[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Figure 1.  Sample Questions from the Self-perception Profile for Children
Sample Questions from the Self-perception Profile for Children
Figure 2.  Self-perception Scale for Children Domain Scores for Amblyopic, Nonamblyopic, and Control Groups
Self-perception Scale for Children Domain Scores for Amblyopic, Nonamblyopic, and Control Groups

The vertical bars show standard errors of the mean.

aThe group’s mean score was significantly lower than the control group’s mean score.

Figure 3.  Associations Between Task Performance Measures and Self-perception Domain Scores
Associations Between Task Performance Measures and Self-perception Domain Scores

The solid lines represent best-fit linear regressions. MABC2 indicates Movement Assessment Battery for Children 2.

Table 1.  Participant Characteristics
Participant Characteristics
Table 2.  Domain Scale Scores
Domain Scale Scores
1.
Grant  S, Conway  ML.  Reach-to-precision grasp deficits in amblyopia: effects of object contrast and low visibility.  Vision Res. 2015;114:100-110. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2014.11.009PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
Grant  S, Melmoth  DR, Morgan  MJ, Finlay  AL.  Prehension deficits in amblyopia.  Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2007;48(3):1139-1148. doi:10.1167/iovs.06-0976PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
Kanonidou  E, Proudlock  FA, Gottlob  I.  Reading strategies in mild to moderate strabismic amblyopia: an eye movement investigation.  Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2010;51(7):3502-3508. doi:10.1167/iovs.09-4236PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Kelly  KR, Jost  RM, De La Cruz  A, Birch  EE.  Amblyopic children read more slowly than controls under natural, binocular reading conditions.  J AAPOS. 2015;19(6):515-520. doi:10.1016/j.jaapos.2015.09.002PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Kelly  KR, Jost  RM, De La Cruz  A,  et al.  Slow reading in children with anisometropic amblyopia is associated with fixation instability and increased saccades.  J AAPOS. 2017;21(6):447-451.e1. doi:10.1016/j.jaapos.2017.10.001PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
O’Connor  AR, Birch  EE, Anderson  S, Draper  H.  Relationship between binocular vision, visual acuity, and fine motor skills.  Optom Vis Sci. 2010;87(12):942-947. doi:10.1097/OPX.0b013e3181fd132ePubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
O’Connor  AR, Birch  EE, Anderson  S, Draper  H; FSOS Research Group.  The functional significance of stereopsis.  Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2010;51(4):2019-2023. doi:10.1167/iovs.09-4434PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Stifter  E, Burggasser  G, Hirmann  E, Thaler  A, Radner  W.  Monocular and binocular reading performance in children with microstrabismic amblyopia.  Br J Ophthalmol. 2005;89(10):1324-1329. doi:10.1136/bjo.2005.066688PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Suttle  CM, Melmoth  DR, Finlay  AL, Sloper  JJ, Grant  S.  Eye-hand coordination skills in children with and without amblyopia.  Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2011;52(3):1851-1864. doi:10.1167/iovs.10-6341PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Webber  AL, Wood  JM, Gole  GA, Brown  B.  The effect of amblyopia on fine motor skills in children.  Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2008;49(2):594-603. doi:10.1167/iovs.07-0869PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
Webber  AL, Wood  JM, Thompson  B.  Fine motor skills of children with amblyopia improve following binocular treatment.  Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2016;57(11):4713-4720. doi:10.1167/iovs.16-19797PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Grant  S, Suttle  C, Melmoth  DR, Conway  ML, Sloper  JJ.  Age- and stereovision-dependent eye-hand coordination deficits in children with amblyopia and abnormal binocularity.  Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2014;55(9):5687-57015. doi:10.1167/iovs.14-14745PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Kanonidou  E, Gottlob  I, Proudlock  FA.  The effect of font size on reading performance in strabismic amblyopia: an eye movement investigation.  Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2014;55(1):451-459. doi:10.1167/iovs.13-13257PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
Phillips  DA.  Socialization of perceived academic competence among highly competent children.  Child Dev. 1987;58(5):1308-1320. doi:10.2307/1130623PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Pomerantz  EM, Ruble  DN, Frey  KS, Greulich  F.  Meeting goals and confronting conflict: children’s changing perceptions of social comparison.  Child Dev. 1995;66(3):723-738. doi:10.2307/1131946PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Piek  JP, Baynam  GB, Barrett  NC.  The relationship between fine and gross motor ability, self-perceptions and self-worth in children and adolescents.  Hum Mov Sci. 2006;25(1):65-75. doi:10.1016/j.humov.2005.10.011PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
Felius  J, Chandler  DL, Holmes  JM,  et al; Pediatric Eye Disease Investigator Group.  Evaluating the burden of amblyopia treatment from the parent and child’s perspective.  J AAPOS. 2010;14(5):389-395. doi:10.1016/j.jaapos.2010.07.009PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
Holmes  JM, Beck  RW, Kraker  RT,  et al; Pediatric Eye Disease Investigator Group.  Impact of patching and atropine treatment on the child and family in the amblyopia treatment study.  Arch Ophthalmol. 2003;121(11):1625-1632. doi:10.1001/archopht.121.11.1625PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
19.
Koklanis  K, Abel  LA, Aroni  R.  Psychosocial impact of amblyopia and its treatment: a multidisciplinary study.  Clin Exp Ophthalmol. 2006;34(8):743-750. doi:10.1111/j.1442-9071.2006.01317.xPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Loudon  SE, Passchier  J, Chaker  L,  et al.  Psychological causes of non-compliance with electronically monitored occlusion therapy for amblyopia.  Br J Ophthalmol. 2009;93(11):1499-1503. doi:10.1136/bjo.2008.149815PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
Williams  C, Horwood  J, Northstone  K, Herrick  D, Waylen  A, Wolke  D; ALSPAC Study Group.  The timing of patching treatment and a child’s wellbeing.  Br J Ophthalmol. 2006;90(6):670-671. doi:10.1136/bjo.2006.091082PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
Packwood  EA, Cruz  OA, Rychwalski  PJ, Keech  RV.  The psychosocial effects of amblyopia study.  J AAPOS. 1999;3(1):15-17. doi:10.1016/S1091-8531(99)70089-3PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
23.
Horwood  J, Waylen  A, Herrick  D, Williams  C, Wolke  D.  Common visual defects and peer victimization in children.  Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2005;46(4):1177-1181. doi:10.1167/iovs.04-0597PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Hrisos  S, Clarke  MP, Wright  CM.  The emotional impact of amblyopia treatment in preschool children: randomized controlled trial.  Ophthalmology. 2004;111(8):1550-1556. doi:10.1016/j.ophtha.2003.12.059PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
Drews-Botsch  CD, Hartmann  EE, Celano  M; Infant Aphakia Treatment Study Group.  Predictors of adherence to occlusion therapy 3 months after cataract extraction in the Infant Aphakia Treatment Study.  J AAPOS. 2012;16(2):150-155. doi:10.1016/j.jaapos.2011.12.149PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
Wilson  GA, Welch  D.  Does amblyopia have a functional impact? findings from the Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development Study.  Clin Exp Ophthalmol. 2013;41(2):127-134. doi:10.1111/j.1442-9071.2012.02842.xPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
27.
Webber  AL, Wood  JM, Gole  GA, Brown  B.  Effect of amblyopia on self-esteem in children.  Optom Vis Sci. 2008;85(11):1074-1081. doi:10.1097/OPX.0b013e31818b9911PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
28.
Pediatric Eye Disease Investigator Group. A randomized trial of atropine vs. patching for treatment of moderate amblyopia in children.  Arch Ophthalmol. 2002;120(3):268-278. doi:10.1001/archopht.120.3.268PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
29.
Moke  PS, Turpin  AH, Beck  RW,  et al.  Computerized method of visual acuity testing: adaptation of the amblyopia treatment study visual acuity testing protocol.  Am J Ophthalmol. 2001;132(6):903-909. doi:10.1016/S0002-9394(01)01256-9PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
30.
Beck  RW, Moke  PS, Turpin  AH,  et al.  A computerized method of visual acuity testing: adaptation of the early treatment of diabetic retinopathy study testing protocol.  Am J Ophthalmol. 2003;135(2):194-205. doi:10.1016/S0002-9394(02)01825-1PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
31.
Harter  S.  The Construction of the Self: Developmental and Socio-Cultural Foundations. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 2012.
32.
Harter  S.  The Construction of the Self. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 1999.
33.
Harter  S.  Self-Perception Profile for Children: Manual and Questionnaires. Denver, CO: University of Denver; 2012.
34.
Niechwiej-Szwedo  E, Chandrakumar  M, Goltz  HC, Wong  AM.  Effects of strabismic amblyopia and strabismus without amblyopia on visuomotor behavior, I: saccadic eye movements.  Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2012;53(12):7458-7468. doi:10.1167/iovs.12-10550PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
35.
Niechwiej-Szwedo  E, Goltz  HC, Chandrakumar  M, Hirji  ZA, Wong  AM.  Effects of anisometropic amblyopia on visuomotor behavior, I: saccadic eye movements.  Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2010;51(12):6348-6354. doi:10.1167/iovs.10-5882PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
36.
McConkie  GW, Kerr  PW, Reddix  MD, Zola  D.  Eye movement control during reading, I: the location of initial eye fixations on words.  Vision Res. 1988;28(10):1107-1118. doi:10.1016/0042-6989(88)90137-XPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
Original Investigation
November 15, 2018

Self-perception of School-aged Children With Amblyopia and Its Association With Reading Speed and Motor Skills

Author Affiliations
  • 1Retina Foundation of the Southwest, Dallas, Texas
  • 2Department of Ophthalmology, University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center, Dallas
  • 3ABC Eyes, Dallas, Texas
  • 4School of Optometry and Vision Science, Queensland University of Technology, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia
JAMA Ophthalmol. 2019;137(2):167-174. doi:10.1001/jamaophthalmol.2018.5527
Key Points

Question  How does amblyopia influence self-perception in children ages 8 to 13 years?

Findings  In this cross-sectional study, children with amblyopia had lower scholastic, social, and athletic competence scores as derived from the Self-perception Profile for Children than control children. Among children with amblyopia, the self-perception of scholastic competence was associated with reading speed, and the self-perception of scholastic, social, and athletic competence was associated with aiming and catching skills.

Meaning  These results suggest lower self-perception and its association with reading speed and motor skills highlight the potentially wide-ranging influence of altered visual development in children in their everyday lives.

Abstract

Importance  Reading and eye-hand coordination deficits in children with amblyopia may impede their ability to demonstrate their knowledge and skills, compete in sports and physical activities, and interact with peers. Because perceived scholastic, social, and athletic competence are key determinants of self-esteem in school-aged children, these deficits may influence a child’s self-perception.

Objective  To determine whether amblyopia is associated with lowered self-perception of competence, appearance, conduct, and global self-worth and whether the self-perception of children with amblyopia is associated with their performance of reading and eye-hand tasks.

Design, Setting, and Participants  This cross-sectional study was conducted from January 2016 to June 2017 at the Pediatric Vision Laboratory of the Retina Foundation of the Southwest and included healthy children in grades 3 to 8, including 50 children with amblyopia; 13 children without amblyopia with strabismus, anisometropia, or both; and 18 control children.

Main Outcomes and Measures  Self-perception was assessed using the Self-perception Profile for Children, which includes 5 domains: scholastic, social, and athletic competence; physical appearance; behavioral conduct; and a separate scale for global self-worth. Reading speed and eye-hand task performance were evaluated with the Readalyzer (Bernell) and Movement Assessment Battery for Children, 2nd Edition. Visual acuity and stereoacuity also were assessed.

Results  Of 50 participants, 31 (62%) were girls, 31 (62%) were non-Hispanic white, 6 (12%) were Hispanic white, 3 (6%) were African American, 4 (8%) were Asian/Pacific Islander, and 3 (6%) were more than 1 race/ethnicity, and the mean [SD] age was 10.6 [1.3] years. Children with amblyopia had significantly lower scores than control children for scholastic (mean [SD], 2.93 [0.74] vs 3.58 [0.24]; mean [SD] difference, 0.65 [0.36]; 95% CI, 0.29-1.01; P = .004), social (mean [SD], 2.95 [0.64] vs 3.62 [0.35]; mean [SD] difference, 0.67 [0.32]; 95% CI, 0.35-0.99] P < .001), and athletic (mean [SD], 2.61 [0.65] vs 3.43 [0.52]; mean [SD] difference, 0.82 [0.34]; 95% CI, 0.48-1.16; P = .001) competence domains. Among children with amblyopia, a lower self-perception of scholastic competence was associated with a slower reading speed (r = 0.49, 95% CI, 0.17-0.72; P = .002) and a lower self-perception of scholastic, social, and athletic competence was associated with worse performance of aiming and catching (scholastic r = 0.48; 95% CI, 0.16-0.71; P = .007; social r = 0.63; 95% CI, 0.35-0.81; P < .001; athletic r = 0.53; 95% CI, 0.21-0.75; P = .003). No differences in the self-perception of physical appearance (mean [SD], 3.32 [0.63] vs 3.64 [0.40]), conduct (mean [SD], 3.09 [0.56] vs 3.34 [0.66]), or global self-worth (mean [SD], 3.42 [0.42] vs 3.69 [0.36]) were found between the amblyopic and control groups.

Conclusions and Relevance  These findings suggest that lower self-perception is associated with slower reading speed and worse motor skills and may highlight the wide-ranging effects of altered visual development for children with amblyopia in their everyday lives.

Introduction

Reading and eye-hand coordination deficits have been reported in children with amblyopia.1-13 These limitations may impede the ability of students with amblyopia to demonstrate their knowledge and skills, compete in sports and physical activities, and interact with their peers. Perceived scholastic, social, and athletic competence, along with physical appearance, are key determinants of self-esteem in school-age children.14-16 Thus, the limitations that are associated with amblyopia may influence a child’s self-perception.

Numerous amblyopia studies have focused on the association of patching treatments with self-esteem, stigma, bullying, and family stress.17-25 On the other hand, to our knowledge, few studies have assessed how amblyopia itself affects self-perception. In a study of adults who were asked to recall how anisometropic amblyopia affected them during their school years, 76% reported that amblyopia affected their self-image, 52% reported that amblyopia interfered with school, and 40% reported that amblyopia interfered with sports.22 Using the Rosenberg Self-esteem questionnaire, a diverse group of students age 11 to 13 years with strabismic, anisometropic, combined mechanism, or deprivation amblyopia had no problems with self-esteem, but the 10-item scale was deemed by the authors to be insensitive because of ceiling effects.26 Low social acceptance scores were found in a group of 9-year-old children that included some with amblyopia and others who had recovered but had a history of amblyopia.27 However, this latter study was conducted more than a decade ago, an era during which amblyopic children were patching much more than the current recommendation of 2 hours per day (https://www.aao.org/ preferred-practice-pattern/amblyopia-ppp-2017) and were wearing their patches to school. It is not surprising that the lower social acceptance scores were associated with a history of patching.27 The goals of this study were to determine whether children’s self-perception is influenced by amblyopia in grades 3 to 8, examine whether self-perception scores are associated with deficits in performance in reading and motor skills tasks, and evaluate whether there are specific clinical factors that are associated with self-perception in children with amblyopia.

Methods
Participants

Fifty children with amblyopia in grades 3 to 8 (age 8 to 13 years) were enrolled. The eligibility criteria were a current diagnosis of strabismic, anisometropic, or combined-mechanism amblyopia (diagnostic criteria were developed by the Pediatric Eye Disease Investigator Group for the Amblyopia Treatment Studies28); an amblyopic eye best-corrected visual acuity (BCVA) of 0.2 logMAR or worse; a fellow eye BCVA of 0.1 logMAR or better; or an interocular difference in BCVA of 0.2 logMAR or greater. To evaluate the effect of amblyogenic factors in the absence of amblyopia, 13 age-similar children without amblyopia with anisometropia, strabismus, or both who had no history of amblyopia diagnosis or patching treatment were enrolled. All children with strabismus had achieved good alignment (<5 prism diopters) with spectacles and/or surgery at the time they participated in this study. As a comparison group, 18 age-similar controls were also enrolled. None of the children were born prematurely (>32 weeks’ postmenstrual age), had coexisting ocular or systemic disease, or had a history of congenital malformation or infection. Because the study included printed questionnaires, a reading test, and motor skills testing, additional exclusion criteria were dyslexia, enrollment in a reading intervention program, and a primary language other than English. The characteristics of each group are summarized in Table 1.

Written informed consent was obtained from a parent after an explanation of the nature and possible consequences of the study. In addition, written informed assent was obtained from children age 10 to 12 years. All procedures and the protocol were approved by the institutional review board of the University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center, followed the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki, and complied with the requirements of the US Health Insurance Portability and Accountability Act of 1996.

Vision

Every participant with or without amblyopia had a comprehensive eye examination that was performed by a fellowship-trained pediatric ophthalmologist who diagnosed whether amblyopia was present and identified the etiologic factors. Children wore optical correction, if needed, in accordance with American Association for Pediatric Ophthalmology and Strabismus guidelines (https://aapos.org//client_data/files/2017/506_aaposmedicalneedforglasses.pdf). The best-corrected visual acuity was obtained for each eye with an opaque occluder patch and the electronic Early Treatment Diabetic Retinopathy Study method.29,30 Random-dot stereoacuity was evaluated using the Randot Preschool Stereoacuity Test (Stereo Optical Co, Inc) and the Stereo Butterfly Test (Stereo Optical Co, Inc), which were administered and scored according to the manufacturer’s instructions.

Self-perception

Self-perception was assessed with the Self-perception Profile for Children.31 This instrument uses a multidimensional approach to the measurement of self-perception. Beginning in middle childhood, children are able to make domain-specific evaluations of their competence or adequacy in different arenas.32 The Self-perception Profile for Children includes 5 specific domains: scholastic competence (perceived cognitive competence, as applied to schoolwork), social competence (perceived competence in skills that are needed to make friends, be popular, and have others like you), athletic competence (perceived competence in sports and outdoor games), physical appearance (feeling happy about looks), and behavioral conduct (the degree to which people do the right thing and avoid trouble). A sixth domain (not a composite score) evaluates the child’s overall sense of their worth as a person (ie, global self-worth; how happy with the way they are leading their lives and the way they are as human beings).

For each domain, there are 6 items in a structured 2-alternative format that is designed to offset the tendency to give socially desirable responses. Specifically, the items are formulated to imply that half of the children in the world view themselves one way and half the other way, which legitimizes either choice. Two sample questions from the scholastic scale are provided in Figure 1. The child was first asked to decide which kids he or she was most like for each statement. Once the child made this choice, he or she was asked to choose whether this is “really true for me” of “sort of true for me.” Harter,31 the developer of the Self-perception Profile for Children, argues that the effectiveness of this approach lies in the implication that half of the children in the world view themselves one way and half the other way, legitimizing either choice. Refining their answer into the “really true for me” or “sort of true for me” category broadens the range of choices into a 4-point scale (1 = lowest perceived competence or adequacy; 4 = highest perceived competence or adequacy). Within each domain, the first alternative is phrased in a positive manner for 3 of the 6 items and in a negative manner for the remaining 3 questions (Figure 1). A mean score was calculated for each subscale.

The Self-perception Profile for Children was administered according to the published instructions.33 All children had practice with a sample question to ensure that they understood the 2-alternative format and the subsequent choice of “really true for me” of “sort or true for me.” For children in grades 3 and 4, the entire questionnaire was read to them by a research staff member. Older children sat with the research staff member but could read the questionnaire on their own, using the staff member as a resource if they had any questions or difficulty understanding any words. The child’s parent was ideally seated in the next room, but if that was not possible, they sat behind the child and were instructed not to assist or coach the child in any way. Self-perception scale scores were not available to the examiner who was responsible for testing reading and motor skills.

Reading

Reading skill was assessed in children with amblyopia during the binocular viewing with the Readalyzer (Compevo AB), an infrared eye movement recording system that was mounted in goggles that were worn over the child’s habitual optical correction.4,5 The child sat at eye level at a comfortable, habitual reading distance (35-40 cm) and was required to silently read printed paragraphs of text (12 lines; 100 words) from a booklet. The child first read a grade level–1 paragraph that served as a practice run, then a paragraph at the last grade level completed. Ten yes/no comprehension questions followed each silent reading. Recordings were considered acceptable for analysis if the comprehension was 80% or greater and the tracking reliability was 70% or greater. Reading speed (words per minute) was used as the outcome measure.

Motor Skills

Children with amblyopia were tested with the Movement Assessment Battery for Children 2 (MABC-2), a standardized test of motor skills and coordination that is administered in age bands (age 8–10 years; age 11-13 years for this study). The Movement Assessment Battery for Children 2 consists of 8 tasks that compose 3 subscales: manual dexterity (unimanual, bimanual, and drawing trail), aiming and catching, and balance (static and dynamic). Raw scores per task are converted into standardized scores, with higher scores indicating a better performance.

Analysis

A 1-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used to evaluate group differences (amblyopic, nonamblyopic, and normal control) for each self-perception domain (scholastic competence, social competence, athletic competence, physical appearance, behavioral conduct, and global self-worth). Significant ANOVAs were followed with post hoc pairwise comparisons of the amblyopic and nonamblyopic groups with the controls (Bonferroni-adjusted α = .025). Pearson r correlations were conducted for children with amblyopia only to examine the associations between task performance (reading speed and motor skills) and self-perception domain scores. Pearson r correlations also were conducted to determine whether clinical factors (amblyopic eye best-corrected visual acuity, stereoacuity) were associated with self-perception domain scores. Lastly, independent t tests were conducted to determine whether children with amblyopia with strabismus and children with amblyopia without strabismus differed on any self-perception domain.

Results
Self-perception and Global Self-worth

The mean self-perception scale scores for children with and without amblyopia and controls are summarized in Figure 2 and Table 2. Scholastic, social, and athletic competence scores were significantly different among groups (F2,78, 9.04, 8.42, and 12.54, respectively; P < .004 for all 3 ANOVAs). Children with amblyopia had lower scores than control children for scholastic (t66 = 3.80; P <.001), social (t66 = 4.24; P < .001), and athletic competence (t66 = 4.50; P < .001), but did not significantly differ from controls for the self-perception of physical appearance (t66 = 2.00; P = .05) or behavioral conduct (t66 = 1.52; P = .13). Children without amblyopia had significantly lower scores than control children for social (t29 = 3.41; P = .02) and athletic competence (t29 = 4.40; P < .001) but did not significantly differ from controls for the self-perception of scholastic competence (t29 = 1.31; P = .20), physical appearance (t29 = 0.37; P = .72), or behavioral conduct (t29 = 0.09; P = .93). Neither children with or without amblyopia differed significantly from controls for global self-worth (t66 = 1.53; P = .13 and t29 = 1.84; P = .08, respectively).

Self-perception and Task Performance in Amblyopia

The associations between competence scores and task performance were examined for children with amblyopia (Figure 3). Scholastic competence scores were positively associated with reading speed (r = 0.49; 95% CI, 0.17-0.72; P = .002). Scholastic, social, and athletic competence scores were positively associated with aiming and catching (scholastic competence, r = 0.48; 95% CI, 0.16-0.71; P = .007; social competence, r = 0.63; 95% CI, 0.35-0.81; P < .001; athletic competence, r = 0.53; 95% CI, 0.21-0.75; P = .003). No significant correlations between self-perception domain scores and manual dexterity or balance scores were observed.

Self-perception and Clinical Factors in Amblyopia

No significant correlations were found for amblyopic eye visual acuity or stereoacuity and domain scores. The scores of subgroups of children with amblyopia with or without strabismus did not differ significantly in any domain. Because nearly every child with amblyopia had a history of treatment with eyeglasses and patching, we were not able to compare the domain scores of the subgroups with and without these treatments.

Discussion

Amblyopia influences self-perception in children enrolled in third through eighth grade, especially the self-perception of scholastic, social, and athletic competence. Children with amblyopia had significantly lower scholastic, social, and athletic competence scores than control children. The self-perception of scholastic competence was associated with reading speed and the self-perception of scholastic, social, and athletic competence was associated with the performance of aiming and catching skills. No differences in the self-perception of physical appearance or behavioral conduct and no differences in global self-worth were found among amblyopic, nonamblyopic, and control groups.

Children without amblyopia with strabismus, anisometropia, or both did not differ from controls in their self-perception of scholastic competence. However, the sample size was small (n = 13), limiting our power to determine whether subtle differences were present. The finding of no significant difference between children without amblyopia and controls is consistent with our previous report that amblyopia, not the associated etiologic factors of strabismus or anisometropia, is the key factor in slow reading in school-age children with amblyopia.4,5 Slow binocular silent reading in children with amblyopia is associated with ocular motor dysfunction. Children with amblyopia make more forward saccades than controls when reading, most likely because fixation instability results in saccades that miss the preferred landing position near the center of a word, thus requiring secondary corrective saccades to acquire the optimal position for decoding.34-36

Our finding that children with amblyopia and children without amblyopia with strabismus, anisometropia, or both have comparably lower social and athletic competence scores than controls suggests that discordant binocular visual experience, and not amblyopia, is influencing self-perception in these domains. Because self-perception is already developing before age 5 years, largely based on perceived competence and perceived social acceptance we, unlike others,9,10,27 included only children with no history of amblyopia diagnosis or patching treatment in our nonamblyopic group. Because the focus of this study was the influence of amblyopia on self-perception, we did not enroll children with strabismus of more than 4 prism diopters.

The self-perception of both social and athletic competence were associated with performance on aiming and catching tasks. A moderate association between social competence and athletic competence has been reported previously.33 While it is difficult to infer causality, it has been suggested that athletic prowess may lead to greater social acceptance or popularity during grades 3 to 8.33

Nearly all of the children with amblyopia and children without amblyopia had spectacle correction while none of the healthy controls wore eyeglasses. Because the children with and without amblyopia differed from controls in their self-perception of social and athletic domains, it is possible that wearing eyeglasses contributed to their altered self-perception of social and athletic competence. However, for the scholastic domain, only the children with amblyopia had lower scores, which was consistent with an effect solely due to amblyopia. This hypothesis is supported by the association between reading speed and the self-perception of scholastic competence.

Limitations

It remains unknown whether improvements in sensory function as a result of amblyopia treatment will result in improved self-perception of scholastic, social, and athletic competence. The lack of an association between visual acuity and stereoacuity and competence domain scores suggests that amblyopia treatment may not improve self-perception. On the other hand, the association between self-perception of scholastic competence and reading speed, along with our prior finding that decreased reading speed in amblyopia results primarily from an abnormally large number of forward saccades,4,5 suggests that amblyopia treatment may improve the self-perception of scholastic competence. Along the same line, data showing that motor skills improve following amblyopia treatment11 suggest that treatment may improve the self-perception of athletic and possibly social competence.

Conclusions

These findings suggest that lower self-perception is associated with slower reading speed and worse motor skills and may highlight the wide-ranging effects of altered visual development on children with amblyopia in their everyday lives.

Back to top
Article Information

Corresponding Author: Eileen E. Birch, PhD, Retina Foundation of the Southwest, 9600 N Central Expressway, Ste 200, Dallas, TX 75231 (ebirch@retinafoundation.org).

Accepted for Publication: September 22, 2018.

Published Online: November 15, 2018. doi:10.1001/jamaophthalmol.2018.5527

Author Contributions: Dr Birch had full access to the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and accuracy of the data analysis.

Study concept and design: Birch.

Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: Birch, Castañeda, Cheng-Patel, Morale, Kelly, Beauchamp.

Drafting of the manuscript: Birch, Cheng-Patel.

Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Birch, Castañeda, Morale, Kelly, Beauchamp.

Statistical analysis: Birch, Cheng-Patel, Kelly.

Obtained funding: Birch.

Administrative, technical, or material support: Castañeda, Cheng-Patel, Morale.

Study supervision: Birch.

Other–recruiting patients: Beauchamp.

Conflict of Interest Disclosures: None reported.

Funding/Support: This research was supported by the National Eye Institute (grants EY022313 and K99EY028224) and the Thrasher Research Fund (13441).

Role of the Funder/Sponsor: The funding organizations had no role in the design and conduct of the study, collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data, preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript, or decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

Meeting Presentation: This study was conducted at the Retina Foundation of the Southwest and preliminary data from this project were presented at the Child Vision Research Annual Meeting; June 19, 2017; Coleraine, Northern Ireland.

References
1.
Grant  S, Conway  ML.  Reach-to-precision grasp deficits in amblyopia: effects of object contrast and low visibility.  Vision Res. 2015;114:100-110. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2014.11.009PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
Grant  S, Melmoth  DR, Morgan  MJ, Finlay  AL.  Prehension deficits in amblyopia.  Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2007;48(3):1139-1148. doi:10.1167/iovs.06-0976PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
Kanonidou  E, Proudlock  FA, Gottlob  I.  Reading strategies in mild to moderate strabismic amblyopia: an eye movement investigation.  Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2010;51(7):3502-3508. doi:10.1167/iovs.09-4236PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Kelly  KR, Jost  RM, De La Cruz  A, Birch  EE.  Amblyopic children read more slowly than controls under natural, binocular reading conditions.  J AAPOS. 2015;19(6):515-520. doi:10.1016/j.jaapos.2015.09.002PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Kelly  KR, Jost  RM, De La Cruz  A,  et al.  Slow reading in children with anisometropic amblyopia is associated with fixation instability and increased saccades.  J AAPOS. 2017;21(6):447-451.e1. doi:10.1016/j.jaapos.2017.10.001PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
O’Connor  AR, Birch  EE, Anderson  S, Draper  H.  Relationship between binocular vision, visual acuity, and fine motor skills.  Optom Vis Sci. 2010;87(12):942-947. doi:10.1097/OPX.0b013e3181fd132ePubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
O’Connor  AR, Birch  EE, Anderson  S, Draper  H; FSOS Research Group.  The functional significance of stereopsis.  Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2010;51(4):2019-2023. doi:10.1167/iovs.09-4434PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Stifter  E, Burggasser  G, Hirmann  E, Thaler  A, Radner  W.  Monocular and binocular reading performance in children with microstrabismic amblyopia.  Br J Ophthalmol. 2005;89(10):1324-1329. doi:10.1136/bjo.2005.066688PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Suttle  CM, Melmoth  DR, Finlay  AL, Sloper  JJ, Grant  S.  Eye-hand coordination skills in children with and without amblyopia.  Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2011;52(3):1851-1864. doi:10.1167/iovs.10-6341PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Webber  AL, Wood  JM, Gole  GA, Brown  B.  The effect of amblyopia on fine motor skills in children.  Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2008;49(2):594-603. doi:10.1167/iovs.07-0869PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
Webber  AL, Wood  JM, Thompson  B.  Fine motor skills of children with amblyopia improve following binocular treatment.  Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2016;57(11):4713-4720. doi:10.1167/iovs.16-19797PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Grant  S, Suttle  C, Melmoth  DR, Conway  ML, Sloper  JJ.  Age- and stereovision-dependent eye-hand coordination deficits in children with amblyopia and abnormal binocularity.  Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2014;55(9):5687-57015. doi:10.1167/iovs.14-14745PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Kanonidou  E, Gottlob  I, Proudlock  FA.  The effect of font size on reading performance in strabismic amblyopia: an eye movement investigation.  Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2014;55(1):451-459. doi:10.1167/iovs.13-13257PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
Phillips  DA.  Socialization of perceived academic competence among highly competent children.  Child Dev. 1987;58(5):1308-1320. doi:10.2307/1130623PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Pomerantz  EM, Ruble  DN, Frey  KS, Greulich  F.  Meeting goals and confronting conflict: children’s changing perceptions of social comparison.  Child Dev. 1995;66(3):723-738. doi:10.2307/1131946PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Piek  JP, Baynam  GB, Barrett  NC.  The relationship between fine and gross motor ability, self-perceptions and self-worth in children and adolescents.  Hum Mov Sci. 2006;25(1):65-75. doi:10.1016/j.humov.2005.10.011PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
Felius  J, Chandler  DL, Holmes  JM,  et al; Pediatric Eye Disease Investigator Group.  Evaluating the burden of amblyopia treatment from the parent and child’s perspective.  J AAPOS. 2010;14(5):389-395. doi:10.1016/j.jaapos.2010.07.009PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
Holmes  JM, Beck  RW, Kraker  RT,  et al; Pediatric Eye Disease Investigator Group.  Impact of patching and atropine treatment on the child and family in the amblyopia treatment study.  Arch Ophthalmol. 2003;121(11):1625-1632. doi:10.1001/archopht.121.11.1625PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
19.
Koklanis  K, Abel  LA, Aroni  R.  Psychosocial impact of amblyopia and its treatment: a multidisciplinary study.  Clin Exp Ophthalmol. 2006;34(8):743-750. doi:10.1111/j.1442-9071.2006.01317.xPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Loudon  SE, Passchier  J, Chaker  L,  et al.  Psychological causes of non-compliance with electronically monitored occlusion therapy for amblyopia.  Br J Ophthalmol. 2009;93(11):1499-1503. doi:10.1136/bjo.2008.149815PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
Williams  C, Horwood  J, Northstone  K, Herrick  D, Waylen  A, Wolke  D; ALSPAC Study Group.  The timing of patching treatment and a child’s wellbeing.  Br J Ophthalmol. 2006;90(6):670-671. doi:10.1136/bjo.2006.091082PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
Packwood  EA, Cruz  OA, Rychwalski  PJ, Keech  RV.  The psychosocial effects of amblyopia study.  J AAPOS. 1999;3(1):15-17. doi:10.1016/S1091-8531(99)70089-3PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
23.
Horwood  J, Waylen  A, Herrick  D, Williams  C, Wolke  D.  Common visual defects and peer victimization in children.  Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2005;46(4):1177-1181. doi:10.1167/iovs.04-0597PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Hrisos  S, Clarke  MP, Wright  CM.  The emotional impact of amblyopia treatment in preschool children: randomized controlled trial.  Ophthalmology. 2004;111(8):1550-1556. doi:10.1016/j.ophtha.2003.12.059PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
Drews-Botsch  CD, Hartmann  EE, Celano  M; Infant Aphakia Treatment Study Group.  Predictors of adherence to occlusion therapy 3 months after cataract extraction in the Infant Aphakia Treatment Study.  J AAPOS. 2012;16(2):150-155. doi:10.1016/j.jaapos.2011.12.149PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
Wilson  GA, Welch  D.  Does amblyopia have a functional impact? findings from the Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development Study.  Clin Exp Ophthalmol. 2013;41(2):127-134. doi:10.1111/j.1442-9071.2012.02842.xPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
27.
Webber  AL, Wood  JM, Gole  GA, Brown  B.  Effect of amblyopia on self-esteem in children.  Optom Vis Sci. 2008;85(11):1074-1081. doi:10.1097/OPX.0b013e31818b9911PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
28.
Pediatric Eye Disease Investigator Group. A randomized trial of atropine vs. patching for treatment of moderate amblyopia in children.  Arch Ophthalmol. 2002;120(3):268-278. doi:10.1001/archopht.120.3.268PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
29.
Moke  PS, Turpin  AH, Beck  RW,  et al.  Computerized method of visual acuity testing: adaptation of the amblyopia treatment study visual acuity testing protocol.  Am J Ophthalmol. 2001;132(6):903-909. doi:10.1016/S0002-9394(01)01256-9PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
30.
Beck  RW, Moke  PS, Turpin  AH,  et al.  A computerized method of visual acuity testing: adaptation of the early treatment of diabetic retinopathy study testing protocol.  Am J Ophthalmol. 2003;135(2):194-205. doi:10.1016/S0002-9394(02)01825-1PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
31.
Harter  S.  The Construction of the Self: Developmental and Socio-Cultural Foundations. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 2012.
32.
Harter  S.  The Construction of the Self. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 1999.
33.
Harter  S.  Self-Perception Profile for Children: Manual and Questionnaires. Denver, CO: University of Denver; 2012.
34.
Niechwiej-Szwedo  E, Chandrakumar  M, Goltz  HC, Wong  AM.  Effects of strabismic amblyopia and strabismus without amblyopia on visuomotor behavior, I: saccadic eye movements.  Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2012;53(12):7458-7468. doi:10.1167/iovs.12-10550PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
35.
Niechwiej-Szwedo  E, Goltz  HC, Chandrakumar  M, Hirji  ZA, Wong  AM.  Effects of anisometropic amblyopia on visuomotor behavior, I: saccadic eye movements.  Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2010;51(12):6348-6354. doi:10.1167/iovs.10-5882PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
36.
McConkie  GW, Kerr  PW, Reddix  MD, Zola  D.  Eye movement control during reading, I: the location of initial eye fixations on words.  Vision Res. 1988;28(10):1107-1118. doi:10.1016/0042-6989(88)90137-XPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
×