During the past 20 years, basic science has shown that there are different critical periods for different visual functions during the development of the visual system. Visual functions processed at higher anatomical levels within the system have a later critical period than functions processed at lower levels. This general principle suggests that treatments for amblyopia should be followed in a logical sequence, with treatment for each visual function to be started before its critical period is over. However, critical periods for some visual functions, such as stereopsis, are not yet fully determined, and the optimal treatment is, therefore, unknown. This article summarizes the current extent of our knowledge and points to the gaps that need to be filled.
Amblyopia comes from the Greek word meaning dull sight or blunt sight. It results from a variety of sensory and motor abnormalities, which have multiple causes and multiple effects. The conditions leading to functional amblyopia are well known1 and include strabismus, anisometropia, astigmatism, myopia, cataract, and other forms of stimulus deprivation. In some of these conditions, amblyopia is not the only result. There may also be changes in binocular summation and suppression, some loss of binocular function, and loss of stereopsis. Amblyopia is not a simple phenomenon.2 As well as loss of Snellen and grating acuity, there may be loss of vernier acuity,3 loss of sensitivity to the contrast in a stimulus,4 distortions in the shape of a stimulus,5 some uncertainty about the position of a stimulus,6 motion deficits,7 and an increase in the magnitude of the crowding effect or separation difficulty.8 This review considers some generalizations about the development of these various phenomena, leading to some conclusions and also some unknowns about related treatments.
There are 3 periods in the development of visual acuity and in the development of ocular dominance. Evidence about acuity comes primarily from studies with humans. During the first 3 to 5 years of life, acuity develops from less than 20/200 to near 20/20, as measured by tests that exclude any crowding effects.9 During these years, acuity can be reduced by the various forms of deprivation listed previously, leading to amblyopia. However, amblyopia is not confined to the first 3 to 5 years of life, but can result from strabismus or anisometropia at any age, from several months to 7 or 8 years of age.1 Recovery of acuity lost to amblyopia can occur in even older individuals. Eye care professionals have obtained positive results after sustained treatment of teenagers, and in a few cases adults who are affected by amblyopia.10 Thus, one can talk about 3 periods for acuity: the period of development of visual acuity (birth to 3-5 years of age), the period during which deprivation is effective in causing amblyopia (a few months to 7 or 8 years of age), and the period during which recovery from amblyopia can be obtained (time of deprivation to the teenaged years or even into the adult years). A similar distinction of periods is found in the study of ocular dominance in animals. The nerve fibers from the lateral geniculate, that terminate in layer IV of the visual cortex, segregate into left and right eye stripes between birth and 6 weeks of age in the macaque monkey,11 but this pattern of stripes can be altered by monocular deprivation until 10 weeks of age.11
The term critical period became widely used in all visual system literature after Wiesel and Hubel12 presented their experiments on monocular deprivation. They talked about the critical period for changes in the ocular dominance of cells in the primary visual cortex of the cat, which occurs between eye opening and several months of age as a result of monocular deprivation. Thus, the critical period corresponds to the period during which deprivation is effective, rather than the initial period of development or the period during which recovery can be obtained. Generally speaking, the critical period starts some time after the onset of development of a visual property. For example, anisometropia does not have much affect on acuity unless it is persistent for 3 years; ie, a short period of anisometropia after birth does not lead to acuity deficits.13
The critical period depends on the anatomical level of the system being studied. The retina projects to the lateral geniculate nucleus, which projects on to the primary visual cortex, then other areas within the occipital cortex, and finally to the visual areas within the parietal and temporal cortex (Figure 1). Cells at higher levels of the system have a critical period that lasts for a longer time. For example, the ocular dominance of cells in layers II, III, V, and VI of the primary visual cortex can be changed by monocular deprivation after the ocular dominance of cells in layer IV has been established.11,14
The critical period also depends on the visual function being studied. Many cells in the visual cortex respond to movement in a particular direction–a property known as direction selectivity. The direction selectivity of cells in the visual cortex can be changed by rearing an animal in an environment continually moving in 1 direction. However, the critical period for changes in direction selectivity ends earlier than the critical period for ocular dominance changes, according to studies of both properties in primary visual cortex.15 Many cells also respond to the orientation of a bar or edge of light, and it seems likely that the critical period for changes in orientation selectivity also ends earlier than the critical period for ocular dominance changes.16,17
Furthermore, the critical period depends on the previous visual history of the animal. For example, rearing an animal in the dark postpones both the start and the end of the critical period for ocular dominance changes.18,19 Thus, one cannot talk simply about the critical period, rather, one has to talk about the critical period for a particular visual property in a particular part of the visual system in a particular species, after a specified form of visual deprivation in an animal with a specified visual history.
Critical periods in the visual system fall between eye opening and puberty.2 The organization of some parts of the visual system can be affected by changes in the electrical activity reaching it before the eyes open,20 but this is not of much practical concern to an ophthalmologist. The critical period also depends on the severity of the deprivation. For example, 10 days of monocular deprivation in the cat produces a critical period that lasts until 3 months of age,21 while 3 months of monocular deprivation produces a critical period that lasts until 9 months of age.14 Some forms of deprivation can even have an effect on ocular dominance in the adult cat.22,23 In the human, there are cases in which enucleation of the nonamblyopic eye in an adult leads to an improvement of acuity in the amblyopic eye.24,25 This does not quite correspond to the experiments in adult cats, because the human result concerned recovery from a deficit, while the cat results concerned creation of a deficit. As mentioned earlier, the period for the former is known to last longer than the period for the latter. Nevertheless, the human observation emphasizes that a severe change (enucleation) may have an effect over a longer period than a milder change (amblyopia therapy).
Another general principle is that a property processed by higher levels of the visual system has a critical period that lasts longer than a property processed at a lower level. This point was inherent in the observation that layer IV of the primary visual cortex has a critical period for ocular dominance ending earlier than the critical period for layers II, III, V, and VI, since layer IV is the input layer that projects to the other layers. This point also explains why the critical period for direction selectivity ends earlier than the critical period for ocular dominance, since direction selectivity is determined largely in the input layer of primary visual cortex in the cat, while binocularity is more developed in the output layers.26 There are not many studies of critical periods for areas higher than primary visual cortex, but it is known that visual areas of temporal cortex in the macaque monkey have a critical period lasting longer than that in the primary visual cortex.27 Monocular deprivation affects a variety of visual functions with critical periods that end earlier for functions dealt with at lower levels of the system. As an example, monocular deprivation in the macaque monkey affects absolute sensitivity to light before 3 months of age, sensitivity to wavelength and increments of light before 6 months of age, sensitivity to contrast at high frequencies before 18 months of age, and binocular summation before 24 months of age.28
How does all this information affect clinical practice? It is easy to state the general principles, but not always easy to apply them, partly because of overriding clinical considerations, and partly because some of the basic science experiments required have not been performed. One general principle is that, if possible, visual functions with an early critical period should be treated before visual functions with a later critical period. As a practical consideration, many strabismologists believe that acuity in the amblyopic eye needs to be improved by patching or other means before the eyes are aligned, otherwise, the patient will not maintain straight eyes after they are aligned. After this treatment there are many other visual functions that can be treated by a variety of orthoptic techniques. These functions include single-letter acuity, contrast sensitivity, binocular function, depth perception, stereopsis, perception of location, vernier acuity, the crowding phenomenon (acuity for letters close to each other in a line), and so on. Information on how the critical periods for these various visual functions differ from each other is limited. There is some evidence that vernier acuity continues to develop after grating acuity reaches adult levels,29 and vernier acuity can be improved by the treatment of adults who are affected by amblyopia.30 There is also evidence that crowding effects continue to decrease after basic acuity has matured. In other words, Snellen acuity continues to improve after grating and single-letter acuity have matured.31 Thus, deficits in the crowding phenomenon, which affect reading, may remain after single-letter acuity has been corrected and the critical period for this is over. In any case, a good rehabilitative program should treat all of these visual properties in a logical sequence.
In the management of amblyopia, clinical intervention should take place as soon after the deficit that all visual functions that the physician and the patient want to treat can be treated. This point arises most clearly in relation to stereopsis. Stereopsis will never be obtained unless amblyopia is treated, the eyes are aligned, and binocular fusion and function are achieved before the critical period for stereopsis ends. Clinical data suggest that this occurs before 24 months of age,1,32,33 but we do not know exactly when it occurs, because crucial pieces of basic science information are missing.
We do know when stereopsis develops. Stereoscopic acuity reaches 60 minutes of arc between 2 and 5 months of age, then increases very rapidly over the next month.34 There is a slower increase between 6 months and 3 years of age.35,36 Going back to general principles, one would expect to be able to destroy stereopsis during a period lasting longer than the first 3 years of life, and to recover it during an even longer period. But, does the general principle arising out of data on acuity and ocular dominance also apply to stereopsis? Is stereopsis, because of its dependence on acuity and binocularity, different from these other functions? Hopefully, basic science experiments performed during the next few years will tell us the answers to these questions. Only then will the clinician have a better idea as to what degree stereopsis can be developed after surgery at various times between 4 and 24 months of age.
In the absence of definite information, the optimum forms of treatment have yet to be elucidated. Experience suggests that perfect stereopsis has never been achieved in cases of congenital or infantile esotropia.37 However, more aggressive practitioners claim that early intervention can produce improved results. In an extreme case of unilateral cataract, intervention on the day after birth, a thoughtfully designed long-term program of patching, produced in the patient at 8 years of age a visual acuity of 20/25 in the treated eye, and stereoacuity of 50 seconds of arc.38 The philosophy that one should intervene around 4 months of age in congenital esotropia may have a good rationale,39 since this is the age at which stereopsis is developing rapidly. Two of 7 patients treated at this age had stereoacuity better than 60 seconds of arc at 5 to 8 years of age.40 Maybe one could leave surgery until later than this, if the period of recovery from stereoscopic deficits was known to last longer than the period for the development of stereopsis.
Of all the evidence that has been produced during the last 40 years, there are still some important missing pieces of information. What are the critical periods for higher visual functions, and what is the critical period for stereopsis? More specifically, when the crowding phenomenon, a higher visual property, is degraded more than acuity, does this justify a different course of therapy?41 Does the critical period for stereopsis end concurrently with the period of development of stereopsis, or does it extend beyond? Perhaps scientists will produce evidence to suggest new sequences of therapy after surgery has been performed. Perhaps the results of scientists' studies will also support the value of early and aggressive treatment of strabismus, just as scientific evidence now supports the early treatment of unilateral cataract,38,42 in cases that would have been regarded as intractable 20 or 30 years ago.
Accepted for publication December 30, 1997.
The study was supported by grants EY 00053 and EY 11353 from the National Eye Institute, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, Md.
I thank Ray Gariano, MD, PhD, Caleb Gonzalez, MD, Bruce Shields, MD, and 2 anonymous reviewers for helpful comments on the manuscript, and Robert Hess, MD, Dennis Levi, PhD, OD, and Judy Seaber, PhD for helpful discussions.
Reprints: Nigel W. Daw, PhD, 333 Cedar St, PO Box 208061, New Haven, CT 06520-8061 (e-mail: Daw@biomed.med.yale.edu).
1.Von Noorden
GK Binocular Vision and Ocular Motility. St Louis, Mo Mosby–Year Book Inc1990;
2.Daw
NW Visual Development. New York, NY Plenum Publishing Corp1995;
4.Bradley
AFreeman
RD Contrast sensitivity in anisometropic amblyopia.
Invest Ophthalmol. 1981;21467- 476
Google Scholar 5.Hess
RFCampbell
FWGreenhalgh
T On the nature of the neural abnormality in human amblyopia: neural aberrations and neural sensitivity loss.
Pfluger's Arch (Berlin). 1978;377201- 207
Google ScholarCrossref 6.Flom
MCBedell
HE Identifying amblyopia using associated conditions, acuity, and nonacuity features.
Am J Optom Physiol Opt. 1985;62153- 160
Google ScholarCrossref 7.Hess
RFDeManins
RBex
PJ A reduced motion after-effect in strabismic amblyopia.
Vision Res. 1997;371303- 1311
Google ScholarCrossref 8.Stuart
JABurian
HM A study of separation difficulty.
Am J Ophthalmol. 1962;53471- 477
Google Scholar 9.Dobson
V Visual acuity testing in infants: from laboratory to clinic. Simons
Ked.
Early Visual Development Normal and Abnormal. New York, NY Oxford University Press Inc1993;318- 334
Google Scholar 10.Birnbaum
MHKoslowe
KSanet
R Success in amblyopia therapy as a function of age: a literature survey.
Am J Optom Physiol Opt. 1977;54269- 275
Google ScholarCrossref 11.LeVay
SWiesel
TNHubel
DH The development of ocular dominance columns in normal and visually deprived monkeys.
J Comp Neurol. 1980;1911- 51
Google ScholarCrossref 12.Wiesel
TNHubel
DH Single cell responses in striate cortex of kittens deprived of vision in one eye.
J Neurophysiol. 1963;261003- 1017
Google Scholar 13.Abrahamsson
MAndersson
AKSjostrand
J A longitudinal study of a population based sample of astigmatic children.
Acta Ophthalmol. 1990;68428- 440
Google ScholarCrossref 14.Daw
NWFox
KSato
HCzepita
D Critical period for monocular deprivation in the cat visual cortex.
J Neurophysiol. 1992;67197- 202
Google Scholar 15.Daw
NWWyatt
HJ Kittens reared in a unidirectional environment: evidence for a critical period.
J Physiol. 1976;257155- 170
Google Scholar 16.Chapman
BStryker
MP Development of orientation selectivity in ferret visual cortex and effects of deprivation.
J Neurosci. 1993;135251- 5262
Google Scholar 17.Kim
DSBonhoeffer
T Chronic observation of the emergence of iso-orientation domains in kitten visual cortex.
Soc Neurosci Abstr. 1993;191800
Google Scholar 18.Cynader
MSMitchell
DE Prolonged sensitivity to monocular deprivation in dark-reared cats.
J Neurophysiol. 1980;431026- 1040
Google Scholar 19.Mower
GD The effect of dark rearing on the time course of the critical period in cat visual cortex.
Brain Res Dev Brain Res. 1991;58151- 158
Google ScholarCrossref 20.Shatz
CJStryker
MP Prenatal tetrodotoxin infusion blocks segregation of retinogeniculate afferents.
Science. 1988;24287- 89
Google ScholarCrossref 21.Olson
CRFreeman
RD Profile of the sensitive period for monocular deprivation in kittens.
Exp Brain Res. 1980;3917- 21
Google Scholar 22.Gilbert
CDWiesel
TN Receptive field dynamics in adult primary visual cortex.
Nature. 1992;356150- 152
Google ScholarCrossref 23.Pettet
MWGilbert
CD Dynamic changes in receptive-field size in cat primary visual cortex.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1992;898366- 8370
Google ScholarCrossref 24.Tierney
DW Vision recovery in amblyopia after contralateral subretinal hemorrhage.
J Am Optom Assoc. 1989;60281- 283
Google Scholar 25.Vereecken
EPBrabant
P Prognosis for vision in amblyopia after loss of the good eye.
Arch Ophthalmol. 1984;102220- 224
Google ScholarCrossref 26.Shatz
CJStryker
MP Ocular dominance in layer IV of the cat's visual cortex and the effects of monocular deprivation.
J Physiol. 1978;281267- 283
Google Scholar 28.Harwerth
RSSmith
ELDuncan
GCCrawford
MLJVon Noorden
GK Multiple sensitive periods in the development of the primate visual system.
Science. 1986;232235- 238
Google ScholarCrossref 29.Levi
DMCarkeet
A Amblyopia: a consequence of abnormal visual development. Simons
Ked.
Early Visual Development Normal and Abnormal. New York, NY Oxford University Press Inc1993;391- 408
Google Scholar 30.Levi
DMPolat
U Neural plasticity in adults with amblyopia.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1996;936830- 6834
Google ScholarCrossref 31.Fern
KDManny
RE Visual acuity of the preschool child: a review.
Am J Optom Physiol Opt. 1986;63319- 345
Google ScholarCrossref 33.Jampolsky
A Unequal visual inputs and strabismus management, a comparison of human and animal strabismus.
Symposium on Strabismus Trans New Orleans Acad Ophthalmol. St Louis, Mo Mosby–Year Book Inc1978;358- 492
Google Scholar 35.Birch
EEHale
LA Operant assessment of stereoacuity.
Clin Vision Sci. 1996;4295- 300
Google Scholar 36.Ciner
EBSchanel-Klitsch
EHerzberg
C Stereoacuity development: 6 months to 5 years: a new tool for testing and screening.
Optom Vis Sci. 1996;7343- 48
Google ScholarCrossref 37.Von Noorden
GK A reassessment of infantile esotropia.
Am J Ophthalmol. 1988;1051- 10
Google Scholar 38.Gregg
FMParks
MM Stereopsis after congenital monocular cataract extraction.
Am J Ophthalmol. 1992;114314- 317
Google Scholar 39.Helveston
EM The origins of congenital esotropia.
J Pediatr Ophthalmol Strabismus. 1993;30215- 232
Google Scholar 40.Wright
KWEdelman
PMMcVey
JHTerry
APLin
M High-grade stereo acuity after early surgery for congenital esotropia.
Arch Ophthalmol. 1994;112913- 919
Google ScholarCrossref 42.Maurer
DLewis
TL Visual outcomes after infantile cataract. Simons
Ked.
Early Visual Development Normal and Abnormal. New York, NY Oxford University Press Inc1993;454- 484
Google Scholar