 in the Supplement. Adjusting for age at diagnosis, cranial RT dose (standardized parameter estimate, 0.156; P = .03) and severe HI (standardized parameter estimate, 0.271; P = .002) had a direct association with verbal fluency. The association of cranial RT with verbal fluency was also statistically significantly mediated by severe HI (standardized parameter estimate, 0.257; P = .001). Direct and indirect statistically significant associations of other neurocognitive outcomes with severe HI and cranial RT dose are presented in eTable 7 in the Supplement. The amount of variance in the association of cranial RT with neurocognitive outcome that was accounted for by severe HI was 20% for verbal memory, 27% for cognitive flexibility, 33% for verbal fluency, 17% for visuomotor speed, 25% for fine motor speed, and 36% for word reading (eTable 7 in the Supplement).
To our knowledge, the present study is the first to objectively measure hearing sensitivity and task-specific neurocognitive function in a large cohort of long-term survivors of childhood cancer stratified by treatment exposures. Consistent with our expectations, survivors with severe HI demonstrated impaired function on 1 or more neurocognitive tests. Adjusting for age at diagnosis, time since diagnosis, sex, and relevant treatment factors, neurocognitive deficits were greater for survivors with severe HI vs normal hearing or mild HI, specifically for measures assessing executive function, processing speed, academic function, and intelligence.
Considerable evidence has been published of children with congenital or prelingual HI or deafness consistently performing worse in language,44,45 reading,46-48 mathematics,49-51 and overall academic achievement52 compared with peers with normal hearing. However, studies evaluating these academic measures in children with acquired HI are limited. Previous research found that approximately half of children with prelingual severe to profound HI were unable to read beyond the fourth-grade level at the time of high school graduation,47 with reading abilities not exceeding sixth-grade level for college students with HI.48,53 Orgel et al26 observed an association between substantial treatment-induced HI and specific neurocognitive deficits among those who survived pediatric brain tumor. Expanding on this research, Olivier et al27 examined specific neurocognitive skills important to reading in children with embryonal brain tumors. The present study expanded the work by Orgel et al26 and Olivier et al27 by showing that survivors who developed severe HI after platinum-based chemotherapy without cochlear RT performed worse on language-based measures, such as verbal intelligence, verbal fluency, and single-word reading compared with survivors with normal hearing or with mild HI. Furthermore, the results show that the association of HI with neurocognitive function does not subside but rather continues well into adulthood.
Poor reading skills have an association with pervasive, inferior overall academic achievement,54 hindering comprehension and the acquisition of higher-level reasoning ability. In a prospective study examining neurocognitive and academic outcomes in pediatric patients treated for medulloblastoma, Schreiber et al25 demonstrated that substantial HI was independently associated with a decline in intellectual and academic skills. Reading and linguistic difficulties in children with HI may expand to difficulties in other areas of academics, including mathematics. Although mathematics is a subject heavily dependent on visualization of figures and symbolic representation, children with HI have been found to consistently underperform in mathematics.51,55-57 In a study by Kelly et al,51 a significant association was observed between language proficiency and reading grade level and mathematical achievement among deaf college students. Consistent with the research by Kelly et al,51 the present study found that survivors with severe HI across all 3 treatment exposure groups were at an increased risk for lower mathematical proficiency. Research has suggested that deaf and hard-of-hearing students had great difficulty with mathematical vocabulary because many words used in testing were abstract, were technical, had multiple meanings, or were represented by abbreviations or symbols.58 These data underscore the need for a more systematic method of teaching fundamental language concepts, such as morphological skills, to children with HI, with the intent of improving linguistic knowledge and generalization of language skills to enable the performance of higher-level neurocognitive tasks, such as mathematics.51
Findings from this cross-sectional study indicated that survivors with severe HI were at an increased risk for deficits not only in verbal abilities but also in less verbally dependent skills such as focused attention, visual memory, executive functioning, and processing speed. Similar results have been reported in studies of children19 and adults59 with HI but without cancer. One theory addressing this finding concluded that auditory deprivation may lead to cortical reorganization between the temporal and frontal lobes of the brain and can alter the development of certain skills such as executive function and memory, particularly in young children with HI.60 A number of studies have suggested an association between language and executive function,61-63 and a recent study by Botting et al64 reinforced the idea of language being a key component in the development of optimal executive functioning skills in deaf children.
Survivors with mild HI in this study also demonstrated deficits in performing neurocognitive tasks compared with those with normal hearing, albeit to a lesser degree than survivors with severe HI. Studies in school-aged children with mild HI found an increased risk for diminished educational performance, attention, and communication skills,65,66 and Lin et al59 reported that older adults with mild HI experienced greater neurocognitive decline compared with individuals with normal hearing. A recent study also observed functional changes in the neural processing of auditory signals in children with mild to moderate HI, suggesting that even milder forms of HI can lead to structural and functional brain reorganization,67 further supporting the need for early intervention and management of HI.
As expected, the present study confirmed that cranial RT has a direct association with neurocognitive function; however, this study is the first, to our knowledge, to demonstrate that severe HI mediates a substantial portion of the association between cranial RT and neurocognitive deficits. This finding supports the need for long-term audiologic follow-up and early HI detection and intervention in a population already at risk for neurocognitive deficits. A neuropsychological consultation may help identify areas of low performance sooner and thus prevent or mitigate the association between HI and neurocognition.
Strengths and Limitations
This study has several strengths. It included a large sample size, a long-term follow-up, and high-quality and standardized audiologic and neurocognitive assessments. It also had a high participation rate.
This study also has some limitations. First, the cross-sectional study design precluded the identification of the onset of neurocognitive difficulties and HI. However, it is probable that the significant associations involved HI preceding neurocognitive dysfunction given that current neurodevelopmental theories do not provide an explanation for how neurocognitive difficulties are associated with HI. Furthermore, we excluded from analyses those survivors with a preexisting neurodevelopmental or genetic condition or non–treatment-induced neurological injury associated with neurocognitive deficits. Prospective screening and intervention are needed nonetheless to fully understand the developmental sequence of these problems. Second, pretreatment neurocognitive assessment results were not available for survivors included in the study. Baseline neurocognitive testing is often not feasible and likely not a valid indication of typical premorbid functioning in survivors of solid tumors or CNS tumors attributed to young age and life-threatening illness at the time of diagnosis. Furthermore, many cognitive skills (eg, executive function) cannot be tested in a 2- or 3-year-old child.
Third, data for HI that was considered conductive, noise-induced, or aminoglycoside-related were not available and could not be included in the analyses. Fourth, only a small proportion of survivors for whom a hearing aid was recommended reported using a hearing aid, and data on key variables (eg, years of hearing aid use, hearing aid adherence, and appropriateness of hearing aid fitting) were not available. Thus, we had limited ability to adequately compare neurocognitive function between those who used a hearing aid and those who did not. Studies of the association of hearing aid use with neurocognitive function in children with HI are sparse; however, such studies in older adults with HI who use hearing aids have demonstrated better psychosocial and neurocognitive outcomes among those who used hearing aids compared with those who did not.59,68-70 These data suggest that earlier intervention with amplification may offset or attenuate the association of HI with neurocognitive performance, social isolation, and depression. Presumably, neurocognitive performance and psychosocial well-being would also be improved in children and young adults who consistently use amplification.
Hearing impairment is a serious medical condition, particularly if undetected or untreated, and, in this cross-sectional study, it appeared to be associated with an increased risk for neurocognitive deficits. More than one-third of childhood cancer survivors who received potentially ototoxic therapy were found to have severe HI. Early screening and intervention for HI, including adherence to hearing aids and cochlear implants, neuropsychological consultation, and educational accommodations, may facilitate the development and maintenance of neurocognitive function and may identify those who are at risk for future impairment. Prospective studies investigating the association between adherence to hearing aid use and neurocognitive outcomes in survivors of childhood cancer are needed.
Accepted for Publication: May 19, 2020.
Corresponding Author: Johnnie K. Bass, PhD, Rehabilitation Services, St. Jude Children’s Research Hospital, 262 Danny Thomas Place, Memphis, TN 38105 (johnnie.bass@stjude.org).
Published Online: July 30, 2020. doi:10.1001/jamaoncol.2020.2822
Author Contributions: Drs Bass and Krull had full access to all of the data in the study and take responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.
Concept and design: Bass, Brinkman, Merchant, Armstrong, Srivastava, Hudson, Krull.
Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: Bass, Liu, Banerjee, Mulrooney, Gajjar, Pappo, Merchant, Armstrong, Srivastava, Robison, Hudson, Krull.
Drafting of the manuscript: Bass, Liu, Banerjee, Merchant, Krull.
Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: All authors.
Statistical analysis: Liu, Srivastava.
Obtained funding: Armstrong, Hudson.
Administrative, technical, or material support: Bass, Banerjee, Gajjar, Pappo, Merchant, Robison, Hudson, Krull.
Supervision: Bass, Merchant, Armstrong, Srivastava, Krull.
Conflict of Interest Disclosures: Dr Bass reported receiving grants from the National Cancer Institute (NCI) during the conduct of the study. Dr Pappo reported receiving personal fees from Bayer, Loxo, and Merck outside the submitted work. Dr Armstrong reported receiving grants from the NCI during the conduct of the study. Dr Hudson reported receiving grants from the NCI during the conduct of the study. Dr Krull reported receiving grants from the NCI during the conduct of the study. No other disclosures were reported.
Funding/Support: This study was funded by grant CA195547 and Cancer Center Support CORE grant CA21765 from the NCI and by the American Lebanese Syrian Associated Charities.
Role of the Funder/Sponsor: The funders had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.
Additional Contributions: We thank the patients and families who volunteered their time and participated in neurocognitive and audiologic assessments in the St. Jude Lifetime Cohort Study.
2.Grewal
S, Merchant
T, Reymond
R, McInerney
M, Hodge
C, Shearer
P. Auditory late effects of childhood cancer therapy: a report from the Children’s Oncology Group.
Pediatrics. 2010;125(4):e938-e950. doi:
10.1542/peds.2009-1597
PubMedGoogle Scholar 3.Brinkman
TM, Bass
JK, Li
Z,
et al. Treatment-induced hearing loss and adult social outcomes in survivors of childhood CNS and non-CNS solid tumors: results from the St. Jude Lifetime Cohort Study.
Cancer. 2015;121(22):4053-4061. doi:
10.1002/cncr.29604
PubMedGoogle Scholar 5.Prasad
PK, Hardy
KK, Zhang
N,
et al. Psychosocial and neurocognitive outcomes in adult survivors of adolescent and early young adult cancer: a report from the Childhood Cancer Survivor Study.
J Clin Oncol. 2015;33(23):2545-2552. doi:
10.1200/JCO.2014.57.7528
PubMedGoogle Scholar 6.Krull
KR, Hardy
KK, Kahalley
LS, Schuitema
I, Kesler
SR. Neurocognitive outcomes and interventions in long-term survivors of childhood cancer.
J Clin Oncol. 2018;36(21):2181-2189. doi:
10.1200/JCO.2017.76.4696
PubMedGoogle Scholar 7.Brinkman
TM, Krasin
MJ, Liu
W,
et al. Long-term neurocognitive functioning and social attainment in adult survivors of pediatric CNS tumors: results from the St Jude Lifetime Cohort Study.
J Clin Oncol. 2016;34(12):1358-1367. doi:
10.1200/JCO.2015.62.2589
PubMedGoogle Scholar 8.Merchant
TE, Conklin
HM, Wu
S, Lustig
RH, Xiong
X. Late effects of conformal radiation therapy for pediatric patients with low-grade glioma: prospective evaluation of cognitive, endocrine, and hearing deficits.
J Clin Oncol. 2009;27(22):3691-3697. doi:
10.1200/JCO.2008.21.2738
PubMedGoogle Scholar 10.de Ruiter
MA, van Mourik
R, Schouten-van Meeteren
AYN, Grootenhuis
MA, Oosterlaan
J. Neurocognitive consequences of a paediatric brain tumour and its treatment: a meta-analysis.
Dev Med Child Neurol. 2013;55(5):408-417. doi:
10.1111/dmcn.12020
PubMedGoogle Scholar 12.Clemens
E, de Vries
AC, Am Zehnhoff-Dinnesen
A,
et al. Hearing loss after platinum treatment is irreversible in noncranial irradiated childhood cancer survivors.
Pediatr Hematol Oncol. 2017;34(2):120-129. doi:
10.1080/08880018.2017.1323985
PubMedGoogle Scholar 14.Jehanne
M, Lumbroso-Le Rouic
L, Savignoni
A,
et al. Analysis of ototoxicity in young children receiving carboplatin in the context of conservative management of unilateral or bilateral retinoblastoma.
Pediatr Blood Cancer. 2009;52(5):637-643. doi:
10.1002/pbc.21898
PubMedGoogle Scholar 18.Williams
GB, Kun
LE, Thompson
JW, Gould
HJ, Stocks
RM. Hearing loss as a late complication of radiotherapy in children with brain tumors.
Ann Otol Rhinol Laryngol. 2005;114(4):328-331. doi:
10.1177/000348940511400413
PubMedGoogle Scholar 23.Gurney
JG, Tersak
JM, Ness
KK, Landier
W, Matthay
KK, Schmidt
ML; Children’s Oncology Group. Hearing loss, quality of life, and academic problems in long-term neuroblastoma survivors: a report from the Children’s Oncology Group.
Pediatrics. 2007;120(5):e1229-e1236. doi:
10.1542/peds.2007-0178
PubMedGoogle Scholar 24.Kadan-Lottick
NS, Zeltzer
LK, Liu
Q,
et al. Neurocognitive functioning in adult survivors of childhood non-central nervous system cancers.
J Natl Cancer Inst. 2010;102(12):881-893. doi:
10.1093/jnci/djq156
PubMedGoogle Scholar 25.Schreiber
JE, Gurney
JG, Palmer
SL,
et al. Examination of risk factors for intellectual and academic outcomes following treatment for pediatric medulloblastoma.
Neuro Oncol. 2014;16(8):1129-1136. doi:
10.1093/neuonc/nou006
PubMedGoogle Scholar 26.Orgel
E, O’Neil
SH, Kayser
K,
et al. Effect of sensorineural hearing loss on neurocognitive functioning in pediatric brain tumor survivors.
Pediatr Blood Cancer. 2016;63(3):527-534. doi:
10.1002/pbc.25804
PubMedGoogle Scholar 27.Olivier
TW, Bass
JK, Ashford
JM,
et al. Cognitive implications of ototoxicity in pediatric patients with embryonal brain tumors.
J Clin Oncol. 2019;37(18):1566-1575. doi:
10.1200/JCO.18.01358
PubMedGoogle Scholar 28.Hudson
MM, Ness
KK, Nolan
VG,
et al. Prospective medical assessment of adults surviving childhood cancer: study design, cohort characteristics, and feasibility of the St. Jude Lifetime Cohort study.
Pediatr Blood Cancer. 2011;56(5):825-836. doi:
10.1002/pbc.22875
PubMedGoogle Scholar 29.Wechsler
D. Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence. Psychological Corp; 1999.
30.Wechsler
D. Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence. 2nd ed. NCS Pearson; 2011.
31.Conners
CK. Conners’ Continuous Performance Test II. Multi-Health Systems Inc; 2001.
32.Wechsler
D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. 3rd ed. Psychological Corp; 1997.
33.Wechsler
D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. 4th ed. Psychological Corp; 2008.
34.Reitan
RM, Wolfson
D. The Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery: Theory and Clinical Interpretation. 2nd ed. Neuropsychology Press; 1993.
35.Delis
DC, Kramer
JH, Kaplan
E, Ober
BA. California Verbal Learning Test. 2nd ed. Psychological Corp; 2000.
36.Reynolds
CR, Voress
JK. Test of Memory and Learning. 2nd ed. PRO-ED; 2007.
37.Lezak
MD, Howieson
DB, Bigler
ED, Tranel
D. Neuropsychological Assessment. 5th ed. Oxford University Press; 2012.
39.Lafayette Instruments. Grooved Pegboard Test User Instructions. Lafayette Instrument Co Inc; 1989.
40.Woodcock
RW, McGrew
KS, Mather
N. Woodcock-Johnson III: Tests of Achievement. Riverside; 2001.
43.Hoffman
HJ, Dobie
RA, Losonczy
KG, Themann
CL, Flamme
GA. Declining prevalence of hearing loss in US adults aged 20 to 69 Years.
JAMA Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. 2017;143(3):274-285. doi:
10.1001/jamaoto.2016.3527
PubMedGoogle Scholar 46.Geers
A, Tobey
E, Moog
J, Brenner
C. Long-term outcomes of cochlear implantation in the preschool years: from elementary grades to high school.
Int J Audiol. 2008;47(suppl 2):S21-S30. doi:
10.1080/14992020802339167
PubMedGoogle Scholar 47.Traxler
CB. The Stanford Achievement Test, 9th edition: national norming and performance standards for deaf and hard-of-hearing students.
J Deaf Stud Deaf Educ. 2000;5(4):337-348. doi:
10.1093/deafed/5.4.337PubMedGoogle Scholar 50.Pagliaro
CM, Kritzer
KL. The Math Gap: a description of the mathematics performance of preschool-aged deaf/hard-of-hearing children.
J Deaf Stud Deaf Educ. 2013;18(2):139-160. doi:
10.1093/deafed/ens070
PubMedGoogle Scholar 51.Kelly
RR, Gaustad
MG. Deaf college students’ mathematical skills relative to morphological knowledge, reading level, and language proficiency.
J Deaf Stud Deaf Educ. 2007;12(1):25-37. doi:
10.1093/deafed/enl012
PubMedGoogle Scholar 52.Sarant
JZ, Harris
DC, Bennet
LA. Academic outcomes for school-aged children with severe-profound hearing loss and early unilateral and bilateral cochlear implants.
J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2015;58(3):1017-1032. doi:
10.1044/2015_JSLHR-H-14-0075
PubMedGoogle Scholar 54.Quigley
SP. Environment and communication in the language development of deaf children. In: Bradford
LJ, Hardy
WG, eds. Hearing and Hearing Impairment. Grune & Stratton; 1979:287-298.
56.Kelly
RR, Lang
HG, Mousley
K, Davis
SM. Deaf college students’ comprehension of relational language in arithmetic compare problems.
J Deaf Stud Deaf Educ. 2003;8(2):120-132. doi:
10.1093/deafed/eng006
PubMedGoogle Scholar 61.Kuhn
LJ, Willoughby
MT, Wilbourn
MP, Vernon-Feagans
L, Blair
CB; Family Life Project Key Investigators. Early communicative gestures prospectively predict language development and executive function in early childhood.
Child Dev. 2014;85(5):1898-1914. doi:
10.1111/cdev.12249
PubMedGoogle Scholar 64.Botting
N, Jones
A, Marshall
C, Denmark
T, Atkinson
J, Morgan
G. Nonverbal executive function is mediated by language: a study of deaf and hearing children.
Child Dev. 2017;88(5):1689-1700. doi:
10.1111/cdev.12659
PubMedGoogle Scholar 66.Davis
JM, Elfenbein
J, Schum
R, Bentler
RA. Effects of mild and moderate hearing impairments on language, educational, and psychosocial behavior of children.
J Speech Hear Disord. 1986;51(1):53-62. doi:
10.1044/jshd.5101.53
PubMedGoogle Scholar 67.Calcus
A, Tuomainen
O, Campos
A, Rosen
S, Halliday
LF. Functional brain alterations following mild-to-moderate sensorineural hearing loss in children.
Elife. 2019;8:e46965. doi:
10.7554/eLife.46965
PubMedGoogle Scholar 68.Maharani
A, Dawes
P, Nazroo
J, Tampubolon
G, Pendleton
N; SENSE-Cog WP1 Group. Longitudinal relationship between hearing aid use and cognitive function in older Americans.
J Am Geriatr Soc. 2018;66(6):1130-1136. doi:
10.1111/jgs.15363
PubMedGoogle Scholar 70.Castiglione
A, Benatti
A, Velardita
C,
et al. Aging, cognitive decline and hearing loss: effects of auditory rehabilitation and training with hearing aids and cochlear implants on cognitive function and depression among older adults.
Audiol Neurootol. 2016;21(suppl 1):21-28. doi:
10.1159/000448350
PubMedGoogle Scholar