Importance
Determination of the mechanisms by which visual loss increases mortality risk is important for developing interventional strategies.
Objective
To evaluate the direct and indirect effects of loss of visual acuity (VA) on mortality risk through functional status changes among aging adults.
Design, Setting, and Participants
Prospective longitudinal study of a population-based sample of 2520 noninstitutionalized adults aged 65 to 84 years from September 16, 1993, through July 26, 2003, in the greater Salisbury area of Maryland. Participants underwent reassessment 2, 6, and 8 years after baseline. Mortality status was ascertained from linkage with the National Death Index through 2009.
Exposures
Results of VA testing and self-reported functional status based on activities of daily living (ADL) and instrumental ADL (IADL).
Main Outcomes and Measure
Mortality.
Results
Worse VA levels at baseline were associated with an increased the risk for mortality (hazard ratio [HR], 1.16 [95% CI, 1.04-1.28]; P < .01) through their effect on lower IADL levels at baseline. Declines in VA over time were associated with increased mortality risk (HR, 1.78 [95% CI, 1.27-2.51]; P < .001) by way of decreasing IADL levels over time. Participants experiencing the mean linear decline in VA of 1 letter on the Early Treatment Diabetic Retinopathy Study acuity chart per year are expected to have a 16% increase in mortality risk during the 8-year study exclusively through associated declines in IADL levels.
Conclusions and Relevance
In this longitudinal study of older adults, VA loss adversely affected IADL levels, which subsequently increased the risk for mortality. Prevention of disabling ocular conditions, treatment of correctable visual impairment, and interventions designed to prevent the effect of visual impairment on IADL declines may all reduce mortality risk in aging adults.
Improving the nation’s access to ocular health care is important, given that visual impairment (VI) is associated with a variety of functional and health outcomes, including increased risk for mortality.1-14 Visual impairment has significant negative effects on physical and psychosocial health,13,15-40 but the specific mechanisms and pathways by which VI increases overall mortality remain unclear.
Several investigators have proposed mechanisms by which VI may increase overall mortality, including increased physical disability, reduced mental well-being, and severe depressive symptoms.13,14,40-42 These analyses, however, are limited by a lack of information on changes in visual acuity (VA) and changes in functioning as people age.
Using longitudinal population-based data collected during 4 assessment periods and a total of 8 years, a previous study43 found that increases in VA loss over time are related to increased difficulties in instrumental activities of daily living (IADL) in men and women and increased difficulties in activities of daily living (ADL) for men only. Activities of daily living are necessary in fundamental daily function (eg, bathing, dressing, eating), whereas IADL are measures of the degree to which an individual lives independently in the community (eg, telephone use, shopping, housework).44,45 To elucidate the relationship between VA loss and increased mortality, the present study used data from the Salisbury Eye Evaluation study to examine whether and to what extent loss of VA increases overall mortality through its effects on changes in ADL and IADL over time.
Study Population and Design
The Salisbury Eye Evaluation is a population-based study of age-related eye diseases, VI, and functional status of noninstitutionalized residents aged 65 to 84 years.46 This project was approved by the University of Miami and the Johns Hopkins School of Medicine institutional review boards. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants. A detailed description of the sampling procedure is given in the eMethods in the Supplement.46,47 Eligible participants had to be able to travel to the clinic for vision tests and to score more than 17 on the Mini-Mental State Examination.48 Eligible participants underwent a 2-hour in-home interview followed by a 4- to 5-hour examination in a clinic. Of those who were eligible, 64.51% participated. The initial cohort included 2520 participants. Reassessments took place 2, 6, and 8 years later. In total, 2240 persons participated in the second round (1995-1997), 1504 in the third round (1999- 2001), and 1250 in the fourth round (2001- 2003), with more than half the loss between rounds due to death. A linkage with National Death Index (NDI) was performed for the entire study population in 2011, with mortality follow-up through December 31, 2009. The mortality follow-up identified 1622 all-cause deaths (64.4%).
Of the 2520 initial study participants in the first round, 42.1% were men, 73.6% were white, and 26.4% were black. The highest educational level attained was less than high school for 51.5% and more than high school for 28.1%. Detailed information on sociodemographic and health-associated characteristics of the study population can be found in eTable 1 in the Supplement.
Best-corrected VA was assessed using the Early Treatment Diabetic Retinopathy Study (ETDRS) chart, and ETDRS refraction was performed on participants with VA of worse than 20/30.49 Best-corrected binocular distance VA was converted to logMAR units. Functional status of ADL and IADL were measured using standardized validated questionnaires.44,45 Assessments of ADL included difficulties with the following items: (1) getting out of bed or a chair; (2) dressing oneself; (3) bathing or showering; (4) using the toilet; and (5) feeding oneself. Assessments of IADL included difficulty with the following items: (1) using the telephone; (2) doing light housework or light yard work; (3) doing heavy housework or heavy yard work; (4) preparing meals; (5) managing money; and (6) shopping for personal items such as medicines. Each question started with the following: “By yourself, that is, without help of another person or special equipment, do you have any difficulty…?” Each question had response options of no difficulty, a little difficulty, some difficulty, a lot of difficulty, and unable to do this for health or physical reasons. We used confirmatory factor analysis and associated model fit statistics to validate the items in the IADL and ADL scales.43 Functional status scores of ADL and IADL were constructed by summing the 5 and 6 items, respectively, at each assessment and then dividing by a factor of 10.
Control variables included demographics, health behavior variables, physical health conditions, and severe depression. A standardized form was used to query all participants about demographics (eg, age, sex, race, formal educational level) and medical history of physical health conditions. All control variables used in the models were measured at baseline assessment. Educational level was measured as the highest grade completed and ranged from 0 to 17 years. The age and educational variables were centralized and rescaled by a factor of 10. Medical history included 15 medical conditions that were self-reported responses to the question, “Has a doctor ever told you that you have…?” The 15 conditions included diabetes mellitus, stroke, heart disease, hypertension, cancer, asthma, arthritis, angina, back problems, a broken hip, congestive heart failure, claudication, emphysema, Meniere disease, and Parkinson disease. Severe depression was assessed using the Severe Depression subscale of the 28-item General Health Questionnaire.49 Health behavior–related variables included questions on smoking and alcohol use (current, past, or never). Height and weight were measured and categorized as normal (reference body mass index [BMI; calculated as weight in kilograms divided by height in meters squared], 18.5 to <25.0), underweight (BMI, <18.5), overweight (BMI, 25.0 to <30.0), obese (BMI, 30.0-35.0), or very obese (BMI, >35.0).
We applied a multistep, theoretically grounded modeling process in a structural equation modeling framework, which is described in greater detail in the eMethods in the Supplement. Variables measuring baseline levels and changes over time (ie, trajectories) in levels of VA, ADL, and IADL were obtained for each study participant from ordinary least squares regression models. Cox proportional hazard regression was used to estimate effects of the trajectory variables on mortality, and linear regression was used to estimate the effects of VA trajectory variables on ADL and IADL trajectory variable mediators.
To evaluate the effects of VA trajectories on mortality under different controls, we estimated 4 mortality models where groups of covariates were added in a hierarchical fashion. In model 1, the effects of baseline VA levels and VA changes on mortality were estimated while controlling for demographic variables such as age, sex, and race. In model 2, health behavior variables (smoking, alcohol use, and BMI) were added. In model 3, the 15 self-reported medical conditions and severe depression were added as control variables. A final structural equation model (model 4) included ADL and IADL trajectories (levels and changes) as mediators of the relationship among VA trajectories, covariates, and mortality (Figure). The VA, ADL, and IADL trajectories and the covariates were all hypothesized to affect mortality directly. In addition, VA trajectories and covariates affected mortality indirectly through ADL and IADL trajectories. Therefore, the ADL and IADL trajectories served as mediators for the relationships between the VA trajectories and mortality.
In model 4, multiple equations were estimated simultaneously using a maximum likelihood estimator with robust standard errors. Indirect (mediated) effects were calculated using a product of coefficients method by multiplying the 2 parameter estimates involved in the mediation relationship.50 For example, the effect of VA levels at baseline on IADL levels at baseline was multiplied by the effect of IADL levels at baseline on mortality. The new estimate was exponentiated to obtain the hazard ratio (HR) for the indirect effect. Hazard ratios for total effects were calculated by taking exponentiation of the summed direct and indirect coefficients. Standard errors for indirect and total effects were obtained using the delta method.51 Hazard ratios of VA trajectories from the 4 mortality models were presented side by side (eTable 2 in the Supplement); only results from model 4 are presented below. We completed descriptive and model-based analyses using commercially available statistical packages (SAS, version 9.252,53 and Mplus 7,54 respectively).
Loss of VA and difficulties with ADL and IADL all increased as this population aged (Table 1). The mean annual decline in best-corrected VA was 0.02 logMAR U, an annual loss of nearly 1 letter on the ETDRS VA chart or close to 1 line during 5 years. Difficulties with ADL increased a mean of 0.013 raw U (or 0.16 standardized U), and difficulties with IADL increased a mean of 0.027 raw U (0.27 standardized U) every year. These annual changes in ADL and IADL cumulated during the 8-year period are equivalent to increasing 1 point on each of the 5 items in the ADL scale (eg, from a little difficulty to some difficulty) and more than 2 points on each of the 6 items in the IADL scale.
In our final, comprehensive model, VA levels at baseline (HR, 0.89 [95% CI, 0.67-1.18]; P = .41) and VA change over time (HR, 1.95 [95% CI, 0.58-6.50]; P = .28) did not directly predict mortality (Table 2 and Figure). As shown in eTable 2 in the Supplement, baseline ADL levels (HR, 1.20 [95% CI, 0.86-1.66]; P = .28) and changes in ADL (HR, 1.01 [95% CI, 0.45-2.22], P = .99) also did not predict mortality. However, baseline IADL levels and changes in IADL levels were significant predictors of mortality even after controlling for all covariates and VA and ADL baseline levels and changes over time. A 1-U increase in baseline IADL score was associated with an increased risk for death (HR, 1.36 [95% CI, 1.10-1.70]; P < .01). Moreover, for a mean increase of 1 U in the annual rate of IADL score, the hazard of death was nearly 3.5 times that of individuals with stable IADL levels over time (HR, 3.49 [95% CI, 1.89-6.47]; P < .001). In other words, individuals who experienced increasing difficulty with IADL by the mean amount (0.027 per year) had an increase in mortality hazard that was 3% greater annually and 31% greater during the 8-year study period than individuals with a stable IADL difficulty level.
Although VA changes did not affect mortality directly in the final model, they affected mortality indirectly through increases in IADL difficulties. The indirect effect of lower VA levels at baseline on mortality through its effect on IADL levels at baseline was an HR of 1.16 (95% CI, 1.04-1.28; P < .01). The indirect effect of decreasing VA over time on mortality through its effect on decreasing IADL over time was an HR of 1.78 (95% CI, 1.27- 2.51; P < .001). Participants experiencing the mean linear decline in VA of 1 letter on the ETDRS acuity chart per year are expected to have a 16% increase in mortality risk during the 8-year study exclusively through associated declines in IADL levels. The total (direct plus indirect through increases in IADL difficulties) effect of VA declines on mortality risk was substantial (HR, 3.47 [95% CI, 1.07-11.31]; P < .05) (Table 2).
We found evidence of nonlinear relationships between VA and ADL, IADL, and mortality. Nonlinear indirect effects of VA through IADL levels were significant at all levels of VI at baseline and all degrees of decline in VA over time (Table 3). For example, participants with baseline VA levels of 20/80 had an 8% increased risk for mortality (HR, 1.08 [95% CI, 1.02-1.15]; P = .005). We found some evidence of a direct nonlinear relationship between VA and mortality, but only at extreme levels. For example, persons who lost a mean of 7 letters of VA per year directly increased their mortality hazard by 18% (HR, 1.18 [95% CI, 1.01-1.39]; P = .04).
Many of the aforementioned studies on VA and mortality include an assessment of VA levels at only 1 time. Consistent with these studies,1-14 we found evidence that baseline levels of VI are associated with an increased risk for mortality after adjustment for potential confounders (models 1 and 2 [eTable 2 in the Supplement). However, the Salisbury Eye Evaluation study was designed to monitor change in functional limitations carefully during 4 periods in addition to monitoring change in VA during the same time period. Therefore, we were able to evaluate the relationships between the dynamic aging processes, including changes in VA, and daily functioning. In addition, we evaluated the mechanisms by which changes in VA affect mortality. We found that declines in VA adversely affect changes in IADL that, in turn, predict mortality. After including IADL as a mediator, baseline VA levels and VA changes did not affect mortality risk directly. Therefore, VA processes affect mortality almost entirely through the effect on IADL processes.
The present findings confirm that declining IADL levels are a potent predictor of mortality and that the deleterious effects of declining VA on mortality appear to operate, in a large way, through these reductions in IADL levels. This finding suggests that the adverse effects of declining VA on health are somewhat insidious in nature because researchers have been unable to study the complex interplay between changes in VA and IADL and the aging process.
The mechanisms by which declining IADL levels increase mortality are likely to be multifactorial. Declining IADL levels are associated with an increased risk for cognitive decline and dementia55,56 and declines in motor performance.57 Declining IADL levels also have profound psychosocial effects, including loneliness, depression, and social isolation, which have all been implicated in excess mortality.58-62 Furthermore, these outcomes may lead to a cascade of behavioral effects that accelerate risk for decline and death. For example, lonely older adults are less likely to engage in physical activity,63 which is negatively related to mortality.64
In the present study, the link from ADL to mortality is not detected in our models that also include IADL levels. Stronger associations between IADL and mortality relative to ADL and mortality have been reported in community-based studies.65,66
In our study, the direct pathway between ADL and mortality is significant when IADL levels are not included in the model (eMethods and eFigure in the Supplement). One possible reason for this finding is that less variability in ADL than in IADL was found in the Salisbury Eye Evaluation population. Also, some participants were ineligible for follow-up because they had moved into institutional settings such as nursing homes. Variability in our ADL measure may have been greater had we been able to assess functional status in this subset of participants, given that difficulties with ADL are an important predictor of institutionalization.67
Our findings have multiple implications. First, these findings reinforce the need for the primary prevention of VI. Primary prevention can be accomplished by addressing the risk factors that led to disabling ocular conditions and through preventive ocular care. For example, the obesity epidemic in the United States has led to an increase in the prevalence of diabetic retinopathy.68 More effective strategies targeting obesity have clear implications for ocular health. Moreover, the early detection of disabling eye diseases is suboptimal in the US health care system, leading to otherwise preventable VI.69 Finally, many Americans live with VI that is correctable through the proper fitting of glasses or contact lenses.70,71
A second implication of our findings suggests that when uncorrectable VI is present, helping affected individuals maintain robust IADL is important. For example, persuading older adults with VI to be more physically active can help to preserve functional status. Lee et al72 found that targeted interventions such as physical activity may improve functioning over time; this finding may be particularly true for those living with VI, but clinical trials supporting this hypothesis have not been published.
More than 20 years ago, Fried and Bush73 proposed that 1 major preventive health care objective in the elderly is the prevention of disability and premature mortality by postponing functional declines such as those reflected in the assessment of IADL. Our findings support their conceptual model that impairment leads to disability (eg, declining IADL), followed eventually by handicap (eg, declining ADL) and premature mortality. To interrupt this downward health spiral, Fried and Bush73 proposed that the critical point for effective intervention is when impairment is first diagnosed. Using data from the National Long-term Care Surveys linked with Medicare claims data, Sloan and colleagues74 reported that the probability of individual IADL declines in grocery shopping, meal preparation, doing laundry, and doing light housework was reduced 5% to 10% per each additional annual eye examination visit during a 5-year period.
Results of the present study therefore reinforce the important gatekeeper role that eye care providers can serve by identifying and referring patients with VI to relevant support services.75 Available resources for maintenance and improvement of ADL and IADL for individuals with VI include centers that provide independent living skills and support for low VA (eg, Lighthouse for the Blind) and specialized fitness and life-enhancing organizations (eg, Beyond Blindness Institute).
Finally, the Affordable Care Act76 will result in an estimated 30 to 33 million newly insured adults by 2016.77 Although planned state-specific essential benefits packages will not mandate comprehensive vision benefits for adults,78 this increase in the number of insured will nevertheless lead to increased detection and treatment of vision-associated ocular conditions such as diabetic retinopathy, macular degeneration, and cataract.79 The present findings also suggest that policymakers may wish to undertake new cost-benefit analyses to consider adding comprehensive vision benefits for adults in future benefit packages.80 These analyses should take into consideration the effect that regular eye care examinations may have on delaying the onset of IADL impairment,74 its potential to reduce, delay, or prevent the transition into expensive institutional care settings,31,81 and its associated effects on increased survival.
Although our models provide stronger tests of association by controlling for all static, within-person characteristics, they do not control for unobserved covariates that may change over time.82,83 For example, we are unable to estimate the model using time-varying assessments of health conditions and therefore we controlled for baseline levels only. Mortality linkage with the National Death Index is probabilistic in nature, and therefore some misclassification of mortality status might result.84 However, such misclassification is likely minimized by the careful collection of name, Social Security number, date of birth, and other identifiers used in the linkage. Although driving ability has been shown to be correlated with VA-related functioning, driving ability was not included in the IADL scale. Finally, this study was based on data from 1993 through 2003, which may not reflect the current medical conditions and care patterns of US adults aged 60 to 80 years.
This longitudinal study of community-residing older adults documented the increased risk for mortality associated with lower levels and declines in best-corrected VA through its adverse effect on IADL. Additional research is needed to confirm this pathway and to understand better how increasing VI leads to these reductions. Interventions designed to reduce the functional burden associated with declining vision are also needed.
Submitted for Publication: November 18, 2013; final revision received May 21, 2014; accepted May 27, 2014.
Corresponding Author: Sharon L. Christ, PhD, Department of Human Development and Family Studies, Purdue University, Fowler Memorial House, 1200 W State St, West Lafayette, IN 47907-2055 (slchrist@purdue.edu).
Published Online: August 21, 2014. doi:10.1001/jamaophthalmol.2014.2847.
Author Contributions: Drs Christ and Zheng had full access to all the data in the study and take responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis. Dr West and Ms Muñoz were involved in the original design and collection of Salisbury Eye Evaluation (SEE) data.
Study concept and design: Christ, West, Tannenbaum, Lee.
Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: Christ, Zheng, Swenor, Lam, Muñoz, Lee.
Drafting of the manuscript: Christ, Zheng, Swenor, Lam, Tannenbaum, Lee.
Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Christ, Lam, West, Tannenbaum, Muñoz, Lee.
Statistical analysis: Christ, Zheng, Swenor.
Obtained funding: Christ, Lam, West, Lee.
Administrative, technical, or material support: Lam, West, Muñoz.
Study supervision: West.
Conflict of Interest Disclosures: All authors have completed and submitted the ICMJE Form for Disclosure of Potential Conflicts of Interest and none were reported.
Funding/Support: This study was supported by grant EY21187 from the National Eye Institute, which provided funding for this study, including linkage of SEE participants with the National Death Index and analyses of data. The original collection of SEE data was supported by grant AG10184 from the National Institute on Aging.
Role of the Funder/Sponsor: The funding source had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.
1.Wang
JJ, Mitchell
P, Simpson
JM, Cumming
RG, Smith
W. Visual impairment, age-related cataract, and mortality.
Arch Ophthalmol. 2001;119(8):1186-1190.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 2.Thompson
JR, Gibson
JM, Jagger
C. The association between visual impairment and mortality in elderly people.
Age Ageing. 1989;18(2):83-88.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 5.Podgor
MJ, Cassel
GH, Kannel
WB. Lens changes and survival in a population-based study.
N Engl J Med. 1985;313(23):1438-1444.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 6.Hu
FB, Hankinson
SE, Stampfer
MJ,
et al. Prospective study of cataract extraction and risk of coronary heart disease in women.
Am J Epidemiol. 2001;153(9):875-881.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 7.Meddings
DR, Hertzman
C, Barer
ML,
et al. Socioeconomic status, mortality, and the development of cataract at a young age.
Soc Sci Med. 1998;46(11):1451-1457.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 9.Lee
DJ, Gómez-Marín
O, Lam
BL, Zheng
DD. Visual acuity impairment and mortality in US adults.
Arch Ophthalmol. 2002;120(11):1544-1550.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 10.Lee
DJ, Gómez-Marín
O, Lam
BL, Zheng
DD. Glaucoma and survival: the National Health Interview Survey 1986-1994.
Ophthalmology. 2003;110(8):1476-1483.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 11.Lee
DJ, Gómez-Marín
O, Lam
BL, Zheng
DD. Visual impairment and unintentional injury mortality: the National Health Interview Survey 1986-1994.
Am J Ophthalmol. 2003;136(6):1152-1154.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 12.Knudtson
MD, Klein
BE, Klein
R. Age-related eye disease, visual impairment, and survival: the Beaver Dam Eye Study.
Arch Ophthalmol. 2006;124(2):243-249.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 13.Zheng
DD, Christ
SL, Lam
BL, Arheart
KL, Galor
A, Lee
DJ. Increased mortality risk among the visually impaired: the roles of mental well-being and preventive care practices.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2012;53(6):2685-2692.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 14.Freeman
EE, Egleston
BL, West
SK, Bandeen-Roche
K, Rubin
G. Visual acuity change and mortality in older adults.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2005;46(11):4040-4045.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 15.Wallhagen
MI, Strawbridge
WJ, Shema
SJ, Kurata
J, Kaplan
GA. Comparative impact of hearing and vision impairment on subsequent functioning.
J Am Geriatr Soc. 2001;49(8):1086-1092.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 16.Wang
JJ, Mitchell
P, Smith
W. Vision and low self-rated health: the Blue Mountains Eye Study.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2000;41(1):49-54.
PubMedGoogle Scholar 17.Ivers
RQ, Mitchell
P, Cumming
RG. Sensory impairment and driving: the Blue Mountains Eye Study.
Am J Public Health. 1999;89(1):85-87.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 18.Owsley
C, Ball
K, McGwin
G
Jr,
et al. Visual processing impairment and risk of motor vehicle crash among older adults.
JAMA. 1998;279(14):1083-1088.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 19.Carabellese
C, Appollonio
I, Rozzini
R,
et al. Sensory impairment and quality of life in a community elderly population.
J Am Geriatr Soc. 1993;41(4):401-407.
PubMedGoogle Scholar 20.West
SK, Muñoz
B, Rubin
GS,
et al. Function and visual impairment in a population-based study of older adults: the SEE project: Salisbury Eye Evaluation.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1997;38(1):72-82.
PubMedGoogle Scholar 21.Bookwala
J, Lawson
B. Poor vision, functioning, and depressive symptoms: a test of the activity restriction model.
Gerontologist. 2011;51(6):798-808.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 22.Wahl
HW, Tesch-Romer
H, Rott
C. Vision and cognitive functioning in old age. In: Silverstone
B, Lang
MA, Rosenthal
MA, Faye
EE, eds. The Lighthouse Handbook on Vision Impairment and Vision Rehabilitation. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2000:431-439.
23.Horowitz
A, Reinhardt
JP. Mental health issues in depression: research in depression, disability, and rehabilitation. In: Silverstone
B, Lang
MA, Rosenthal
MA, Faye
EE, eds. The Lighthouse Handbook on Vision Impairment and Vision Rehabilitation. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2000:1089-1109.
24.Lee
DJ, Gomez-Marin
O, Lam
BL. Current depression, life history of depression, and visual acuity in Hispanic adults.
J Vis Impair Blind. 2000;94:86-96.
Google Scholar 25.Chia
EM, Wang
JJ, Rochtchina
E, Smith
W, Cumming
RR, Mitchell
P. Impact of bilateral visual impairment on health-related quality of life: the Blue Mountains Eye Study.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2004;45(1):71-76.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 26.Varma
R, Wu
J, Chong
K, Azen
SP, Hays
RD; Los Angeles Latino Eye Study Group. Impact of severity and bilaterality of visual impairment on health-related quality of life.
Ophthalmology. 2006;113(10):1846-1853.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 27.Salive
ME, Guralnik
J, Glynn
RJ, Christen
W, Wallace
RB, Ostfeld
AM. Association of visual impairment with mobility and physical function.
J Am Geriatr Soc. 1994;42(3):287-292.
PubMedGoogle Scholar 29.West
SK, Rubin
GS, Broman
AT, Muñoz
B, Bandeen-Roche
K, Turano
K. How does visual impairment affect performance on tasks of everyday life? the SEE Project: Salisbury Eye Evaluation.
Arch Ophthalmol. 2002;120(6):774-780.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 30.Rubin
GS, Bandeen-Roche
K, Huang
GH,
et al. The association of multiple visual impairments with self-reported visual disability: SEE project.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2001;42(1):64-72.
PubMedGoogle Scholar 31.Klein
BE, Moss
SE, Klein
R, Lee
KE, Cruickshanks
KJ. Associations of visual function with physical outcomes and limitations 5 years later in an older population: the Beaver Dam Eye Study.
Ophthalmology. 2003;110(4):644-650.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 32.Rubin
GS, Roche
KB, Prasada-Rao
P, Fried
LP. Visual impairment and disability in older adults.
Optom Vis Sci.1994;71(12):750-760.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 33.Tournier
M, Moride
Y, Ducruet
T, Moshyk
A, Rochon
S. Depression and mortality in the visually-impaired, community-dwelling, elderly population of Quebec.
Acta Ophthalmol. 2008;86(2):196-201.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 34.Whitson
HE, Cousins
SW, Burchett
BM, Hybels
CF, Pieper
CF, Cohen
HJ. The combined effect of visual impairment and cognitive impairment on disability in older people.
J Am Geriatr Soc. 2007;55(6):885-891.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 35.Jacobs
JM, Hammerman-Rozenberg
R, Maaravi
Y, Cohen
A, Stessman
J. The impact of visual impairment on health, function and mortality.
Aging Clin Exp Res. 2005;17(4):281-286.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 36.Rudberg
MA, Furner
SE, Dunn
JE, Cassel
CK. The relationship of visual and hearing impairments to disability: an analysis using the Longitudinal Study of Aging.
J Gerontol. 1993;48(6):M261-M265.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 37.Swanson
MW, McGwin
G. Visual impairment and functional status from the 1995 National Health Interview Survey on Disability.
Ophthalmic Epidemiol. 2004;11(3):227-239.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 38.Lee
PP, Smith
JP, Kington
RS. The associations between self-rated vision and hearing and functional status in middle age.
Ophthalmology. 1999;106(2):401-405.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 39.Cummings
SR, Nevitt
MC, Browner
WS,
et al; Study of Osteoporotic Fractures Research Group. Risk factors for hip fracture in white women.
N Engl J Med. 1995;332(12):767-773.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 40.Klein
BE, Klein
R, Lee
KE, Cruickshanks
KJ. Performance-based and self-assessed measures of visual function as related to history of falls, hip fractures, and measured gait time: the Beaver Dam Eye Study.
Ophthalmology. 1998;105(1):160-164.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 41.Christ
SL, Lee
DJ, Lam
B, Zheng
D, Arheart
K. Assessment of the effect of visual impairment on mortality through multiple health pathways using structural equation modeling.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci.2008;49(8):3318-3323.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 42.Karpa
MJ, Mitchell
P, Beath
K, Rochtchina
E, Cumming
RG, Wang
JJ; Blue Mountains Eye Study. Direct and indirect effects of visual impairment on mortality risk in older persons.
Arch Ophthalmol. 2009;127(10):1347-1353.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 43.Lam
BL, Christ
SL, Zheng
DD,
et al. Longitudinal relationships among visual acuity and tasks of everyday life: the Salisbury Eye Evaluation study.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2013;54(1):193-200.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 44.Lawton
MP, Brody
EM. Assessment of older people: self-maintaining and instrumental activities of daily living.
Gerontologist. 1969;9(3):179-186.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 45.Katz
S, Ford
AB, Moskowitz
RW, Jackson
BA, Jaffe
MW. Studies of illness in the aged: the index of ADL: a standardized measure of biological and psychosocial function.
JAMA. 1963;185:914-919.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 46.Muñoz
B, West
S, Rubin
GS, Schein
OD, Fried
LP, Bandeen-Roche
K. Who participates in population based studies of visual impairment? the Salisbury Eye Evaluation project experience.
Ann Epidemiol. 1999;9(1):53-59.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 47.Rubin
GS, West
SK, Muñoz
B,
et al. A comprehensive assessment of visual impairment in a population of older Americans: the SEE study: Salisbury Eye Evaluation Project.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1997;38(3):557-568.
PubMedGoogle Scholar 48.Folstein
MF, Folstein
SE, McHugh
PR. “Mini-Mental State”: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician.
J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12(3):189-198.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 52.SAS. SAS, Version 9.2. Cary, NC: SAS Institute Inc; 2009.
53.Skinner
CJ. Domain means, regression and multivariate analysis. In: Skinner
CJ, Holt
D, Smith
TMF, eds. Analysis of Complex Surveys. New York, NY: Wiley; 1989:59-87.
54.Muthén
LK, Muthén
BO. Mplus User’s Guide. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén; 1998-2007.
55.Rajan
KB, Hebert
LE, Scherr
PA, Mendes de Leon
CF, Evans
DA. Disability in basic and instrumental activities of daily living is associated with faster rate of decline in cognitive function of older adults.
J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2013;68(5):624-630.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 56.Pérès
K, Helmer
C, Amieva
H,
et al. Natural history of decline in instrumental activities of daily living performance over the 10 years preceding the clinical diagnosis of dementia: a prospective population-based study.
J Am Geriatr Soc. 2008;56(1):37-44.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 57.Inzitari
M, Carlo
A, Baldereschi
M,
et al; ILSA Working Group. Risk and predictors of motor-performance decline in a normally functioning population-based sample of elderly subjects: the Italian Longitudinal Study on Aging.
J Am Geriatr Soc. 2006;54(2):318-324.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 58.House
JS. Understanding social factors and inequalities in health: 20th century progress and 21st century prospects.
J Health Soc Behav. 2002;43(2):125-142.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 59.Tilvis
RS, Laitala
V, Routasalo
PE, Pitkala
KH. Suffering from loneliness indicates significant mortality risk of older people.
J Aging Res.2011;2011:534781. doi:10.4061/2011/534781.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 60.Perissinotto
CM, Stijacic Cenzer
I, Covinsky
KE. Loneliness in older persons: a predictor of functional decline and death.
Arch Intern Med. 2012;172(14):1078-1083.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 61.Luo
Y, Hawkley
LC, Waite
LJ, Cacioppo
JT. Loneliness, health, and mortality in old age: a national longitudinal study.
Soc Sci Med. 2012;74(6):907-914.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 62.Bowling
AP, Edelmann
RJ, Leaver
J, Hoekel
T. Loneliness, mobility, well-being and social support in a sample of over 85-year-olds.
Pers Individ Dif. 1989;10(11):1189-1192.
Google ScholarCrossref 63.Hawkley
LC, Thisted
RA, Cacioppo
JT. Loneliness predicts reduced physical activity: cross-sectional & longitudinal analyses.
Health Psychol.2009;28(3):354-363.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 64.Kokkinos
P, Myers
J, Faselis
C,
et al. Exercise capacity and mortality in older men: a 20-year follow-up study.
Circulation. 2010;122(8):790-797.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 65.Noale
M, Maggi
S, Minicuci
N,
et al; ILSA Working Group. Dementia and disability: impact on mortality: the Italian Longitudinal Study on Aging.
Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2003;16(1):7-14.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 66.Keller
BK, Morton
JL, Thomas
VS, Potter
JF. The effect of visual and hearing impairments on functional status.
J Am Geriatr Soc. 1999;47(11):1319-1325.
PubMedGoogle Scholar 67.Miller
EA, Weissert
WG. Predicting elderly people’s risk for nursing home placement, hospitalization, functional impairment, and mortality: a synthesis.
Med Care Res Rev. 2000;57(3):259-297.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 68.Ko
F, Vitale
S, Chou
CF, Cotch
MF, Saaddine
J, Friedman
DS. Prevalence of nonrefractive visual impairment in US adults and associated risk factors, 1999-2002 and 2005-2008.
JAMA. 2012;308(22):2361-2368.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 70.Lee
DJ, Gomez-Marin
O, Lam
BL. Prevalence of uncorrected binocular distance visual acuity in Hispanic and non-Hispanic adults: results from the HHANES and the NHANES I.
Ophthalmology. 1998;105(3):552-560.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 71.Klein
R, Lee
KE, Gangnon
RE, Klein
BE. Incidence of visual impairment over a 20-year period: the Beaver Dam Eye Study.
Ophthalmology. 2013;120(6):1210-1219.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 72.Lee
HB, Kasper
JD, Shore
AD, Yokley
JL, Black
BS, Rabins
PV. Level of cognitive impairment predicts mortality in high-risk community samples: the Memory and Medical Care Study.
J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2006;18(4):543-546.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 73.Fried
LP, Bush
TL. Morbidity as a focus of preventive health care in the elderly.
Epidemiol Rev. 1988;10:48-64.
PubMedGoogle Scholar 74.Sloan
FA, Picone
G, Brown
DS, Lee
PP. Longitudinal analysis of the relationship between regular eye examinations and changes in visual and functional status.
J Am Geriatr Soc. 2005;53(11):1867-1874.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 75.Wang
JJ, Mitchell
P, Smith
W, Cumming
RG, Attebo
K. Impact of visual impairment on use of community support services by elderly persons: the Blue Mountains Eye Study.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1999;40(1):12-19.
PubMedGoogle Scholar 77.Armstrong
B. A simple estimator of minimum detectable relative risk, sample size, or power in cohort studies.
Am J Epidemiol. 1987;126(2):356-358.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 78.Barbeau
EM, Krieger
N, Soobader
MJ. Working class matters: socioeconomic disadvantage, race/ethnicity, gender, and smoking in NHIS 2000.
Am J Public Health. 2004;94(2):269-278.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 79.Lee
DJ, Lam
BL, Arora
S,
et al. Reported eye care utilization and health insurance status among US adults.
Arch Ophthalmol.2009;127(3):303-310.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 80.Chou
R, Dana
T, Bougatsos
C. Screening older adults for impaired visual acuity: a review of the evidence for the US Preventive Services Task Force.
Ann Intern Med.2009;151(1):44-58, W11-20.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 81.Wysong
A, Lee
PP, Sloan
FA. Longitudinal incidence of adverse outcomes of age-related macular degeneration.
Arch Ophthalmol. 2009;127(3):320-327.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 83.Pearl
J. The causal foundations of structural equation modeling. In: Hoyle
RH, ed. Handbook of Structural Equation Modeling. New York. NY: Guilford Press; 2012:68-91.
84.Breslow
NE, Day
NE. Statistical methods in cancer research, volume II: the design and analysis of cohort studies.
IARC Sci Publ. 1987;(82):1-406.
PubMedGoogle Scholar